Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-8zxtt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T20:46:28.578Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Childhood infectious diseases and old age cognitive functioning: a nationally representative sample of community-dwelling older adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 July 2020

A. Rotstein*
Affiliation:
Department of Community Mental Health, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
S. Z. Levine
Affiliation:
Department of Community Mental Health, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Anat Rotstein, Department of Community Mental Health, Faculty of Social Welfare and Health Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa3498838, Israel. Phone: +972 5226 29707; Fax +972 3761 7374. Email: rotstein.anat@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Cumulative evidence suggests that health-related risk factors during midlife and old-age are associated with cognitive impairment. However, studies are needed to clarify the association between early-life risk factors and impaired cognitive functioning to increment existing knowledge.

Objective:

To examine the association between childhood infectious diseases and late-life cognitive functioning in a nationally representative sample of older adults.

Participants:

Eligible respondents were 2994 community-dwelling individuals aged 65–85.

Measurements:

Cognitive functioning was assessed using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Childhood infectious diseases (i.e. chicken pox, measles, and mumps) were self-reported. The study covariates were age, sex, highest educational level achieved, smoking status, body mass index, and depression. The primary statistical analysis examined the association between the number of childhood infectious diseases and total MMSE scores, accounting for all study covariates. Regression models of progressive complexity were examined for parsimony. The robustness of the primary results was tested in 17 sensitivity analyses.

Results:

The most parsimonious model was a linear adjusted model (Bayesian Information Criterion = 12646.09). Late-life cognitive functioning significantly improved as the number of childhood infectious diseases increased (β = 0.18; 95% CI = 0.11, 0.26; p < 0.001). This effect was not significantly attenuated in all sensitivity analyses.

Conclusion:

The current study results are consistent with prior ecological findings indicating that some childhood infectious diseases are associated with better cognitive functioning in old-age. This points to an early-life modifiable risk factor associated with older-life cognitive functioning. Our results may reflect selective mortality and/or beneficial effects via hormetic processes.

Type
Original Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2020

Introduction

Impaired cognitive functioning among community-dwelling older adults is estimated to have a prevalence of 18.3% in the UK (Rait et al., Reference Rait2005), ranging between 8.7% and 22.2% in the United States (Plassman et al., Reference Plassman2008; Langa et al., Reference Langa2008). Impaired cognitive functioning is associated with increased risks of Alzheimer’s disease (Bäckman et al., Reference Bäckman2005) and mortality (Langa et al., Reference Langa2008). The identification of risk factors and preventative strategies are primary objectives in research on impaired cognitive functioning in older adults (Brayne, Reference Brayne2007; Treichler and Jeste, Reference Treichler and Jeste2019). Indeed, general health-related risk factors have been identified during midlife and old age (Launer, Reference Launer2005; Launer, Reference Launer2007; Kaufman and Perales-Puchalt, Reference Kaufman and Perales-Puchalt2019; Lee et al., Reference Lee2019; Shaaban et al., Reference Shaaban2019; Tu et al., Reference Tu2020; Walker et al., Reference Walker2019). Nonetheless, a recent shift has seen studies that examine the role of early-life risk factors to increment current knowledge of risk factors (Grainger et al., Reference Grainger2019; Launer, Reference Launer2007; Whalley et al., Reference Whalley2006; Williamson and Leroi, Reference Williamson and Leroi2019).

Few observational studies have examined the association between health in early-life and late-life cognition. Two studies found that retrospective reports of better global childhood health were associated with improved cognitive functioning in old age (Luo and Waite, Reference Luo and Waite2005; Yi et al., Reference Yi2007). Three others studies reported a null association (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes2012; Yount, Reference Yount2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang2018). A single study examined the association between childhood infectious diseases and late-life cognitive functioning and showed mixed disease effects (Case and Paxson, Reference Case and Paxson2009). This study used an ecological design to examine childhood health based on region-level historical data and linked it with performance on cognitive tests during adulthood. Ecological studies are valuable for assessing the global disease burden, yet may not accurately reflect the true association for a given group (Lilienfeld, Reference Lilienfeld1983; Piantadosi et al., Reference Piantadosi1988). In addition, ecological studies raise concerns of the ecological fallacy (Robinson, Reference Robinson1950), whereby lower and higher levels of abstraction are not interchangeable (i.e. implying individual-level characteristics from collective ones may be erroneous).

Competing views exist concerning the long-term impact of childhood infectious diseases. Exposure to childhood infectious diseases has been shown to be associated with both negative (Case and Paxson, Reference Case and Paxson2009; Dalman et al., Reference Dalman2008) and positive health outcomes in later life (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander2000; Amirian et al., Reference Amirian2016; Case and Paxson, Reference Case and Paxson2009). Negative outcomes may be explained via direct effects of pathogens and indirect effects of inflammatory response (Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker2012). Positive outcomes may be attributed to mild stress-induced repeated stimulation of protective mechanisms in cells and organisms which has a wide range of health-promoting and life span-extending effects (Rattan, Reference Rattan2008), and/or selective mortality.

The current study aims to examine the direction of the association between common childhood infectious diseases and late-life cognitive functioning in a nationally representative sample of community-dwelling older adults.

Methods

Design and data source

The current study cohort was based on a nationally representative sample of community-dwelling individuals aged 65 and over from the Republic of Ireland. Data were obtained from the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (Kenny, Reference Kenny2018). This large-scale, nationally representative, aging study was conducted in Ireland. The current data were derived from interviews, undertaken between January 2016 and December 2016. The response rate was 62.0%. The study was approved by the Faculty of Health Sciences Research Ethics Committee in Trinity College Dublin. Further details of sampling and study design have been described elsewhere (Whelan and Savva, Reference Whelan and Savva2013).

Participants

Eligible respondents for this study included community-dwelling individuals aged 65–85 (N = 3434). People with known or suspected dementia were not recruited. A subsample of people who did not have any information on past illnesses of any sort were removed (N = 440). Therefore, the final sample was 2994 people.

Cognitive functioning

Cognitive functioning was assessed with the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., Reference Folstein1975). The MMSE is one of the most widely used research and screening measures of cognitive functioning in aging populations (Folstein et al., 1975, Reference Folstein1983). It is easy to administer, available in over 15 languages (Steis and Schrauf, 2009), and has high levels of acceptability by health professionals and researchers as a diagnostic instrument (Nieuwenhuis-Mark, 2010). The scale has 20 items to assess orientation, recall, attention, calculation, language, and visuospatial abilities. It is divided into two sections, the first of which requires vocal responses only and covers orientation, memory, and attention; the maximum score is 21. The second part tests cognitive abilities to name, follow verbal and written commands, write a sentence spontaneously, and copy a polygon; the maximum score is nine. The maximum overall score is 30, with higher scores representing better cognitive functioning (Folstein et al., Reference Folstein1983). Questions were asked and scored immediately. The test was not timed. The tester was instructed first to make the individual comfortable, to establish rapport, to praise successes, and to avoid pressing on items which the individual finds difficult.

In the current study, MMSE scores were computed, based on the original instructions (Folstein et al., Reference Folstein1975) for each of the five subscales: orientation, registration, attention and calculation, recall, and language. Total scores were computed as well. For the primary analysis, the total test scores were not dichotomized into categories of impaired cognitive functioning and intact cognitive functioning because dichotomizing leads to information lost, increased risk of a false positive and may seriously underestimate the extent of variation in outcome such that individuals close to but on opposite sides of the cut point are characterized very differently rather than similarly (Altman and Royston, Reference Altman and Royston2006). However, for robustness, when recomputing the sensitivity analysis, the total test scores were dichotomized into categories of impaired and intact cognitive functioning.

Childhood infectious diseases

The childhood infectious diseases selected for this study were chickenpox, measles, and mumps. These were chosen because they are viral diseases, infection with which are followed by an enduring immunity (Simpson, Reference Simpson1952) and were common at the time of our sample’s childhood years due to recurrent outbreaks (London and Yorke, Reference London and Yorke1973). Childhood infectious diseases were assessed self-reported by asking participant if they have had chicken pox, measles, and mumps in childhood. Responses were classified as “yes” (score of 1), “no” (score of 0), or “don’t know” (coded as missing values). A total number of childhood infectious diseases score was computed for each person as the sum of the reported diseases of chicken pox, measles, and mumps. The total score ranged from 0 (having had no diseases) to 3 (having had all three diseases).

Covariates

The following covariates were included in the analyses to adjust for potential confounding, model effect modification and to define subgroups of particular interest with possible differential effects on cognitive functioning. The covariates considered (and entered in the following order) were demographics, metabolic, and psychiatric. Demographic covariates were age at the time of data collection and sex (van der Flier and Scheltens, Reference van der Flier and Scheltens2005), highest education achieved [primary or none, secondary, third, or higher (Solfrizzi et al., Reference Solfrizzi2004; Tervo et al., Reference Tervo2004; Tyas et al., Reference Tyas2007)], and smoking status at the time of data collection [classified as smoker or non-smoker (Swan and Lessov-Schlaggar, Reference Swan and Lessov-Schlaggar2007)]. The metabolic covariate was body mass index categories [0–25, 25–30, 30–40, 40+ (Atti et al., Reference Atti2008)]. The psychiatric variable was depression (Butters et al., Reference Butters2008) as measured by the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (Robins et al., Reference Robins1988). This established instrument is widely used for assessing a clinical diagnosis of major depression in epidemiological and clinical studies. Respondents received binary scores depending on whether they had fulfilled criteria for a major depressive episode in the last 12 months or not.

Analytic approach

First, missing values were examined, and the data were imputed accordingly using MICE (Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations; van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2010) in R package for multivariate imputation by chained equations. Second, sample characteristics were computed.

Third, the primary statistical analysis tested the association between the total MMSE scores as a function of the number of childhood infectious diseases using regression models. The assumptions of the regression models were tested. Visual inspection of residual figures was performed in order to reveal deviations from homoscedasticity or normality. An inspection for normality of error terms followed using a histogram and probability plots of the residuals. Independence of the error term was examined through a scatter plot of residuals by the predicted values to show that no discernible association existed. Next, multiple regression analysis models were computed. Regression models were computed in ascending complexity and tested without adjustment and adjusted for confounding of age at the time of data collection, sex, highest education achieved, smoking status, body mass index, and depression. Models were numbered as follows. The first model accounted for a linear effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases on MMSE scores (model 1 hereafter). The second model accounted for a linear effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases, age, sex, education level, smoking status, body mass index, and depression on MMSE scores (model 2 hereafter). The third model accounted for a quadratic effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases on MMSE scores (model 3 hereafter). The fourth model accounted for a quadratic effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases, age, sex, education level, smoking status, body mass index, and depression on MMSE scores (model 4 hereafter). The fifth model accounted for a cubic effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases on MMSE scores (model 5 hereafter). The sixth model accounted for a cubic effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases, age, sex, education level, smoking status, body mass index, and depression on MMSE scores (model 6 hereafter).

Fourth, the six models were compared for parsimony based on the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) for model selection (Schwarz, Reference Schwarz1978), similar to prior research (Rotstein et al., Reference Rotstein2018). Lower BIC values represent more parsimonious models and so are a better fit to the data. The best fitting model was then chosen based on the lowest BIC score and plotted using the ggplot2 library (Wickham, Reference Wickham2011). All analyses were computed in R (R Core Team, 2018).

