Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-5c569c448b-8lphq Total loading time: 0.376 Render date: 2022-07-06T00:49:14.884Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "useRatesEcommerce": false, "useNewApi": true } hasContentIssue true

Explicating the Conceptual Basis of Language*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

David McNeill
Affiliation:
University of Chicago

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Notes and discussion
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

Preparation of this Note was supported by grants to the University of Chicago from the Spencer Foundation and the Sloan Foundation. I am indebted, as always, to Nobuko B. McNeill for commenting on the paper. Address for correspondence: Center for Cognitive Science, University of Chicago, Beecher Hall, 5848 South University Ave., Chicago, 111. 60637, U.S.A.

References

Bates, E. (1980). Review of the conceptual basis of language. American Scientist 68. 702.Google Scholar
Bernstein, N. A. (1967). The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J. (1978). A realistic transformational grammar. In Halle, M., Bresnan, J. and Miller, G. A. (eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. JChLang 2. 119.Google Scholar
Foster, S. (1981). Review of the conceptual basis of language. JChLang 8. 660–3.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1978). Structural parallels between language and action in development. In Lock, A. J. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean. London: Edward Arnold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozhevnikov, V. A. & Chistovich, L. A. (1965). Speech: articulation and perception. Washington: U.S. Department of Commerce, Joint Publication Research Service (30,543).Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1975). Semiotic extension. In Solso, R. L. (ed.), Information processing and cognition. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1979). The conceptual basis of language. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google ScholarPubMed
Peirce, C. S. (19311958). The collected works of Charles Sanders Peirce (Harshorne, C. & Weiss, P., eds.). Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Humanities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Saussure, F. (1959). Course in general linguistics. New York: Philosophical Library. (First published in 1915.)Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. Chicago: Follett Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought and reality. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1961). Tractatus logico-philosophicus. (Trans, by Pears, D. F. and Mc-Guinness, B. F..) London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar

Save article to Kindle

To save this article to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Explicating the Conceptual Basis of Language*
Available formats
×

Save article to Dropbox

To save this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Explicating the Conceptual Basis of Language*
Available formats
×

Save article to Google Drive

To save this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Explicating the Conceptual Basis of Language*
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response

Please enter your response.

Your details

Please enter a valid email address.

Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *