Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-cjp7w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-26T02:21:09.997Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive differences among first-year and senior engineering students when generating design solutions with and without additional dimensions of sustainability

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2021

Mo Hu*
Affiliation:
Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, USA
Tripp Shealy
Affiliation:
Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, USA
Julie Milovanovic
Affiliation:
UMR AAU-CRENAU, Department of Architecture, Graduate School of Architecture Nantes, Nantes, France
*
Corresponding author Mo Hu moh6@vt.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The research presented in this paper explores how engineering students cognitively manage concept generation and measures the effects of additional dimensions of sustainability on design cognition. Twelve first-year and eight senior engineering students generated solutions to 10 design problems. Half of the problems included additional dimensions of sustainability. The number of unique design solutions students developed and their neurocognitive activation were measured. Without additional requirements for sustainability, first-year students generated significantly more solutions than senior engineering students. First-year students recruited higher cortical activation in the brain region generally associated with cognitive flexibility, and divergent and convergent thinking. Senior engineering students recruited higher activation in the brain region generally associated with uncertainty processing and self-reflection. When additional dimensions of sustainability were present, first-year students produced fewer solutions. Senior engineering students generated a similar number of solutions. Senior engineering students required less cortical activation to generate a similar number of solutions. The varying patterns of cortical activation and different number of solutions between first-year and senior engineering students begin to highlight cognitive differences in how students manage and retrieve information in their brain during design. Students’ ability to manage complex requirements like sustainability may improve with education.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Engineering design is a goal-oriented process to solve complex problems that involve technical, economic, social and environmental dimensions (Pahl et al. Reference Pahl, Beitz, Feldhusen and Grote2007). Engineering designers use a range of techniques to develop solutions to complex problems. For example, framing the design problem in multiple ways (Wright et al. Reference Wright, Rutgers, Daly, Jablokow and Yilmaz2015), applying structured design principles to generate new ideas (Cross & Cross Reference Cross and Cross1998), allowing time for reflection (Schön Reference Schön1983) and engaging with stakeholders (Bucciarelli Reference Bucciarelli1988). No matter the technique or process, some form of idea generation or concept generation is involved (Smith Reference Smith1998; Goldenberg et al. Reference Goldenberg, Mazursky and Solomon1999; Jonson Reference Jonson2005; Knoll & Horton Reference Knoll and Horton2010). Idea generation is arguably the most critical phase of design because future solutions are dictated by the ideas generated during this phase.

Idea generation also offers an opportunity to consider additional dimensions of sustainability (Ashford Reference Ashford2004; Morris et al. Reference Morris, Childs and Hamilton2007; Collado-Ruiz & Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi Reference Collado-Ruiz and Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi2010; Cucuzzella Reference Cucuzzella2016). Dimensions of sustainability refer to an integration of economic performance, social inclusiveness and environmental resilience in a balanced manner for the benefits of current and future generations (Chitchyan et al. Reference Chitchyan, Becker, Betz, Duboc, Penzenstadler, Seyff and Venters2016; Pieroni et al. Reference Pieroni, McAloone and Pigosso2019). Integrating sustainability into engineering design and adopting sustainable methods, such as passive design, biomimetic design and circular economy, is crucial considering the economic, social and environmental crisis (i.e., climate change, resources constraints and economic inequality) (Curran Reference Curran2009; Pereira Reference Pereira2009). Design for sustainability sets higher requirements for engineers to satisfy (Collado-Ruiz & Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi Reference Collado-Ruiz and Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi2010; Ceschin Reference Ceschin2014; Maccioni et al. Reference Maccioni, Borgianni, Rotini, Campana, Howlett, Setchi and Cimatti2017) and might reduce the problem and solution space during engineering design (Hay et al. Reference Hay, Duffy, McTeague, Pidgeon, Vuletic and Grealy2017a).

Engineers must not only meet economic constraints, but also find solutions with higher mutual environmental and societal benefits. Knowing when and how to integrate additional dimensions of sustainability into the engineering design process can be a challenge (Collado-Ruiz & Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi Reference Collado-Ruiz and Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi2010; Ceschin Reference Ceschin2014). For instance, providing engineers with environmental information too early, or having them reflect on these dimensions too soon, can hinder their creative ability due to fixation (Collado-Ruiz & Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi Reference Collado-Ruiz and Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi2010). Finding methods that promote sustainability as a driver for creative solutions is essential (Klotz et al. Reference Klotz, Pickering, Schmidt and Weber2019).

Developing engineers who can create design solutions to meet sustainability challenges is increasingly part of engineering education (Dym et al. Reference Dym, Agogino, Eris, Frey and Leifer2005; Allenby Reference Allenby2011). Teaching design for sustainability requires students to push past traditional problem-solving approaches (Mills & Treagust Reference Mills and Treagust2003; Dym et al. Reference Dym, Agogino, Eris, Frey and Leifer2005; Daly et al. Reference Daly, Christian, Yilmaz, Seifert and Gonzalez2012, Reference Daly, Mosyjowski and Seifert2014). Students must use new search processes that include empathy (Blizzard Reference Blizzard2013), systems thinking (Cardenas et al. Reference Cardenas, Sosa, Moysen and Martinez2010) and draw on requisite knowledge from the physical and social sciences (Klotz et al. Reference Klotz, Weber, Johnson, Shealy, Hernandez and Gordon2018).

Engineering design is a complex cognitive process that relies on a set of explicit (knowing that) and tactical skills (knowing how) developed through experience (Schön Reference Schön, Porter, Kilbridge and Andrew1981). Engineering design is not an assembly of a solution from existing components, but instead a search process for appropriate solutions using all cognitive means necessary to gain a new perspective on the problem at hand (Howard et al. Reference Howard, Culley and Dekoninck2008). Including more dimensions about sustainability inevitably makes this cognitive process more challenging because it requires cognitively managing multiple dimensions during design (Hu et al. Reference Hu, Shealy, Grohs and Panneton2019).

To help refine engineering education so students can efficiently create solutions that enhance outcomes for sustainability will require a better understanding of the cognitive processes involved in design (Cross Reference Cross2001) and design for sustainability (Hu & Shealy Reference Shealy, Hu and Gero2018). Investigating cognitive differences of engineers’ behaviour between novice and trained engineers can also provide new insight for design education (Newstetter & McCracken Reference Newstetter and Michael McCracken2001; Cross Reference Cross2004). Trained engineers tend to use more structured approaches to design. This can limit the number of cognitive processes that designers use as a control or inhibitor to cognitive activation (Kavakli & Gero Reference Kavakli, Gero and Lindemann2003).

Methods for measuring design cognition for sustainability are generally limited to protocol analysis (Ericsson & Simon Reference Ericsson and Simon1984; Van Someren et al. Reference Van Someren, Barnard and Sandberg1994; Gero & Mc Neill Reference Gero and Mc Neill1998), ethnography, posttask surveys and observation (Coley et al. Reference Coley, Houseman and Roy2007). These methods require extensive resources to obtain reliable results. Recent advances in neuroscience now enable more direct measure of design cognition for sustainability to supplement behavioural analysis (Goucher-Lambert & Cagan Reference Goucher-Lambert and Cagan2015; Seitamaa-Hakkarainen et al. Reference Seitamaa-Hakkarainen, Huotilainen, Mäkelä, Groth and Hakkarainen2016; Goucher-Lambert et al. Reference Goucher-Lambert, Moss and Cagan2017; Gero & Milovanovic Reference Gero and Milovanovic2020).

In this paper, a combined approach is used to explore students’ performance to address engineering design problems and how students cognitively manage additional dimensions of sustainability. Students’ neurocognitive activation was measured while generating solutions to brainstorming tasks with or without additional dimensions of sustainability. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) was used to measure neurocognition. The objective of the research was to identify underlying neurocognitive processes in first-year and senior engineering students when brainstorming and measure the effect of including additional dimensions of sustainability in the design problem. Better understanding the neurocognitive activation associated with this type of cognitive design process is the first step to provide a framework to train engineering students to leverage sustainability, rather than let it inhibit, more creative solutions (Klotz et al. Reference Klotz, Pickering, Schmidt and Weber2019).

The paper begins with an overview of different methods to help designers generate ideas. This is followed by an explanation about why sustainability challenges in engineering are important prerequisites for engineering design. The challenge to integrate design, creativity and sustainability into the engineering curriculum is also raised. The background section is followed by the research questions, which explore the neurocognitive activation patterns during concept generation and the effects on neurocognition when additional dimensions of sustainability are included in the design problem. The methodology combines measures of neurophysiology and behaviour to understand differences in idea generation between first-year and senior engineering students, with and without additional dimensions of sustainability. The results include independent t-tests and paired t-tests performed on behavioural and neurocognitive data to explore differences between the two participant groups (first-year and senior engineering undergraduate students) for two types of tasks (concept generation and concept generation when design problems contain additional dimensions of sustainability).

2. Background

2.1. Design ideation in engineering

Designing is a reflective process (Schön Reference Schön1983), based on an iteration of problem formulation, idea generation and evaluation of the proposed concepts to formalize design solutions. It relies on a set of cognitive processes that promote an alternation of divergent and convergent thinking (Goldschmidt Reference Goldschmidt2016), and of problem and solution space exploration (Maher & Poon Reference Maher and Poon1996; Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001). Idea generation aims to address a given design problem by the proposal of multiple ideas. Divergent thinking serves to explore the solution space by formulating multiple design possibilities. As design problems are considered ill-structured (Simon Reference Simon1973), the proposal and evaluation of multiple concepts is a way to reframe and structure the problem space and begin the design process again.

