Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T02:02:39.468Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reading the Minds of Others: Radical Interpretation and the Empirical Study of Childhood Cognitive Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 April 2010

Manuela Ungureanu
Affiliation:
Okanagan University College, Kelowna

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Philosophical Association 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andrews, Kristin 2002Interpreting Autism: A Critique of Davidson on Thought and Language.” Philosophical Psychology, 15, 3: 317–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, Janet W. 1996 “What Is Theoretical about a Child's Theory of Mind? A Vygoskian View of Its Development.” In Theories of Theories of Mind. Edited by Carruthers, Peter and Smith, Peter K.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 184–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, Janet W. 2001The Future of Theory-of-Mind Research: Understanding Motivational States, the Role of Language, and Real-World Consequences.” Child Development, 72, 3: 685–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Astington, Janet W., and Jenkins, Jennifer M. 1999A Longitudinal Study of the Relation between Language and Theory-of-Mind Development.” Developmental Psychology, 35, 5: 1311–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berk, Laura E. 1994Why Children Talk to Themselves.” Scientific American, 11: 7883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, Paul 2000 How Children Learn the Meaning of Words. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, Paul 2004 “Myths of Word Learning.” In Weaving a Lexicon. Edited by Hall, D. Geoffrey and Waxman, Sandra R.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, Paul, and German, Tim 2000Two Reasons to Abandon the False Belief Task as a Test of Theory of Mind.” Cognition, 11: B25–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, Paul, and Markson, Lori 2001Are There Principles that Apply Only to the Acquisition of Words?Cognition, 78: 8990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandom, Robert 1995Knowledge and the Social Articulation of the Space of Reasons.” Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 40, 4: 895908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1975 Reflections on Language. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1992 “Language and Interpretation: Philosophical Reflections and Empirical Inquiry.” In Inference, Explanation and Other Frustrations: Essays in the Philosophy of Science. Edited by Earman, John. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, pp. 99128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1965 “Theories of Meaning and Learnable Languages.” In The Proceedings of the 1964 International Congress of Logic, Methodology and Philosophy of Science. Edited by Bar-Hillel, Y.. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company. (Reprinted in his Inquiries into Truth and interpretation [Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1984], pp. 3–16.)Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1971 “Agency.” In Agent, Action and Reason. Edited by Binkley, R., Bronaugh, R., and Marras, A.. Toronto: University of Toronto Press, pp. 325. (Reprinted in his Essays on Actions and Events [Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1980], pp. 43–61.)Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1973a “Radical Interpretation.” Dialectica, 27: 314328. (Reprinted in his Inquiries into Truth and Interpretation [Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1984], pp. 125–40.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1973b “The Material Mind.” In The Proceedings of the Fourth International Congress of Logic, Methodology and Philosophy of Science Section 9: Methodology and Philosophy of Psychological Sciences. Edited by Suppes, P., Henkin, L., Moisil, G. C., and Joja, A.. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company. (Reprinted in his Essays on Actions and Events [Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1980], pp. 245–60.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1974Belief and the Basis of Meaning.” Synthese, 27 : 309–23. (Reprinted in his Inquiries into Truth and Interpretation [Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1984], pp. 141–54.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1975 “Thought and Talk.” In Mind and Language. Edited by Guttenplan, S.. Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Reprinted in his Inquiries into Truth and Interpretation [Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1984), pp. 155–70.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1980 Essays on Actions and Events. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1984 Inquiries into Truth and Interpretation. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1985 “Reply to Suppes.” In Essays on Davidson: Actions and Events. Edited by Vermazen, Bruce and Hintikka, Merril B.. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 247–52.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1990 “Afterthoughts.” In Reading Rorty. Edited by Malachowski, Alan R.. New York: Blackwell, pp. 134–38.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1992 “The Second Person.” Midwest Studies in Philosophy. Vol. 17: The Wittgenstein Legacy. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press, pp. 255–67.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1994a “The Social Aspect of Language.” In The Philosophy of Michael Dummett. Edited by McGuiness, Brian F. and Olivieri, Gianluigi. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 116.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1994b “Radical Interpretation Interpreted.” In Philosophical Perspectives. Vol. 8: Logic and Language. Edited by Tomberlin, James. Atascadero, CA: Ridgeview, pp. 121–28.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1994c “Interpreting Radical Interpretation.” In Reflecting Davidson: Donald Davidson Responding to an International Forum of Philosophers. Edited by Stoecker, Ralph. Berlin: de Gruyter, pp. 7784.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1995Could There Be a Science of Rationality?International Journal for Philosophical Studies, 3: 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1997Seeing through Language.” Philosophy, 42: 1527. (Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement: Thought and Language. Edited by John Preston. [Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.])Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald 1999The Emergence of Thought.” Erkenntnis, 51: 717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diesendruck, Gil, and Markson, Lori 2001Children's Avoidance of Lexical Overlap: A Pragmatic Account.” Developmental Psychology, 37, 5: 630–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, Judy 1991 “Understanding Others: Evidence from Naturalistic Studies of Children.” In Natural Theories of Mind: Evolution, Development and Simulation of Everyday Mind-Reading. Edited by Whiten, Andrew. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 233–35.Google Scholar
Engel, Pascal 1988Radical Interpretation.” Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 88: 161–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evnine, Simon 1991 Donald Davidson. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry 1983 The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Jerry, and LePore, Ernest 1994 “Is Radical Interpretation Possible?” In Philosophical Perspectives. Vol. 8: Logic and Language. Edited by Tomberlin, J.. Atascadero, CA: Ridgeview, pp. 101–20.Google Scholar
Friedman, Michael 2001 Dynamics of Reason. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Frith, Uta, and Happé, Francisca 1994Autism: Beyond Theory of Mind.” Cognition, 50: 115–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garfield, Jay L., Peterson, Candida C., and Perry, Tricia 2001Social Cognition, Language Acquistion and the Development of the Theory of Mind.” Mind and Language, 16, 5: 491541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, Lila, Gleitman, Henry, and Shipley, Elizabeth 1972The Emergence of the Child as Grammarian.” Cognition, 1, 1: 137–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, Lila, and Newport, Elissa L. 1995 “The Invention of Language by Children: Environmental and Biological Influences on the Acquisition of Language.” In An Invitation to Cognitive Science. Vol. I: Language. Edited by Gleitman, Lila and Liberman, Mark. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, Lila, and Bloom, Paul 1999 “Language Acquisition.” In MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Edited by Wilson, Robert and Keil, Frank. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 434–38.Google Scholar
Gopnik, Alison 1996 “Theories and Modules: Creation Myths, Developmental Realities and Neurath's Boat.” In Theories of Theories of Mind. Edited by Carruthers, Peter and Smith, Peter K.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 169–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, Alison, and Meltzoff, Andrew N. 1998Theories vs. Modules: To the Max and Beyond—Reply to Poulin-Dubois and to Stich and Nichols.” Mind and Language, 13: 450–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, Alison, and Astington, Janet W. 2000 “Children's Understanding of Representational Change and Its Relation to the Understanding of False Belief and the Appearance-Reality Distinction.” In Childhood Cognitive Development: The Essential Readings. Edited by Kang, Lee. Maiden, MA: Blackwell, pp. 177–99.Google Scholar
Haslanger, Sally 1999 “What Knowledge Is and What It Ought to Be: Feminist Values and Normative Epistemology.” In Philosophical Perspectives. Vol. 13: Epistemology. Edited by Tomberlin, J.. Atascadero, CA: Ridgeview, pp. 459–80.Google Scholar
Higginbotham, James 1994Priorities in the Philosophy of Thought.” Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, Suppl. vol. 68: 85106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, Richard, and Segal, Gabriel 1995 Knowledge of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LePore, Ernest 1982In Defense of Davidson.” Linguistics and Philosophy, 5: 277–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LePore, Ernest, and Loewer, Barry 1990 “What Davidson Should Have Said.” In Information, Semantics and Epistemology. Edited by Villanueva, Enrique. New York: Blackwell, pp. 190–99.Google Scholar
Leslie, Alan, and Roth, Daniel 1993 “What Autism Teaches Us about Metarepresentation.” In Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism. Edited by Baron-Cohen, Simon, Tager-Flusberg, Helen, and Cohen, Donald. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 83111.Google Scholar
Lewis, David 1983 “Radical Interpretation.” In his Philosophical Papers. Vol. 1. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 108–18.Google Scholar
Malpas, Jeff 1998 “Donald Davidson.” In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Edited by Edward N. Zalta. See: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/davidson/.Google Scholar
McGeer, Victoria 2001Psycho-Practice, Psycho-Theory and the Contrastive Case of Autism.” Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8: 109–32.Google Scholar
Meins, Elizabeth, Fernyhough, Charles, Wainwright, Rachel, Gupta, Mani Das, Fradley, Emma, and Tuckey, Michelle 2002Maternal Mind-Mindedness and Attachment Security as Predictors of Theory of Mind Understanding.” Child Development, 73: 1715–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, Katherine, Plesa, Daniela, and Henseler, Sarah 1998Children's Theory of Mind: An Experiential Interpretation.” Human Development, 41: 729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, David R. 1993The Development of Representations: The Origins of Mental Life.” Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne, 34, 3: 293306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, Daniela K. 1996Two-Year-Old Children's Sensitivity to a Parent's Knowledge State When Making Requests.” Child Development, 67: 659–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PernerJosef, Ted Ruffman, and Susan R. Leekam Josef, Ted Ruffman, and Susan R. Leekam 1994Theory of Mind Is Contagious: You Catch It from Your Sibs.” Child Development, 65: 1228–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PetersonCandida C, and Michael Siegal Candida C, and Michael Siegal 1995Deafness, Conversation and Theory of Mind.” Journal of Child Psychology, 36: 459–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pietroski, Paul, and Crain, Stephen 2001Nature, Nurture and Universal Grammar.” Linguistics and Philosophy, 24: 139–86.Google Scholar
Premack, David, and Woodruff, Guy 1978Does the Chimpanzee Have a Theory of Mind?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 4: 515–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, Willard Van Orman 1960 Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Robinson, Elizabeth 1994 “What People Say, What They Think and What Is Really the Case: Children's Understanding of Utterances as Sources of Knowledge.” In Children's Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development. Edited by Lewis, C. and Mitchell, P.. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, pp. 355–81.Google Scholar
Ruffman, Ted, Slade, Lance, and Crowe, Elena 2002The Relation between Children's and Mothers' Mental State Language and Theory-of-Mind Understanding.” Child Development, 73: 734–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuels, Richard 2002Nativism in Cognitive Science.” Mind and Language, 17: 233–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwitzgebel, Eric 1997 “Difficulties in Davidson's Arguments against Belief without Language.” Unpublished manuscript. See: http://www.facultv.ucr.edu/ ∼ eschwitz.Google Scholar
Segal, Gabriel 1994Priorities in the Philosophy of Thought.” Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, Suppl. vol. 68: 85106.Google Scholar
Segal, Gabriel 1996 “The Modularity of Theory of Mind.” In Theories of Theories of Mind. Edited by Carruthers, Peter and Smith, Peter K.. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 141–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segal, Gabriel 1998 “Representing Representations.” In Language and Thought: Interdisciplinary Themes. Edited by Carruthers, Peter and Boucher, Jill. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 146–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stich, Stephen 1978Belief and Sub-Doxastic States.” Philosophy of Science, 45: 499518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinden, Penelope 1996Junin Quechua Children's Understanding of Mind.” Child Development, 67: 1707–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, Henry, and Perner, Josef 1983Beliefs about Beliefs: Representation and Constraining of Wrong Beliefs in Young Children's Understanding of Deception.” Cognition, 13: 103–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed