Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T10:04:00.427Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The association of educational attainment, cognitive level of job, and leisure activities during the course of adulthood with cognitive performance in old age: the role of openness to experience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 November 2015

Andreas Ihle*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Michel Oris
Affiliation:
Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Delphine Fagot
Affiliation:
Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Christian Maggiori
Affiliation:
School of Social Work, University of Applied Science and Arts Western Switzerland, Fribourg, Switzerland
Matthias Kliegel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Andreas Ihle, CIGEV, University of Geneva, route des Acacias 54, 1227 Carouge, Switzerland. Phone: +41 22 37 98308. Email: Andreas.Ihle@unige.ch.

Abstract

Background:

The relevance of mental health for everyday life functioning and well-being is crucial. In this context, higher educational attainment, higher cognitive level of one's occupation, and more engaging in stimulating leisure activities have been found to be associated with better cognitive functioning in old age. Yet, the detailed pattern of the potential interplay of such a cognitively engaged lifestyle with personality dimensions, such as openness to experience, in their relations to cognitive functioning remains unclear.

Methods:

Two thousand eight hundred and twelve older adults served as sample for the present study. Psychometric tests on verbal abilities and processing speed were administered. In addition, individuals were retrospectively interviewed on their educational attainment, occupation, and regarding 18 leisure activities that had been carried out in mid-life. Moreover, openness to experience was assessed.

Results:

We found that the effect of openness to experience on cognitive functioning was mediated by educational attainment, cognitive level of job, and engaging in different leisure activities. Data were not better described by alternative moderation models testing for interactive (i.e. dependent) effects of openness to experience and cognitively stimulating engagement.

Conclusions:

To explain interindividual differences in cognitive functioning in old age, present data are in line with a mechanism in which individuals with high openness to experience may have been more engaged in stimulating activities in early and mid-life. Possibly by increasing their cognitive reserve throughout adulthood, this may finally enhance their cognitive performance level later in old age.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bielak, A. A. M., Cherbuin, N., Bunce, D. and Anstey, K. J. (2014). Preserved differentiation between physical activity and cognitive performance across young, middle, and older adulthood over 8 years. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69, 523532.Google Scholar
Chamorro-Premuzic, T. and Furnham, A. (2004). A possible model for understanding the personality-intelligence interface. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 249264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deltour, J. (1993). Echelle de Vocabulaire Mill Hill de J.C. Raven. [Raven Mill Hill Vocabulary Scale]. Braine-le Chateau, Belgium: Editions l’Application des Techniques Modernes.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., Peterson, J. B. and Gray, J. R. (2014). Openness to experience, intellect, and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96, 4652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabriel, R., Oris, M., Studer, M. and Baeriswyl, M. (in press). The persistence of social stratification? A life course perspective on old-age poverty in Switzerland. Swiss Journal of Sociology.Google Scholar
Gatz, M., Svedberg, P., Pedersen, N. L., Mortimer, J.A., Berg, S. and Johansson, B. (2001). Education and the risk of Alzheimer's disease: findings from the study of dementia in Swedish twins. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 56B, 292300.Google Scholar
Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S. and Lindenberger, U. (2008). Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9, 165.Google Scholar
Hultsch, D. F., Hammer, M. and Small, B. J. (1993). Age differences in cognitive performance in later life: relationships to self-reported health and activity life style. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 48, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J. and Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging, 14, 245263.Google Scholar
Ihle, A., Oris, O., Fagot, D., Baeriswyl, M., Guichard, E. and Kliegel, M. (2015a). The association of leisure activities in middle adulthood with cognitive performance in old age: the moderating role of educational level. Gerontology, 61, 543550.Google Scholar
Ihle, A., Schnitzspahn, K., Rendell, P. G., Luong, C. and Kliegel, M. (2012). Age benefits in everyday prospective memory: the influence of personal task importance, use of reminders and everyday stress. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19, 84101.Google Scholar
Ihle, A. et al. (2015b). The role of cognitive resources for subjective work ability and health in nursing. European Journal of Ageing, 12, 131140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janzarik, W. (2012). Cognitive losses in old age, cognitive reserve, structural resistance. Nervenarzt, 83, 3132.Google Scholar
Karp, A., Paillard-Borg, S., Wang, H. X., Silverstein, M., Winblad, B. and Fratiglioni, L. (2006). Mental, physical and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia risk. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21, 6573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R. and Costa, P. T. Jr. (1999). A five-factor theory of personality. In Pervin, L. A. and John, O. P. (eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, 2nd edn, (pp.139153). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
McDowell, I., Xi, G. L., Lindsay, J. and Tierney, M. (2007). Mapping the connections between education and dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 127141.Google Scholar
Nucci, M., Mapelli, D. and Mondini, S. (2012). Cognitive reserve index questionnaire (CRIq): a new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 218226.Google Scholar
Orrell, M. and Sahakian, B. (1995). Education and dementia: research evidence supports the concept of “use it or lose it”. British Medical Journal, 310, 951952.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2014). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Rammstedt, B. and John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: a 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 203212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schooler, C. and Mulatu, M. S. (2001). The reciprocal effects of leisure time activities and intellectual functioning in older people: a longitudinal analysis. Psychology and Aging, 16, 466482.Google Scholar
Schooler, C., Mulatu, M. S. and Oates, G. (1999). The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychology and Aging, 14, 483506.Google Scholar
Soubelet, A. and Salthouse, T. A. (2010). The role of activity engagement in the relations between openness/intellect and cognition. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 896901.Google Scholar
Specht, J., Egloff, B. and Schmukle, S. C. (2011). Stability and change of personality across the life course: the impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 862882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448460.Google Scholar
Stern, Y. et al. (1995). Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow - implications for a reserve against alzheimers-disease pathology. Neurology, 45, 5560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, A. M. and Stern, Y. (2011). Cognitive reserve in aging. Current Alzheimer Research, 8, 354360.Google Scholar
Wainwright, M. A., Wright, M. J., Luciano, M., Geffen, G. M. and Martin, N. G. (2008). Genetic covariation among facets of openness to experience and general cognitive ability. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 11, 275286.Google Scholar
Williams, P. G., Suchy, Y. and Kraybill, M. L. (2013). Preliminary evidence for low openness to experience as a pre-clinical marker of incipient cognitive decline in older adults. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 945951.Google Scholar