Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-xm8r8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-20T18:22:01.232Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Factors affecting sensitivity and specificity of head-turning sign in the studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 April 2018

Pinar Soysal
Affiliation:
Kayseri Education and Research Hospital, Geriatric Center, Kayseri, Turkey
Ahmet Turan Isik*
Affiliation:
Unit for Aging Brain and Dementia, Department of Geriatric Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Ahmet Turan Isik, M.D., Unit for Aging Brain and Dementia, Department of Geriatric Medicine, Faculty of Medicine, Dokuz Eylul University, 35340 Balcova, Izmir, Turkey. Phone: +90-232-412-4341; Fax: +90-232-412-4339. Email: atisik@yahoo.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

We read with interest the comment by Williamson and Larner on our recent study titled “Attended With and Head-Turning Sign can be clinical markers of cognitive impairment in older adults” (Soysal et al., 2017). After the authors read the study, they re-examined their data according to the presence of Attended With (AW) and Head-Turning Sign (HTS), and compared their results with ours (Larner, 2014). Then, they found that while the sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value, and negative predictive value of AW in detecting cognitive impairment were similar to ours, HTS had lower sensitivity (80.95% vs. 65.0%) and higher specificity (64.7% vs. 95.0%) than our results (Larner, 2014; Soysal et al., 2017). We think that some methodological and cultural differences may explain these discrepancies between the two.

Type
Letter to the Editor
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2018 

We read with interest the comment by Williamson and Larner on our recent study titled “Attended With and Head-Turning Sign can be clinical markers of cognitive impairment in older adults” (Soysal et al., Reference Soysal, Usarel, Ispirli and Isik2017). After the authors read the study, they re-examined their data according to the presence of Attended With (AW) and Head-Turning Sign (HTS), and compared their results with ours (Larner, Reference Larner2014). Then, they found that while the sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value, and negative predictive value of AW in detecting cognitive impairment were similar to ours, HTS had lower sensitivity (80.95% vs. 65.0%) and higher specificity (64.7% vs. 95.0%) than our results (Larner, Reference Larner2014; Soysal et al., Reference Soysal, Usarel, Ispirli and Isik2017). We think that some methodological and cultural differences may explain these discrepancies between the two.

First, Williamson and Larner evaluated 16–92 years old patients, but we included just the patients who were over 60 years and had memory complaints. That may lead to decline in specificity, and rise in sensitivity of HTS. Because, previous studies showed that HTS was more prevalent in both benign senescent forgetfulness and memory-related cognitive impairment, such as Alzheimer's disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment, which are quite common among older people (Isik et al., Reference Isik, Soysal, Kaya and Usarel2018). On the other hand, the authors reported that 26.7% of the patients were referred by psychiatry services and neurologists, and 68.9% of them were referred by primary care physicians. It means that approximately all these patients had been already eliminated at least once for their memory problems when they were included in the study (Larner, Reference Larner2014). However, the most of our patients were first admitted to our clinic. Owing to this methodological difference, their specificity of HTS might be higher than ours.

Second reason may be that female patients are more prevalent in our study (64.8% vs. 47.2%). In general, women may become more dependent on others when facing difficulties, while men tend to feel obligated to overcome adversities without help (Fukui et al., Reference Fukui, Yamazaki and Kinno2011). Furthermore, it was shown that “help seeking” and depression were more prevalent in women with dementia, while aggressive behaviors were more frequent in men (Lövheim et al., Reference Lövheim, Sandman, Karlsson and Gustafson2009). When it is considered that HTS is essentially a behavior of help seeking, it can be said that female patients tend to turn their heads even if they have not any cognitive impairment. Last, older adults have lower educational level in our country, and thus they usually need to be approved even if they know the answer of the questions very well. Therefore, the gender and educational differences can explain why the sensitivity and specificity of HTS are different from Larner's study.

Both AW and HTS are fast, simple, and effective methods in screening cognitive impairment. However, some methodological and cultural differences should be kept in mind while evaluating the studies’ results.

Conflict of interest

None.

Description of authors’ roles

P. Soysal evaluated the comment, examined the data, and wrote the article. A.T. Isik evaluated the comment and wrote the article.

References

Fukui, T., Yamazaki, T. and Kinno, R. (2011). Can the ‘head-turning sign’ be a clinical marker of Alzheimer's disease? Dementia Geriatric Cognitive Disease Extra, 1, 310317.Google Scholar
Isik, A. T., Soysal, P., Kaya, D. and Usarel, C. (2018). Triple test, a diagnostic observation, can detect the cognitive impairment in older adults. Psychogeriatrics. In press.Google Scholar
Larner, A. J. (2014). Screening utility of the “attended alone” sign for subjective memory impairment. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 28, 364365.Google Scholar
Lövheim, H., Sandman, P. O., Karlsson, S. and Gustafson, Y. (2009). Sex differences in the prevalence of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. International Psychogeriatrics, 21, 469475.Google Scholar
Soysal, P., Usarel, C., Ispirli, G. and Isik, A. T. (2017). Attended With and Head-Turning Sign can be clinical markers of cognitive impairment in older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 29, 17631769.Google Scholar