Concept generation is a crucial part of the design process and supports designers’ reflective conversation with the concepts being developed (Schön Reference Schön1992). The underlying objective of promoting concept generation is to support creativity and innovation in the design solutions generated. Many tools are available to encourage concept generation (Goldenberg et al. Reference Goldenberg, Mazursky and Solomon1999; Jonson Reference Jonson2005; Knoll & Horton Reference Knoll and Horton2010). Each technique relies on diverse strategies (Smith Reference Smith1998). For instance, morphological analysis depends on an analytical strategy to decompose the problem and systematically associate a partial solution to subproblems to stimulate unconscious thought (Allen Reference Allen1962). Brainstorming is another approach. It builds upon suspending judgement and criticisms during ideation to increase the flow of ideas (Osborn Reference Osborn1993). The generation of multiple solutions for later evaluation increases the chance for better design and serves to avoid fixation on an initial solution (Ball et al. Reference Ball, Maskill and Ormerod1998). Indeed, studies support that the number of ideas generated correlate positively with the creativeness of those ideas (Osborn Reference Osborn1993; Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Kohn and Arditti2011).

Learning how to design and be creative is part of the engineering education process. Students rely on these skills to address real world engineering problems. A common instructional approach to teaching idea generation in engineering courses is to challenge students with open-ended problems without defined target products. The openness of problems can entice the flow of ideas among students (Daly et al. Reference Daly, Mosyjowski and Seifert2014). Idea generation tools such as brainstorming and concept generation can be integrated into engineering courses to improve the type of solutions students develop (going beyond basic principles) and promote creativity (Daly et al. Reference Daly, Christian, Yilmaz, Seifert and Gonzalez2012).

2.2. Importance of integrating sustainability into engineering design

The proposal of ideas is a way to explore the problem space by reframing it through the evaluation of the concepts generated. Engineering design problems are complex as they aim to resolve both technical, social and economic parameters. Environmental factors should not be disregarded (Goucher-Lambert & Cagan Reference Goucher-Lambert and Cagan2015). Issues such as climate change, resource constraints and rising poverty highlight the need to pursue more sustainable solutions that meet current needs without compromising the ability of future generations to do the same (Brundtland Reference Brundtland1987). Government organizations (EPA 2007), professional societies (ASEE 1999), national academies (NAE 2008) and foundations (National Science Foundation 2009) recognize these needs and call for more participation among engineers.

To explore the impact of considering sustainability in the design of transportation, consider an engineer tasked with alleviating congestion on a highway. Guided by conventional design theory and processes, an engineer may consider the level of service on the existing roadway and decide to add more road lanes to reduce congestion. However, such a design solution could be counterproductive, or further exacerbate the initial problem by induced demand (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Klein and Camus1999; Hymel et al. Reference Hymel, Small and Dender2010). Adding a new road lane will bring new automobile drivers, and over time, typically within 5 years, it leads to an increase in traffic (Noland Reference Noland2001; Cervero Reference Cervero2003).

Adding design constraints for sustainability can entice creative solution generation (Litman & Laube Reference Litman and Laube2002; Fini et al. Reference Fini, Awadallah, Parast and Abu-Lebdeh2018). For example, the development of a subway system was not economically feasible in Curitiba, Brazil and an investment to expand their highway system was not socially acceptable given only 10 per cent of their population owned vehicles. These economic and social constraints encouraged engineers and planners to propose a radically different idea: to repurpose existing road lanes into bus rapid transit lanes. At the time, removing existing road lanes to reduce traffic seemed counterintuitive but it helped alleviate traffic, and today residents enjoy the lowest per capita transportation costs and best air quality in the country (Lindau et al. Reference Lindau, Hidalgo and Facchini2010).

Sustainable design solutions, like the example of Curitiba, are more likely to emerge from a design approach that includes multiple dimensions of sustainability. The Mayor of Curitiba explained ‘…creativity starts when you cut a zero from your budget. If you can cut two zeroes, it’s much better’ (Lerner Reference Lerner2007). In other words, the presence of economic constraints led to a more sustainable outcome.

Engineers need technical and analytical skills to undertake engineering design but they also need to consider how their design affects broad dimensions of sustainability such as local development, health and downstream pollution (Azapagic et al. Reference Azapagic, Perdan and Shallcross2005; Staniškis & Katiliūtė Reference Staniškis and Katiliūtė2016). Engineers should learn how to take advantage of methods such as passive design (Bhikhoo et al. Reference Bhikhoo, Hashemi and Cruickshank2017), biomimetic design (Linke & Moreno Reference Linke and Moreno2015) or circular economy (Pieroni et al. Reference Pieroni, McAloone and Pigosso2019) to solve engineering problems. Sustainability requirements are as important as technical ones, and should be integrated early because introducing them later into the design process creates less opportunity for change (Gervásio et al. Reference Gervásio, Santos, Martins and Simões da Silva2014), and often results in ‘greenwashing’ (Kapalko Reference Kapalko2010). Creative design and innovation are necessary to go beyond social and cultural boundaries (Cucuzzella Reference Cucuzzella2016). Cucuzzella (Reference Cucuzzella2016) identified four levels of design for sustainable innovations: product improvement, product redesign, function innovation and system innovation. The later tackles sustainable issues at a global scale, challenging existing social and cultural assumptions to exceed beyond a mere artifact redesign.

