Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-fv566 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T00:30:09.469Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive processes and collective representations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 July 2009

Get access

Extract

Anthropologists typically adopt the assumption of psychic unity: man universally exhibits the same fundamental cognitive processes. Hence, any apparent heterogeneity in collective representations may not be explained by psychological postulates. On the basis of recent cross-cultural studies of cognitive processes, we move beyond this standard anthropological assumption, arguing that there is cultural variability in the mode of thinking which is elaborated in specific conceptual domains. We shall also reject, however, the traditional alternatives to the assumption of psychic unity: the hypothesis that there is a pervasive mode of primitive thought or that in the course of history the intellect has marched through general stages of thinking.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Archives Européenes de Sociology 1979

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Beattie, J.H.M. (1970). On understanding ritual, in Wilson, B. R. (ed.), Rationality, pp. 240268 (Oxford, Basil Blackwell).Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language (Cambridge, Harvard University Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., and Herrnstein, R. (1975). Psychology (London, Methuen).Google Scholar
Brown, J. K. (1975). The archaic illusion: a re-examination of Lévi-Strauss' view of the developing child, in Williams, T. R. (ed.), Psychological Anthropology, pp. 103108 (The Hague, Mouton).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S., Greenfield, P. M. and Olver, R. (1966). Studies in Cognitive Growth (New York, John Wiley).Google Scholar
Bryant, P. E. (1974). Perception and Understanding in Young Children (New York, Basic Books).Google Scholar
Burnham, J. (1973). The Structure of Art (New York, Braziller).Google Scholar
Childers, P. and Wimmer, M. (1971). The concept of death in early childhood, Child Development, XLII, 12991301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, M. and Bruner, J.S. (1971). Cultural differences and inferences about psychological processes, American Psychologist, XXVI, 867876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J. and Sharp, D. (1971). The Cultural Context of Learning and Thinking (London, Methuen).Google Scholar
Cole, M. and Scribner, S. (1974). Culture and Thought (New York, Wiley).Google Scholar
Dasen, P.R. (1972). Cross-cultural Piagetan research: a summary, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, III, 2339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasen, P. R. (1977 a). Piagetian Psychology: cross-cultural contributions (New York, Gardner Press).Google Scholar
Dasen, P. R. (1977 b) Are cognitive processes universal? A Contribution to cross-cultural Piagetan psychology, in Warren, N. (ed.), Studies in Cross-cultural Psychology, pp. 155201 (London, Academic Press).Google Scholar
Davison, M.L., Robbins, S. and Swanson, D. B. (1978). Stage structure in objective moral judgements, Developmental Psychology, XIV, 137146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, M. (1970). Purity and Danger (Harmondsworth, Penguin).Google Scholar
Edwards, C. P. (1975). Society complexity and moral development: a Kenyan study, Ethos, III, 505527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E.E. (1956). Nuer Religion (Oxford, Clarendon Press).Google Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1965). Theories of Primitive Religion (Oxford, Clarendon Press).Google Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E.E. (1973). The Intellectualist (English) interpretation of magic, Journal of the Anthropological Society of Oxford, IV, n° 3, 123–42.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1977). Cognitive Development (New York, John Wiley).Google Scholar
Fortes, M. (1959). Oedipus and Job in West African Religion (Cambridge, University Press).Google Scholar
Gablik, S. (1976). Progress in Art (London, Thames and Hudson).Google Scholar
Gelman, R. (1972). The nature and development of early number concepts, in Reese, H. W. (ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behaviour, Vol. VII, pp. 116167 (New York, Academic Press).Google Scholar
Gelman, R. (1978). Cognitive development, in Rozenzweig, M. R. and Porter, L. W. (eds.), Annual Review of Psychology, vol. XXIX, pp. 297332 (Palo Alto, Annual Reviews Inc.).Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1966). The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems (Boston, Houghton and Mifflin).Google Scholar
Goodnow, J. J. (1962). A Test of milieu differences with some of Piaget's tasks, Psychological Monographs, LXXVI, No. 36, whole no. 555.Google Scholar
Goodenough, F. L. (1926). Measurement of Intelligence by Drawing (New York, World Book Company).Google Scholar
Gorsuch, R. and Barnes, M. (1973). Stages of ethical reasoning and moral norms of Carib youth, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, IV, 283301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and Phytogeny (London, Harvard University Press).Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. and Bruner, J.S. (1966). Culture and cognitive growth, International Journal of Psychology, I, 89107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallpike, C. (1976). Is there a primitive mentality? Man, XI, 253270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L. (1975). Search strategies and object permanence during infancy, Psychological Bulletin, LXXXII, 332344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L. (1978). Developmental aspects of children's memory, in Gruneberg, M. M. and Morris, P. E. (eds), Aspects of Memory, pp. 132152 (London, Methuen).Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (1979). Perception and cognition in infancy, in Connolly, K. (ed.), Psychology Survey II (London, Allen and Unwin).Google Scholar
