Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-tn8tq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-19T13:38:55.221Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive and anatomical correlates of anosognosia in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and early-stage Alzheimer's disease

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2016

Gulben Senturk
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Istanbul Faculty of Medicine, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey
Basar Bilgic*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Istanbul Faculty of Medicine, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey Hulusi Behcet Life Sciences Research Center, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey
Ali Bilgin Arslan
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, USA
Ali Bayram
Affiliation:
Hulusi Behcet Life Sciences Research Center, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey Institute of Experimental Medicine, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey
Hasmet Hanagasi
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Istanbul Faculty of Medicine, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey
Hakan Gurvit
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Istanbul Faculty of Medicine, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey
Murat Emre
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Istanbul Faculty of Medicine, Istanbul University, Istanbul, Turkey
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Basar Bilgic, Dept. of Neurology, Istanbul Faculty of Medicine, Istanbul University, 34093 Capa, Istanbul, Turkey. Phone: +902124142000; Fax: +902125338575. Email: bilgicb@gmail.com.

Abstract

Background:

Anosognosia is a common feature in Alzheimer's disease (AD). The brain substrates of anosognosia are not fully understood, and less is known about the cognitive substrates of anosognosia in prodromal and early stages of AD.

Methods:

Fourty-seven patients with amnestic-type mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) (n = 26) and early-stage AD (n = 21) were included, and Clinical Insight Rating Scale and Anosognosia Questionnaire for Dementia (AQ-D) were used to assess anosognosia. A detailed neuropsychological battery was administered; each patient underwent a structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Correlation between anosognosia and performance in individual cognitive domains as well as correlation between anosognosia and cortical thickness values in regions of interest were assessed.

Results:

Performance of the anosognosic patients in Digit Ordering Test (DOT), Digit Span Backwards, and Clock Drawing Test (CDT) was significantly worse compared to non-anosognosic patients in the total study population and in the aMCI subgroup but not in AD group. AQ-D scores negatively correlated with Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), Digit Span Backwards and CDT scores in total group and MMSE, CVLT, DOT, and Digit Span Backwards scores in the aMCI group. No significant correlations were found between cortical thickness measurements and AQ-D scores in any of the patient populations.

Conclusions:

Anosognosia was associated with episodic memory, working memory, and executive functions in the total population and aMCI group, but no association was found in early-stage AD patients. Anosognosia in the early stages of AD may be related with non-structural changes such as hypoconnectivity rather than structural changes.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aalten, P. et al. (2006). Awareness and behavioral problems in dementia patients: a prospective study. International Psychogeriatrics, 18, 317. doi: 10.1017/S1041610205002772.Google Scholar
Addis, D. R. and Tippett, L. J. (2004). Memory of myself: autobiographical memory and identity in Alzheimer's disease. Memory, 12, 5674. doi: 10.1080/09658210244000423.Google Scholar
Clare, L. et al. (2012). Longitudinal trajectories of awareness in early-stage dementia. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 26, 140147. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31822c55c4.Google Scholar
Clare, L. et al. (2013). Memory awareness profiles differentiate mild cognitive impairment from early-stage dementia: evidence from assessments of performance monitoring and evaluative judgement. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 35, 266279. doi: 10.1159/000346735.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 594628. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A. and Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261288.Google Scholar
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B. and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage, 9, 179194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Destrieux, C., Fischl, B., Dale, A. and Halgren, E. (2010) Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. NeuroImage, 53, 115. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.010.Google Scholar
Edmonds, E. C. et al. (2014). Subjective cognitive complaints contribute to misdiagnosis of mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20, 836847. doi: 10.1017/S135561771400068X.Google Scholar
Feher, E. P. et al. (1991). Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 4, 136146.Google Scholar
Fischl, B. and Dale, A. M. (2000). Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97, 1105011055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797.Google Scholar
Giovannetti, T., Libon, D. J. and Hart, T. (2002). Awareness of naturalistic action errors in dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 633644.Google Scholar
Green, J., Goldstein, F. C., Sirockman, B. E. and Green, R. C. (1993). Variable awareness of deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 6, 159165.Google Scholar
Hannesdottir, K. and Morris, R. G. (2007). Primary and secondary anosognosia for memory impairment in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 43, 10201030.Google Scholar
Hanyu, H. et al. (2008). Neuroanatomical correlates of unawareness of memory deficits in early Alzheimer's disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 25, 347353. doi: 10.1159/000119594.Google Scholar
Harwood, D. G., Sultzer, D. L. and Wheatley, M. V. (2000). Impaired insight in Alzheimer disease: association with cognitive deficits, psychiatric symptoms, and behavioral disturbances. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 13, 8388.Google Scholar
Hornberger, M. et al. (2014). Ventromedial-frontopolar prefrontal cortex atrophy correlates with insight loss in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 616626. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22200.Google Scholar
Kashiwa, Y. et al. (2005). Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease: association with patient characteristics, psychiatric symptoms and cognitive deficits. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 59, 697704. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01439.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehrner, J. et al. (2015). Awareness of memory deficits in subjective cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. International Psychogeriatrics, 27, 357366. doi: 10.1017/S1041610214002245.Google Scholar
Loebel, J. P., Dager, S. R., Berg, G. and Hyde, T. S. (1990). Fluency of speech and self-awareness of memory deficit in alzheimer's disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 5, 4145. doi: 10.1002/gps.930050107.Google Scholar
Lopez, O. L. et al. (1994). Awareness of cognitive deficits and anosognosia in probable Alzheimer's disease. European Neurology, 34, 277282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangone, C. A. et al. (1991). Impaired insight in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 4, 189193.Google Scholar
Manos, P. J. and Wu, R. (1994). The ten point clock test: a quick screen and grading method for cognitive impairment in medical and surgical patients. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 24, 229244.Google Scholar
Markova, I. (2005). Insight in Psychiatry, 1st edn. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKhann, G. et al. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of department of health and human services task force on Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 34, 939944.Google Scholar
Mesulam, M. M. (1981). A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Annals of Neurology, 10, 309325. doi: 10.1002/ana.410100402.Google Scholar
Michon, A. et al. (1994). Relation of anosognosia to frontal lobe dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 805809.Google Scholar
Migliorelli, R. et al. (1995). Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease: a study of associated factors. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 7, 338344. doi: 10.1176/jnp.7.3.338.Google Scholar
Mograbi, D. C., Brown, R. G. and Morris, R. G. (2009). Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease – the petrified self. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 9891003. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.07.005.Google Scholar
Orfei, M. D. et al. (2010). Anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease: frequency and neuropsychological correlates. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry: Official Journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 11331140. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181dd1c50.Google Scholar
Ott, B. R. et al. (1996). Impaired awareness of deficits in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 10, 6876.Google Scholar
Perrotin, A. et al. (2015). Anosognosia in Alzheimer disease: disconnection between memory and self-related brain networks. Annals of Neurology, 78, 477486. doi: 10.1002/ana.24462.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E. et al. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 676682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676.Google Scholar
Reed, B. R., Jagust, W. J. and Coulter, L. (1993). Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease: relationships to depression, cognitive function, and cerebral perfusion. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 231244. doi: 10.1080/01688639308402560.Google Scholar
Ries, M. L. et al. (2007). Anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment: relationship to activation of cortical midline structures involved in self-appraisal. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 450461. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070488.Google Scholar
Ries, M. L. et al. (2012). Medial prefrontal functional connectivity–relation to memory self-appraisal accuracy in older adults with and without memory disorders. Neuropsychologia, 50, 603611. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.014.Google Scholar
Rosen, H. J. et al. (2010). Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive self-appraisal in neurodegenerative disease. NeuroImage, 49, 3358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.041.Google Scholar
Salmon, E. et al. (2006). Neural correlates of anosognosia for cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's disease. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 588597. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20203.Google Scholar
Scherling, C. S. et al. (2016). Decreased self-appraisal accuracy on cognitive tests of executive functioning is a predictor of decline in mild cognitive impairment. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 120. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00120.Google Scholar
Sedaghat, F. et al. (2010). Brain SPECT findings of anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's disease, 21, 641647. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-090631.Google Scholar
Sevush, S. (1999). Relationship between denial of memory deficit and dementia severity in Alzheimer disease. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 12, 8894.Google Scholar
Spalletta, G. et al. (2014). Neuroanatomical correlates of awareness of illness in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment who will or will not convert to Alzheimer's disease. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 61, 183195. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.10.010.Google Scholar
Starkstein, S. E. et al. (1996). Two domains of anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 61, 485490.Google Scholar
Starkstein, S. E. et al. (1997). Prospective longitudinal study of depression and anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 171, 4752.Google Scholar
Starkstein, S. E. (2014). Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease: diagnosis, frequency, mechanism and clinical correlates. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 61, 6473. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starkstein, S. E., Jorge, R., Mizrahi, R. and Robinson, R. G. (2006). A diagnostic formulation for anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 77, 719725. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.085373.Google Scholar
Tabert, M. H. et al. (2002). Functional deficits in patients with mild cognitive impairment: prediction of AD. Neurology, 58, 758764.Google Scholar
Vasterling, J. J., Seltzer, B., Foss, M. W. and Vanderbrook, V. (1995). Unawareness of deficit in Alzheimer's disease: domain-specific differences and disease correlates. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 8, 2632.Google Scholar
Verhey, F. R. J., Rozendaal, N., Ponds, R. W. H. M. and Jolles, J. (1993). Dementia, awareness and depression. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 8, 851856. doi: 10.1002/gps.930081008.Google Scholar
Vogel, A. et al. (2005). Cognitive and functional neuroimaging correlate for anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20, 238246. doi: 10.1002/gps.1272.Google Scholar
Weinstein, E. A., Friedland, R. P. and Wagner, E. E. (1994). Denial/unawareness of impairment and symbolic behavior in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 7, 176184.Google Scholar
Winblad, B. et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the international working group on mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Internal Medicine, 256, 240246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x.Google Scholar
Zamboni, G. et al. (2013). Neuroanatomy of impaired self-awareness in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 49, 668678. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.011.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Senturk supplementary material

Table S1

Download Senturk supplementary material(File)
File 35.3 KB