Fifth, the robustness of the primary results was tested in 17 sensitivity analyses. The most parsimonious regression model was recomputed in subgroups with differential effects on cognitive functioning. First, the most parsimonious model was recomputed without sex as a covariate for females then males, since females are at greater risk for impaired cognitive functioning (van der Flier and Scheltens, Reference van der Flier and Scheltens2005). Second, the most parsimonious model was recomputed for persons aged 65–75 then aged 75–85, since cognitive functioning is related to age (van der Flier and Scheltens, Reference van der Flier and Scheltens2005). Third, the most parsimonious model was computed for each of the three diseases (i.e. chicken pox, measles, and mumps) separately to show their individual effect on cognitive functioning. Fourth, the most parsimonious model was computed using a saturated model in which dummy variables for one, two, or three childhood infectious diseases were entered as covariates. Fifth, the most parsimonious model was computed for each of the MMSE subscales (i.e. orientation, registration, attention and calculation, recall, and language) separately. Sixth, the most parsimonious model was computed without education to account for effects of mediation. Seventh, possible methodological confounders were considered. Although dichotomizing may be problematic (Altman and Royston, Reference Altman and Royston2006), for robustness and because dichotomous models have increased clinical lure, the most parsimonious model was recomputed dichotomized to account for intact cognitive functioning versus impaired cognitive functioning. Impaired cognitive functioning was defined using a cutoff score of 1.5 standard deviations below the mean score of the MMSE (Palmer et al., Reference Palmer2002). Additionally, the most parsimonious model was recomputed for observed data with missing values and compared to the primary analysis based on imputed data (Sterne et al., Reference Sterne2009).

Results

Data imputation and sample characteristics

The data were imputed using multiple imputations because simulation studies showed the technique to be robust even if the data are not missing at random (Sinharay et al., Reference Sinharay2001). The analytic sample consisted of 2994 older adults. The sample had a mean age of 73.50 (SD = 6.18) and 54.2% (N = 1,622) were female. The average MMSE score was 28.37 (SD = 2.14). Most of the participants had a history of measles (89.7%; N = 2,684), chicken pox (67.8%; N = 2,029), and/or mumps (62.3%; N = 1,866). See Table 1 for all sample characteristics.

Table 1. Sample characteristics

Primary statistical analysis: cognitive functioning and the number of childhood infectious diseases

Comparison of the six regression models showed that model 2 was the most parsimonious (BIC = 12646.09; see supplementary Table S1 for comparison of BIC values for all regression models published as supplementary material online attached to the electronic version of this paper at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610220001404). This model accounted for the linear effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases on MMSE scores (Figure 1), adjusted for age, sex, education level, smoking status, body mass index, and depression. Late-life cognitive functioning improved as the number of childhood infectious diseases increased. Each disease incremented the MMSE score by 0.18. For two diseases, the MMSE total score increased by 0.36. Having had three diseases increased the MMSE score by 0.54. See Table 2 for model statistics.

Note. For this adjusted model, having had one disease increases the Mini-Mental State Examination score by 0.18, all other covariates equal. Having had two diseases increases the Mini-Mental State Examination total score by 0.36, all other covariates equal. Having had three diseases increases the score by 0.54, all other covariates equal.

Figure 1. The linear adjusted effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases on Mini-Mental State Examination scores.

Table 2. Model 2 statistics

Note. Model 2 is the linear effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases, age, sex, education level, smoking status, body mass index, and depression on Mini-Mental State Examination scores.

Sex reference group = male.

Education reference group = primary/none.

Smoking status reference group = non-smoker.

Body mass index reference group = 0–24.99.

Depression reference group = not having had a major depressive episode in the last 12 months.

Abbreviations: BMI = Body Mass Index; No. diseases = the number of childhood infectious diseases.

Sensitivity analyses

Model 2, accounting for the linear effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases, age, education level, smoking status, body mass index, and depression on MMSE scores, was recomputed for all sensitivity analyses. A significant effect of childhood infectious diseases was found for males (N = 1372), females (N = 1622), persons aged 65–75 (N = 1828), persons aged 75–85 (N = 1166), each of the childhood infectious diseases separately (chicken pox, measles, and mumps), a saturated model (in which the effect size increased as the number of childhood infectious diseases increased), each subscale of the MMSE separately, a dichotomized regression model (intact cognitive functioning vs. impaired cognitive functioning), a model without education, and observed data with missing values (in which 191 people were excluded due to missingness; N = 2803). See supplementary Tables S2S17 for model statistics (published as supplementary material online attached to the electronic version of this paper).

Discussion

The current study examined the association between childhood infectious diseases and old age cognitive functioning, among community-dwelling older adults, using data from a representative national sample. The primary results show that late-life cognitive functioning improved as the number of childhood infectious diseases increased. Specifically, for each additional disease, there was an improvement in cognition reflecting a 0.18 MMSE total point increase. The primary result was not statistically significantly attenuated in a series of sensitivity analyses, including four subgroups with potentially differential cognitive functioning, two methodological confounders, two alternative models, each infectious disease and each MMSE subscale examined. The strongest effects were found among females and among those aged 75–85 years.

The current study results are consistent with prior ecological findings showing that some childhood infectious diseases (i.e. influenza) are associated with better cognitive functioning (i.e. successful counting) in old age (Case and Paxson, Reference Case and Paxson2009). The literature provides related examples of early-life infectious diseases having a protective effect on health in later life. For instance, the varicella zoster virus that causes chickenpox is consistently reported to have an inverse association with glioma (Amirian et al., Reference Amirian2016). Similarly, measles and/or combined childhood infections (chicken pox, measles, mumps, pertussis, and rubella) were found protective for Hodgkin’s disease (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander2000).

Consistent with prior studies that have reported positive long-term health effects of childhood infectious diseases, our results contribute to identifying modifiable risk factors positively related to older-life cognitive functioning. Tentative explanations of our results are (I) selective mortality; and/or (II) that low levels of exposure to harmful agents may have beneficial effects via hormetic processes (Rattan, Reference Rattan2008). Future studies may identify possible mechanisms.

The current study has several limitations. First, selective mortality may have occurred, biasing the current study results. Second, our results are restricted to chickenpox, measles, and mumps. Future research is warranted to ascertain the effects of other childhood infectious diseases on cognitive functioning in old age. Third, childhood infectious diseases were assessed through self-reports which are less reliable than testing for serum antibodies (Mortimer, Reference Mortimer1978). Specifically, higher rates of prevalence for actual mumps antibodies are expected since one person in three who contracts mumps does not present any symptoms (Mortimer, Reference Mortimer1978). Still, prior evidence has suggested that these assessments of childhood health have reasonably good reliability and validity (Haas, Reference Haas2007). In addition, past epidemiological studies have found similar prevalence rates as those reported in the current study for chickenpox, measles, and self-reported mumps (Mortimer, Reference Mortimer1978; Pollock and Golding, Reference Pollock and Golding1993; Stocks, Reference Stocks1928). Nonetheless, it should be noted that a selection bias may have occurred as respondents with better cognitive function may have recalled their childhood experience of infectious diseases more accurately. Fourth, the MMSE relies on an interviewer administration and rating which may introduce further biases. Although the interviewer is given specific instructions for administration, differences resulting from the skill and style of the interviewer in eliciting answers and in scoring the answers given by the subject exist (Bowie et al., Reference Bowie1999). Fifth, the sample did not include individuals with verified diagnoses of dementia or mild cognitive decline. Future studies may focus on individuals with and without dementia to extend the current findings.

Conclusions

In summary, despite its limitations, the current study draws on a large representative sample and is the first to examine the individual and cumulative effects of common childhood infectious diseases (chickenpox, measles, and mumps) on late-life cognitive functioning. The study results show a consistent positive association between the number of childhood infectious diseases and late-life cognitive functioning. Childhood infectious diseases may be a future direction for modifiable risk factors related to older-life cognitive functioning.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Description of authors’ role

Both authors designed the study, conducted the statistical analyses, interpreted the data, and prepared the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

Based on data collected through the Irish Longitudinal study on Ageing. Accessed via the Irish Social Science Data Archive -www.ucd.ie/issda.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610220001404

References

Alexander, F. E. et al. (2000). Risk factors for Hodgkin’s disease by Epstein-Barr virus (EBV) status: prior infection by EBV and other agents. British Journal of Cancer, 82, 11171121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altman, D. G. and Royston, P. (2006). The cost of dichotomising continuous variables. BMJ, 332(7549), 1080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amirian, E. S. et al. (2016). History of chickenpox in glioma risk: a report from the glioma international case–control study (GICC). Cancer Medicine, 5, 13521358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atti, A. R. et al. (2008). Late-life body mass index and dementia incidence: nine-year follow-up data from the Kungsholmen Project. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 56(1), 111116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bäckman, L. et al. (2005). Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 19(4), 520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, L. L. et al. (2012). Effects of early-life adversity on cognitive decline in older African Americans and whites. Neurology, 79(24), 23212327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowie, P. et al. (1999). Should the Mini Mental State Examination be used to monitor dementia treatments? The Lancet, 354(9189), 15271528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brayne, C. (2007). The elephant in the room—healthy brains in later life, epidemiology and public health. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(3), 233239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butters, M. A. et al. (2008). Pathways linking late-life depression to persistent cognitive impairment and dementia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 10(3), 345.Google ScholarPubMed
Case, A. and Paxson, C. (2009). Early life health and cognitive function in old age. American Economic Review, 99(2), 104109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalman, C. et al. (2008). Infections in the CNS during childhood and the risk of subsequent psychotic illness: a cohort study of more than one million Swedish subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 5965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. F. et al. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. F. et al. (1983). The mini-mental state examination. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40(7), 812.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grainger, S. A. et al. (2019). An investigation into early-life stress and cognitive function in older age. International Psychogeriatrics, 15. doi: 10.1017/S1041610219001583 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haas, S. A. (2007). The long-term effects of poor childhood health: An assessment and application of retrospective reports. Demography, 44(1), 113135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, C. S. and Perales-Puchalt, J. (2019). Cardiovascular contributions to dementia: beyond individual risk factors. International Psychogeriatrics, 31(10), 13871389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenny, R. A. (2018). The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA), 2012-2013.Google Scholar
Khandaker, G. M. et al. (2012). Childhood infection and adult schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of population-based studies. Schizophrenia Research, 139(1–3), 161168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langa, K. M. et al. (2008). Trends in the prevalence and mortality of cognitive impairment in the United States: is there evidence of a compression of cognitive morbidity? Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 4(2), 134144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Launer, L. J. (2005). The epidemiologic study of dementia: a life-long quest? Neurobiology of Aging, 26(3), 335340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Launer, L. J. (2007). Next steps in Alzheimer’s disease research: interaction between epidemiology and basic science. Current Alzheimer Research, 4(2), 141143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, E. E. et al. (2019). High prevalence and adverse health effects of loneliness in community-dwelling adults across the lifespan: role of wisdom as a protective factor. International Psychogeriatrics, 31(10), 14471462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lilienfeld, A. M. (1983). Practical limitations of epidemiologic methods. Environmental Health Perspectives, 52, 38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
London, W. P. and Yorke, J. A. (1973). Recurrent outbreaks of measles, chickenpox and mumps: I. Seasonal variation in contact rates. American Journal of Epidemiology, 98(6), 453468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, Y. and Waite, L. J. (2005). The impact of childhood and adult SES on physical, mental, and cognitive well-being in later life. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60(2), S93S101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mortimer, P. P. (1978). Mumps prophylaxis in the light of a new test for antibody. British Medical Journal, 2(6151), 15231524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieuwenhuis-Mark, R. E. (2010). The death knoll for the MMSE: has it outlived its purpose? Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 23(3), 151157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, K. et al. (2002). Differential evolution of cognitive impairment in nondemented older persons: results from the Kungsholmen Project. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(3), 436442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piantadosi, S. et al. (1988). The ecological fallacy. American Journal of Epidemiology, 127(5), 893904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plassman, B. L. et al. (2008). Prevalence of cognitive impairment without dementia in the United States. Annals of Internal Medicine, 148(6), 427434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollock, J. I. and Golding, J. (1993). Social epidemiology of chickenpox in two British national cohorts. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 47(4), 274281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rait, G. et al. (2005). Prevalence of cognitive impairment: results from the MRC trial of assessment and management of older people in the community. Age and Ageing, 34(3), 242248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rattan, S. I. S. (2008). Hormesis in aging. Ageing Research Reviews, 7(1), 6378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robins, L. N. et al. (1988). The Composite International Diagnostic Interview: an epidemiologic instrument suitable for use in conjunction with different diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45(12), 10691077.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, W. (1950). Ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals, American Sociological Review, 15, 351357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotstein, A. et al. (2018). Age of onset and quality of life among males and females with schizophrenia: a national study. European Psychiatry, 53, 100106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics, 6(2), 461464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaaban, C. E. et al. (2019). Independent and joint effects of vascular and cardiometabolic risk factor pairs for risk of all-cause dementia: a prospective population-based study. International Psychogeriatrics, 31(10), 14211432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, R. H. (1952). Infectiousness of communicable diseases in the household (measles, chickenpox, and mumps). Lancet, 549554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinharay, S. et al. (2001). The use of multiple imputation for the analysis of missing data. Psychological Methods, 6(4), 317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solfrizzi, V. et al. (2004). Vascular risk factors, incidence of MCI, and rates of progression to dementia. Neurology, 63(10), 18821891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steis, M. R. and Schrauf, R. W. (2009). A review of translations and adaptations of the Mini-Mental State Examination in languages other than English and Spanish. Research in Gerontological Nursing, 2(3), 214224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterne, J. A. C. et al. (2009). Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ, 338, b2393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stocks, P. (1928). A study of the epidemiology of measles. Annals of Eugenics, 3(3–4), 361398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swan, G. E. and Lessov-Schlaggar, C. N. (2007). The effects of tobacco smoke and nicotine on cognition and the brain. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 259273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2018). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Austria: R Core Team.Google Scholar
Tervo, S. et al. (2004). Incidence and risk factors for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based three-year follow-up study of cognitively healthy elderly subjects. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 17(3), 196203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treichler, E. B. and Jeste, D. V. (2019). Cognitive decline in older adults: applying multiple perspectives to develop novel prevention strategies. International Psychogeriatrics, 31(7), 913916.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tu, L. et al. (2020). Trajectories of cognitive function and their determinants in older people: 12 years of follow-up in the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey. International Psychogeriatrics, 32(6), 765775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyas, S. L. et al. (2007). Transitions to mild cognitive impairments, dementia, and death: findings from the Nun Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 165(11), 12311238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Buuren, S. and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, C. G. (2010). Mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 168.Google Scholar
van der Flier, W. M. and Scheltens, P. (2005). Epidemiology and risk factors of dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 76(Suppl. 5), v2v7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, K. A. et al. (2019). Association of midlife to late-life blood pressure patterns with incident dementia. JAMA, 322(6), 535545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalley, L. J. et al. (2006). A life-course approach to the aetiology of late-onset dementias. The Lancet Neurology, 5(1), 8796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whelan, B. J. and Savva, G. M. (2013). Design and methodology of the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 61, S265S268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H. (2011). ggplot2. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Computational Statistics, 3(2), 180185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williamson, W. and Leroi, I. (2019). Thinking about dementia: is childhood too early? International Psychogeriatrics, 31(12), 16891690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yi, Z. et al. (2007). The association of childhood socioeconomic conditions with healthy longevity at the oldest-old ages in China. Demography, 44(3), 497518.Google ScholarPubMed
Yount, K. M. (2016). Gender, resources across the life course, and cognitive functioning in Egypt. In: N. Chamlou and M. Karshenas (Eds.), Women, Work and Welfare in the Middle East and North Africa (pp. 107133). Singapore: World Scientific.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Z. et al. (2018). The long arm of childhood in China: early-life conditions and cognitive function among middle-aged and older adults. Journal of Aging and Health, 30(8), 13191344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics

Figure 1

Figure 1. The linear adjusted effect of the number of childhood infectious diseases on Mini-Mental State Examination scores.

Note. For this adjusted model, having had one disease increases the Mini-Mental State Examination score by 0.18, all other covariates equal. Having had two diseases increases the Mini-Mental State Examination total score by 0.36, all other covariates equal. Having had three diseases increases the score by 0.54, all other covariates equal.
Figure 2

Table 2. Model 2 statistics

Supplementary material: File

Rotstein and Levine supplementary material

Rotstein and Levine supplementary material

Download Rotstein and Levine supplementary material(File)
File 65.7 KB