Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T14:16:38.068Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Agent–patient word-order preference in the acquisition of Tagalog*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Norman S. Segalowitz
Affiliation:
Concordia University, Montreal
Rosita G. Galang
Affiliation:
Ateneo University-Philippine Normal College Linguistics Consortium, Manila

Abstract

This study investigated 3-, 5- and 7-year-old Tagalog-speaking children's mastery of agent-focus (active) and patient-focus (passive) sentence structures. In contrast to the usual English developmental pattern with actives and passives, the Tagalog children generally showed better mastery of patient-focus sentences than agent-focus sentences. These results were attributed to the children's strategy of interpreting the first noun of a sentence to be the agent of the action. The structure and usage of these sentence types in Tagalog do not appear to provide linguistic pressures favouring such a strategy. Thus these results are consistent with the view that such a strategy might be non-linguistic in origin.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1978

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The authors are grateful to Dr Felisa Anonuevo, Principal of the University of the Philippines Elementary School and to Mr and Mrs Alberto Racho and Mrs Paulina Santos for assistance in testing and making arrangements to test the children in this study. We would also like to thank Drs Tannis Arbuckle, Anna-Beth Doyle and Elizabeth Gatbonton and Ms Helen Gougeon for their constructive criticisms of earlier drafts of this paper. The senior author would also like to thank Dr Bonifacio Sibayan, President of the Philippine Normal College, for an invitation to teach at the Summer Institute which made it possible to conduct the present research. Address for correspondence: N. Segalowitz, Psychology Department, Concordia University, 1455 de Maisonneuve Blvd. West, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

References

REFERENCES

Anisfeld, M. & Klenbort, I. (1973). On the functions of structural paraphrase: the view from the passive voice. PsychBull 79. 117–26.Google Scholar
Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bowen, J. D. (1969). Beginning Tagalog. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. JChLang 2. 119.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. (1968). The case for case. In Bach, E. & Harms, R. (eds), Universals in linguistic theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Fraser, C. J., Bellugi, U. & Brown, R. (1963). Control of grammar in imitation, comprehension and production. JVLVB 2. 121–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaer, E. P. (1969). Children's understanding and production of sentences. JVLVB 8. 289–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman-Eisler, F. & Cohen, M. (1970). Is N, P, and PN difficulty a valid criterion of transformational operations? JVLVB 9. 161–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. H. (1963). Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In Greenberg, J. H. (ed.), Universals of language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Hayhurst, H. (1967). Some errors of young children in producing passive sentences. JVLVB 6. 634–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornby, P. (1971). Surface structure and topic-comment distinction: a developmental study. ChDev 42. 1975–88.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. (1976). Towards a universal definition of ‘subject’. In Li, C. (ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lopez, L. A. (1974). A study of the internalised Tagalog grammar rules by three-, four-, five-, six- and seven-year-old Tagalog speaking children. Unpublished M.A. Thesis, Philippine Normal College, Manila, Philippines.Google Scholar
Lovell, K. & Dixon, E. M. (1967). The growth of the control of grammar in imitation, comprehension and production. JCPsycholPsychiat 8. 31–9.Google Scholar
Luna, P. P. (1968). The development of actor and goal focus in the sentence structure of nursery, kindergarten, first-grade and second-grade school children. Unpublished M.A. thesis, Philippine Normal College, Manila, Philippines.Google Scholar
Menyuk, P. (1971). The acquisition and development of language. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Oestman, B. I. (1974). Tagalog child language. Unpublished M.A. Thesis, University of California.Google Scholar
Savin, H. B. & Perchonock, E. (1965). Grammatical structure and the immediate recall of English sentences. JVLVB 4. 348–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachter, P. (1976). The subject in Philippine languages: topic, actor, actor-topic, or none of the above. In Li, C. (ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schachter, P. & Otanes, F. (1972). Tagalog reference grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1966). Grammatical transformations and sentence comprehension in childhood and adulthood. JVLVB 5. 219–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Strohner, H. & Nelson, K. (1974). The young child's development of sentence comprehension: influence of event probability, non-verbal context, syntactic form, and strategies. ChDev 45. 567–76.Google Scholar
Tucker, G. R. (1971). Focus acquisition by Filipino children. Philippine Journal of Psychology 4. 21–4.Google Scholar
Turner, E. A. & Rommetveit, R. (1967). The acquisition of sentence voice and reversibility. ChDev 38. 649–60.Google ScholarPubMed