Horton, R. (1961). Destiny and the unconscious in West Africa, Africa, II, 110–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horton, R. (1970). African traditional thought and Western science, in Wilson, B.R. (ed.), Rationality, pp. 131171 (Oxford, Basil Blackwell).Google Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1958). Child animism I, A critical survey of cross-cultural research, Journal of Social Psychology, IV, 197212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1970). A Psychologist's perspective, in Mayer, P. (ed.) Socialization: the approach from social anthropology, pp 3349 (London, Tavistock).Google Scholar
Kamara, A. and Eastley, J. Jr, (1977). Is the rate of cognitive development uniform across cultures? in Dasen, P. (ed.), Piagetian Psychology: cross-cultural contributions, pp. 2663 (New York, Gardner).Google Scholar
Kelly, M. (1977). Papua New Guinea and Piaget: an eight year study, Piagetian Psychology: cross-cultural contributions pp. 169202.Google Scholar
Kiminyo, D. (1977). A Cross-cultural study of the development of conservation of mass, weight and volume among Kamba children, Piagetian Psychology: cross-cultural contributions pp. 6488.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: the cognitive-developmental approach to socialization, in Goslin, D. (ed.), Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research, pp. 347480 (Chicago, Rand McNally & Co).Google Scholar
Kreutzer, M. A., Leonard, C. and Flavell, J. H. (1975). An interview study of children's knowledge about memory, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, XL (Serial no 159).Google Scholar
Kurtines, W. and Greif, E. B.. The development of moral thought: review and evaluation of Kohlberg's approach, Psychological Bulletin, LXXXI (1974), pp. 453–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laurendeau-Bendavid, M. (1977). Culture, schooling and cognitive development: a comparative study of children in French Canada and Rwanda, in Dasen, P., op. cit. pp. 123168.Google Scholar
Leach, E. (1969). Genesis as Myth (London, Jonathan Cape).Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1966). The Savage Mind (London, Weidenfeld and Nicolson).Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1970) (orig. 1949). The elementary structures of kinship (London, Tavistock).Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1971). Towards the problem of the historical nature of psychological processes, International Journal of Psychology, VI, 259–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modgil, S. and Modgil, C. (1976). Piagetian Research Compilation and Commentary, II (Windsor, NFER Pub. Com.)Google Scholar
Paredes, J. A. and Hepburn, M. J. (1976). The split brain and the culture and cognition paradox, Current Anthropology, XVII, 121127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peluffo, N. (1967). Culture and cognitive problems, International Journal of Psychology, II, 187188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child (New York, Basic Books).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1967). Review of ‘Studies in Cognitive Growth’, ap. Contemporary psychology, XII, 532533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price-Williams, D. R. (1961). A Study concerning concepts of conservation of quantities among primitive children, Acta Psychologica, XVIII, 293305.Google Scholar
Read, K. E. (1967). Morality and the concept of the person among the Gahuku-Gama, in Middleton, J. (ed.), Myth and Cosmos pp. 185229 (New York, The Natural History Press).Google Scholar
Rest, J. R., Davison, M. L. and Robbins, S. (1978). Age trends in judging moral issues: a review of cross-sectional, longitudinal and sequential studies of the Defining Issues test, Child Development, XLIX, 263279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scribner, S. (1977). Modes of thinking and ways of speaking: culture and logic reconsidered, in Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Wason, P. C. (eds.), Thinking: Readings in Cognitive Science, pp. 483500 (Cambridge University Press).Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1977). Likeness and likelihood in everyday thought: magical thinking in judgements about personality, Current Anthropology, XVIII, 637658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, M. (1967). A Short History of Chinese Art (London, Faber and Faber).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tambiah, S. J. (1968). The magical power of words, Man, III, 175208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tenhouten, W. (1976). More on splitbrain research, culture and cognition, Current Anthropology, XVII, 503506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E., Edwards, C. P. and Kohlerg, L. (1978). Moral development in Turkish children, adolescents and young adults, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, IX, 7586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, C. B., Bushnelland, N., Regnemer, J. L., Moral development in Bahamian school children: a 3-year examination of Kohlberg's stages of moral development, Developmental Psychology, XIV, 5865.Google Scholar
White, E., Elsom, B. and Prawar, R. (1978). Children's conceptions of death, Child Development, XLIX, 307310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willats, J. (1977). How children learn to draw realistic pictures, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, XXIX, 367382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, R. (1977). Is there a primitive mentality? Correspondence, Man, XII, 530.Google Scholar
Wohlwill, J. (1973). The study of behavioral development (New York, Academic Press).Google Scholar
Worsley, (1967). Groote Eylandt totemism and le totémisme aujourd'hui, in Leach, E. (ed), The structural study of myth and totemism, pp. 141159 (London, Tavistock).Google Scholar