2.3. Educating engineering students about sustainable design

Teaching sustainability calls for a transdisciplinary approach to foster system innovation (Ashford Reference Ashford2004). Active learning is best suited to teach transdisciplinary skills and systems thinking needed to tackle sustainable development projects (Mulder et al. Reference Mulder, Segalàs, Ferrer‐Balas and Svanström2012). A lack of consistent integration of active learning and focus on sustainability in courses across engineering programs still persist, even though recent progress has been made in that direction (Siller Reference Siller2001; Azapagic Reference Azapagic2005; Chau Reference Chau2007; Huntzinger et al. Reference Huntzinger, Hutchins, Gierke and Sutherland2007). The process of integrating sustainability in engineering curriculums is still an active area of research (Lozano Reference Lozano2010; Bielefeldt Reference Bielefeldt2013).

Students learning to design for sustainability are encouraged to develop complex cognitive skills to resolve both abstract and applied problems (Salganik & Rychen Reference Salganik and Rychen2003; Barth et al. Reference Barth, Godemann, Rieckmann, Stoltenberg, Adomssent, Godemann and Michelsen2007; Lambrechts et al. Reference Lambrechts, Mulà, Ceulemans, Molderez and Gaeremynck2013). For example, sustainable thinking is a key element into a paradigmatic shift from designing things right to designing the right things, meaning that it aligns with current needs without compromising future generations (Cardenas et al. Reference Cardenas, Sosa, Moysen and Martinez2010). Sustainable design has the potential to integrate both paradigms to design the right things right.

Creative brainstorming is a necessary tool in education to help students frame complex and intertwined concepts and find ideas during design ideation (Kajzer Mitchell & Walinga Reference Kajzer Mitchell and Walinga2017). Requiring students to practice brainstorming concepts that also meet sustainability requirements is effective in teaching and learning about sustainable design. It can enhance higher-order thinking skills (Fini et al. Reference Fini, Awadallah, Parast and Abu-Lebdeh2018). Adding sustainability as a design requirement in the educational context is also a driver for the development of students’ creativity (Bremer et al. Reference Bremer, Gonzalez and Mercado2010). Creativity is fostered through design for sustainability, as the aim of this type of design is not only to provide a solution to the design problem but to question the problem itself (Morris et al. Reference Morris, Childs and Hamilton2007).

While engineering education is meant to help students cognitively deal with the complexity of sustainability through training and the use of more structured approaches for design, education attainment and prior knowledge can result in design fixation (Genco et al. Reference Genco, Hölttä‐Otto and Seepersad2012). Designers with expertise are more likely to attach new design concepts to their initial and early solution concepts and fail to consider more radical alternatives (Cross Reference Cross2011; Genco et al. Reference Genco, Hölttä‐Otto and Seepersad2012). To address complex and interdisciplinary sustainability problems, engineers need to generate a wide range of alternative solutions, sometimes outside of industry norms (Cross Reference Cross2004). Education provides both positive and potentially negative effects through fixation and reliance on learned industry norms during design.

2.4. Measuring design neurocognition to reflect on design education in engineering

Including sustainability into the design processes adds more requirements to an already cognitively complex process. The effect of adding sustainable constraints when generating design solutions is not well understood. Including sustainable requirements makes engineering design problems more complex and brings into question ways to teach engineering design.

Prior design studies usually measure design cognition (i.e., high-order thinking or creative thinking) through self-report or protocol analysis (Ericsson & Simon Reference Ericsson and Simon1984). Both self-report and protocol analysis are limited in their ability to explain how the designer cognitively processes and manages dimensions of sustainability. Advances in cognitive neuroscience offer an opportunity for design researchers to explore cognitive activation in the human brain during design. Methods from cognitive neuroscience provide an objective measurement representing changes in design cognition (Borgianni & Maccioni Reference Borgianni and Maccioni2020; Gero & Milovanovic Reference Gero and Milovanovic2020).

2.4.1. Role of the prefrontal cortex in design cognition

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is a critical brain region for problem solving and decision-making (Siddiqui et al. Reference Siddiqui, Chatterjee, Kumar, Siddiqui and Goyal2008). The cognitive functions of the PFC include executive control functions, attention, working memory, planning and inhibition. These functions play important roles during design ideation (Gibson et al. Reference Gibson, Folley and Park2009; Goel Reference Goel2014). Prior research highlights how the PFC in the right and left hemispheres perform distinct functions in concept generation. The right PFC is more active in divergent thinking and sustained attention (Cabeza & Nyberg Reference Cabeza and Nyberg2000; Moore et al. Reference Moore, Bhadelia, Billings, Fulwiler, Heilman, Rood and Gansler2009). The left PFC shows more activation in goal-directed planning and making analytic judgments (Aziz-Zadeh et al. Reference Aziz-Zadeh, Liew and Dandekar2013; Luft et al. Reference Luft, Zioga, Banissy and Bhattacharya2017).

Subregions in the PFC are also involved in varying functions necessary for concept generation. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), in both hemispheres, are active during creative thinking and making new associations (Funahashi Reference Funahashi2017). Creative tasks tend to activate the right dlPFC more strongly (Shealy et al. Reference Shealy, Hu and Gero2018). Literature about the right dlPFC is also known to contribute to exploration (i.e., without rules) and improvization; for example, electric stimulus to the right dlPFC increases performance among novice jazz musicians (Rosen et al. Reference Rosen, Erickson, Kim, Mirman, Hamilton and Kounios2016). The right dlPFC is known to be a critical area for creative design tasks (Hay et al. Reference Hay, Duffy, Gilbert, Lyall, Campbell, Coyle and Grealy2019). Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) is associated with self-reflection (left vlPFC) and uncertainty processing (right vlPFC) (Levy & Wagner Reference Levy and Wagner2011; Herwig et al. Reference Herwig, Kaffenberger, Schell, Jäncke and Brühl2012). The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is involved in memory retrieval and design empathy (Seitz et al. Reference Seitz, Nickel and Azari2006; Euston et al. Reference Euston, Gruber and McNaughton2012).

Among engineering students, an intervention involving concept mapping reduced cognitive activation in the dlPFC but lead to more concepts generated among students (Hu et al. Reference Hu, Shealy, Grohs and Panneton2019). More structured concept generation techniques, such as TRIZ can also decrease cognitive load broadly across the dlPFC compared to brainstorming (Shealy et al. Reference Shealy, Hu and Gero2018). Brainstorming usually demands bilateral coordination between the left and right PFC (Shealy & Gero Reference Shealy and Gero2019).

2.4.2. Effect of design education and expertise on brain activation

A direction of prior design cognition research explores how interventions, expertise, training and education change patterns of brain activation (Berkowitz & Ansari Reference Berkowitz and Ansari2010; Huang et al. Reference Huang, Qiu, Shen, Zhang, Song, Qi, Gong and Xie2013; Benedek et al. Reference Benedek, Jauk, Fink, Koschutnig, Reishofer, Ebner and Neubauer2014; Yang Reference Yang2015; Borgianni & Maccioni Reference Borgianni and Maccioni2020). Expert designers elicit significantly higher activation in their PFC compared to novices during creative conceptual imagination tasks (Liang et al. Reference Liang, Chang and Liu2019). Experts produce higher creative quality products than novice designers and elicit higher activation in their right hemisphere (Sun et al. Reference Sun, Xiang, Chai, Wang and Liu2013). Sun et al. (Reference Sun, Xiang, Chai, Wang and Liu2013) also found that the use of text as an intervention when sketching is effective to increase the activation in the right hemisphere associated with diverse analytical thinking. Although neurocognition among designers is not that straightforward. Skilled musicians show deactivation or reduced activation compared to novice or nonmusicians during music improvization (Berkowitz & Ansari Reference Berkowitz and Ansari2010) and music matching tasks (Petrini et al. Reference Petrini, Pollick, Dahl, McAleer, McKay, Rocchesso, Haakon Waadeland, Love, Avanzini and Puce2011). Hu and Reid (Reference Hu and Reid2018) also found that contextual experience (i.e., prior experience with specific design context) has a negative effect on designers’ mental state associated with creativity and also limited the novelty of their proposed solutions.

3. Research questions

The complexity of engineering design cognition and the additional dimensions of sustainability during design motivated the study presented in this paper. Engineering education should help students manage these complexities (Collado-Ruiz & Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi Reference Collado-Ruiz and Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi2010; Hu et al. Reference Hu, Shealy, Grohs and Panneton2019). In particular, during the concept generation phase to ensure the right types of solutions are being put forward. The purpose of this study was to explore how first-year and senior engineering students develop design solutions and cognitively manage aspects of sustainability. The research questions are:

  1. (i) How does the year of educational training in engineering influence students’ ability to generate solutions?

  2. (ii) How do additional dimensions of sustainability influence students’ ability to generate solutions?

3.1. Hypotheses

The number of unique solutions generated (the behavioural component) and the cortical activation in the PFC (the neurocognitive component) were the two factors measured. Engineering students are trained and practice concept generation during their undergraduate education, so the first hypothesis is that senior undergraduate engineering students generate more solutions and generate solutions with less cognitive effort (measured from changes in oxygenated blood in the PFC). The expectation is concept generation will require a greater diversity of active brain regions as well as greater intensity of activity (i.e., greater amplitude of oxygenated blood in the PFC) among first-year students compared to senior students.

The senior engineering students who participated in the study were trained in sustainability through their coursework. The second hypothesis is senior undergraduate engineering students manage the additional dimensions of sustainability with more cognitive ease. In other words, they generate more ideas to address sustainable design problems than first-year engineering students with less cognitive effort. When dimensions of sustainability are included, the hypothesis is higher cognitive effort in the dlPFC is recruited from both groups but less among senior engineering students compared to first-year engineering students.

4. Methods

4.1. Experimental design

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Virginia Tech. Engineering undergraduate students (n = 23, 12 first-year students and 11 seniors) participated in the study. All senior engineering students reported they have taken courses related to sustainability. Students either took a course titled Sustainable Systems or Sustainable Infrastructure. None of the students took both courses.

Students were given 10 engineering design problems based on Richard Smalley’s list of the most pressing issues facing humanity in the next 50 years (Smalley Reference Smalley2003). Any list of design problems like this is inherently limited, but this list was previously used for similar tasks in similar studies with engineering students in college (Blizzard & Klotz Reference Blizzard and Klotz2012). It also provides design problems with which previous engineering students were familiar and tasks that span across environmental, social and economic dimensions of sustainability (Blizzard et al. Reference Blizzard, Klotz, Potvin, Hazari, Cribbs and Godwin2015).

The design tasks were also checked for content validity with three experts in engineering education and a group of 15 graduate engineering students. The graduate engineering students were asked to brainstorm solutions to the 10 problems and then rate the level of difficulty of each problem. Students reported in interviews that there was little difference in task difficulty between the problems. The problems spanned topics including renewable energy, water quality, poverty and air pollution.

Students who participated in the research received the engineering problems in random order. The 10 problems were randomly divided into two categories: five were not given any additional dimension of sustainability and another five were given additional requirements for sustainability. The topics that received the additional dimension of sustainability were randomly selected out of the 10. The five tasks without additional specific dimensions of sustainability included: ‘Prevent accident deaths in a busy intersection’, ‘Alternative power generation sources other than coal for Haiti’, ‘Provide water in rural African villages’, ‘Remove carbon dioxide from the atmosphere’ and ‘Reduce slum populations in developing countries’.

The other five tasks that included additional dimensions of sustainability were: ‘Heat a house in winter. Your solution cannot include mechanical systems’ (i.e., passive design), ‘Reduce construction waste going to the landfill. Your solution must reuse discarded objects to create new products’ (i.e., circular economy), ‘Prevent water body contamination in cities. Your solutions must mimic or include processes found in nature’ (i.e., biomimicry), ‘Reduce traffic congestion in and around Washington, DC. Your solution must minimize demand for cars’ (i.e., human-centered solutions) and ‘Protect New York City against another Super Storm Sandy. Your solution must also restore nature’ (i.e., natural systems and resilience).

The presence of additional sustainability requirements is explicit in some tasks, such as a forced inclusion of environmental benefits in the solution, while in other cases the additional sustainability requirement is derived from imposing a specific sustainable design method, for example, biomimicry or passive design. The randomization of tasks that received the additional dimensions of sustainability and the averaging of the number of outcomes and neurocognitive response over the five tasks, described in more detail in the analysis techniques section, helped limit the possible effect of an individual task on the overall data. In addition to the broad integration of sustainability requirements taken from similar prior experiments (Klotz et al. Reference Klotz, Potvin, Godwin, Cribbs, Hazari and Barclay2014; Blizzard et al. Reference Blizzard, Klotz, Potvin, Hazari, Cribbs and Godwin2015; Hu & Shealy Reference Shealy, Hu and Gero2018), the use of multiple tasks offers a more comprehensive understanding about the broad effects of adding dimensions of sustainability and enabled a more broad contribution to the understanding of design cognition for sustainability.

Students were given 60 seconds to develop as many solutions as possible to each problem. Following each 60-second concept generation session, students were given a 30-second rest period before the next design problem. In total, the experiment lasted around 16.5 minutes. The timing of 60 seconds for each task was based on pilot studies (Grohs et al. Reference Grohs, Shealy, Darren, Mo, Panneton and Yang2017; Shealy et al. Reference Shealy, Grohs, Hu, Maczka and Panneton2017). The time frame for the resting period was chosen considering the pattern of human cortical activation. The 30-second time frame is double the length of the typical hemodynamic response for a cognitive task (Pinti et al. Reference Pinti, Aichelburg, Lind, Power, Swingler, Merla, Hamilton, Gilbert, Burgess and Tachtsidis2015). The purpose of the 30-second rest period was to bring the activated brain regions back to a resting state before the next task. In a pilot study, data and video recordings captured a spike in the cognitive activation in the PFC during the rest period. In discussion with participants during the pilot study, students described reflecting on their performance during this rest period. To reduce this reflection, basic arithmetic questions were included every 10 seconds during the 30 second rest period. These arithmetical problems do require cognitive activation in memory but less processing and effort than concept generation or reflection, and the region of activation is not the same (Dresler et al. Reference Dresler, Obersteiner, Schecklmann, Vogel, Ehlis, Richter, Plichta, Reiss, Pekrun and Fallgatter2009). The experimental design is outlined in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Block design process.

During the concept generation task, students verbally described their design solutions and a researcher tallied the number of unique solutions for each task. For example, a participant who suggested to reduce construction waste by integrating cut timber from the job site into the constructed building and developing a recycling program would receive two tallied solutions. Repeated answers, for example, mentioning a recycling program twice for the same engineering task was recorded only once. Metrics for measuring design ideation usually includes quantity, quality, novelty and variety (Shah et al. Reference Shah, Smith and Vargas-Hernandez2003). Experiments about concept generation, generally requires suspended evaluation, and are typically evaluated based on the number or novelty of solutions generated (Kudrowitz & Wallace Reference Kudrowitz and Wallace2013). In this study, the quantity (i.e., number of unique responses) was the main measurement because of its objectivity and prior literature demonstrating that the higher the fluency, the higher the novelty of solutions (Hocevar Reference Hocevar1979; Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Kohn and Arditti2011; Kudrowitz & Wallace Reference Kudrowitz and Wallace2013; Shiu Reference Shiu2014). Novelty is related to fluency (Hocevar Reference Hocevar1979; Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Kohn and Arditti2011; Kudrowitz & Wallace Reference Kudrowitz and Wallace2013; Shiu Reference Shiu2014). The quality of solutions is also important for design but generally in phased concept generation sessions, assessment of the quality of solutions is reserved for after the concept generation phase is over.

4.2. fNIRS data acquisition

fNIRS was used to measure change in cognitive activation. fNIRS is unique compared to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) because fNIRS allows participants to operate a computer or perform a task in an upright sitting position (Eysenck & Keane Reference Eysenck and Keane2015). A drawback of fNIRS compared to fMRI is that fNIRS is unable to capture subcortical activation in the brain due to limited power of the light emitted into the cortex. Although, brain regions critical for design cognition, such as the PFC, are adequately accessible with fNIRS (Strait & Scheutz Reference Strait and Scheutz2014). Another instrument used frequently in neurocognition research is Electroencephalogram (EEG). Compared to EEG, fNIRS has higher spatial resolution and allows for better detection of change in cortical regions of the brain (Eysenck & Keane Reference Eysenck and Keane2015). A limitation of fNIRS is that it fails to capture the activation on the order of milliseconds like EEG.

fNIRS is safe, portable and noninvasive. It is worn as a cap, similar to EEG, and emits light at specific wavelengths (700–900 nm) into the scalp. The light scatters, and some is absorbed, before reflecting back to the sensor. The deoxy-hemoglobin (deoxy-Hb) and oxy-hemoglobin (oxy-Hb) absorb more light than water and tissue in the brain. The relative concentration of oxy-Hb is calculated from the photon path length, based on the Modified Beer–Lambert Law. The assumption with measuring oxy-Hb is when neuronal activity is increased in one part of the brain, there is also an increased amount of cerebral blood flow to that area (Glimcher & Fehr Reference Glimcher and Fehr2013). This increase in blood flow produces an increase in the ratio of oxy-Hb relative to deoxy-Hb in that specific area. Both are measured with fNIRS, though typically only the oxy-Hb is reported in fNRIS studies (Hu & Shealy Reference Hu and Shealy2019).

fNIRS sensors, including light sources emitting the near-infrared light and detectors measuring the reflected light, were placed along the prefrontal frontal cortex so that fNIRS instrument will capture the change in hemoglobin in this region of interest. Figure 2 shows a participate wearing the cap and the brain regions covered by the sensors. In total eight sources and eight detectors were placed along the left and right hemisphere (four sources and four detectors on each hemisphere) of the scalp. These sensors and detectors composed 18 channels. A channel is the connection between one light source and one neighbouring detector.

Figure 2. fNIRS placement along the frontal cortex.

4.3. Analysis techniques

The data collected in the study included the behavioural data (number of solutions generated in brainstorming tasks) and neurocognitive data (change in Oxy-Hb using fNIRS in the PFC). Statistical methods, including two-way ANOVA, two sample t-tests and paired t-tests, were used to compare the difference between first-year and senior engineering students and the effects of added dimension of sustainability on engineering design cognition.

For each subject, the number of solutions in each task was counted and averaged respectively to obtain the number of solutions when additional dimensions of sustainability were and were not present. This averaging technique reduced the possible effects from different design problems. The statistical method used to compare the number of solutions was a two-way mixed-design ANOVA with repeated measure on one factor between first-year and senior engineering students when dimensions of sustainability were and were not present.

fNIRS raw data were processed using the package of HomER 2 NIRS in Matrix laboratory (MATLAB) (Huppert et al. Reference Huppert, Diamond, Franceschini and Boas2009) and only oxy-Hb response was analysed and reported since it has relatively higher amplitudes and sensitivity than deoxy-Hb (Cazzell et al. Reference Cazzell, Li, Lin, Patel and Liu2012). The algorithm of hmrMotionArtifactByChannel, hmrMotionCorrectSpline and Band-pass filter (0.03–0.2 Hz in 3rd order) in HomER 2 was used to remove and correct motion artefacts and other noises (e.g., instrumental noise and psychological noise) (Yücel et al. Reference Yücel, Selb, Copper and Boas2014; Naseer & Hong Reference Naseer and Hong2015). Due to a bad signal, data from three subjects were removed from the analysis. Among the remaining 20 participants (average 19.5 ± 1.64 years old, 10 females, right-handed), 12 were first-year engineering students and eight were senior engineering students.

A block averaging technique was applied for additional noise removal (Kirilina et al. Reference Kirilina, Yu, Jelzow, Wabnitz, Jacobs and Tachtsidis2013). The change of oxy-Hb in the five nonparameter tasks were averaged for each subject. The 30-second baseline data for each individual participant was subtracted from each channel when additional sustainability dimensions were or were not included in the brainstorming task. The resulting processed data are representative of the increase in neurocognitive activation due to the task.

To analyse cognitive difference between first-year and senior students, fNIRS data were averaged channel by channel across the sample of first-year and senior engineering students. This averaging approach follows similar previous fNIRS studies (Bunce et al. Reference Bunce, Izzetoglu, Ayaz, Shewokis, Izzetoglu, Pourrezaei and Onaral2011; Glotzbach et al. Reference Glotzbach, Mühlberger, Gschwendtner, Fallgatter, Pauli and Herrmann2011; Ferrari & Quaresima Reference Ferrari and Quaresima2012). The result is two series of data that represents the average oxy-Hb responses in the PFC.

To analyse neurocognitive difference among engineering students when additional dimensions of sustainability were present in the task, fNIRS data were averaged across all samples in the tasks without additional dimensions of sustainability and in the tasks when additional dimensions of sustainability were present. A Bonferroni correction was applied to increase the confidence interval from 95% (p < 0.05) to 99.998% (p < 0.002). The Bonferroni correction was to control the familywise error rate when using multiple t-tests. Two sample t-tests and paired t-tests were performed to compare the difference in cortical activation between first-year and senior students.

5. Results

The results are presented in the order of the research questions. First, the number of unique solutions generated and differences in neurocognition between first-year and senior students. The subsequent subsections describe the influence of the additional dimension of sustainability on the number of solutions engineering students were able to generate and the effect on their neurocognition.

5.1. First-year students produced significantly more solutions than seniors when brainstorming

A two-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with repeated measure was performed to compare the number of solutions generated by first-year and senior students, with and without dimensions of sustainability. First-year and senior engineering students generated a significantly different (F(1,39) = 9.12, p = 0.007) number of solutions when additional dimensions of sustainability were not included.

A posthoc Tukey test suggests significant difference within subjects (t = 25.92, p < 0.001) but not between subjects. An independent t-test identified the difference is between subjects when additional dimensions of sustainability were not included (t = 2.19, p = 0.04). The first-year engineering students generated significantly more solutions (5.63 ± 2.14) than the senior engineering students (4.10 ± 0.92) with a large effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.92). Figure 3 illustrates the number of solutions generated by first-year and senior engineering students in the two types of engineering design problems.

Figure 3. Average number of solutions generated by first-year students and seniors in the brainstorming tasks.

5.2. Neurocognitive differences are observed between first-year and senior engineering students during the brainstorming tasks

Cortical activation (i.e., the level of oxy-Hb) in 14 out of 18 channels is significantly (p < 0.002) different between first-year and senior engineering students when generating concepts. First-year students recruited significantly more cognitive activation in 11 out of the 14 channels. The most significant difference between first-year and senior engineering students occurred in the left and right dlPFC. First-year students recruited and sustained higher cognitive activation for a longer period of time in the dlPFC compared to senior students. This increased cognitive activation suggests more cognitive resources allocated to this region of the brain when generating design solutions by first-year students. The dlPFC plays a critical role in working memory (Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Leung and Johnson2003), cognitive flexibility (Grattan et al. Reference Grattan, Bloomer, Archambault and Eslinger1994) and abstract reasoning (Kroger et al. Reference Kroger, Sabb, Fales, Bookheimer, Cohen and Holyoak2002). Figure 4 shows the significant differences in activation between first-year and senior engineering students in four of the channels located in the dlPFC. The shaded areas in the figure represent the standard error among participants. The effect size in all 11 channels was large (Cohen’s d > 0.8).

Figure 4. Higher cognitive activation in the dlPFC among first-year students engineering students when generating concepts.

Senior engineering students produced significantly higher levels of activation in three channels, located in left and right vlPFC, shown in Figure 5. Previous research suggests that the left vlPFC is recruited for self-reflection during decision-making (Herwig et al. Reference Herwig, Kaffenberger, Schell, Jäncke and Brühl2012). While the right vlPFC is recruited to handle uncertainty processing (Levy & Wagner Reference Levy and Wagner2011). This result might suggest senior engineering students allocated more cognitive resources for self-reflection of their solutions and processing uncertainty, and less cognitive resources might have been available for the dlPFC, associated with divergent and convergent thinking.

Figure 5. Higher cognitive activation in the vlPFC among senior engineering students when generating concepts.

5.3. Including dimensions of sustainability reduces the number of solutions generated by first-year engineering students but not senior engineering students

A paired t-test indicated that additional dimensions of sustainability significantly reduced the number of solutions generated by first-year students (t = 5.02, p < 0.001, with a large effect size) but not for seniors (t = 2.01, p = 0.08), illustrated in Figure 3. The number of solutions senior engineering students generated remained consistent with and without additional dimensions of sustainability. Three out of eight seniors generated more solutions when dimensions of sustainability were required. There was no significant (p = 0.49) difference in the number of solutions generated by first-year and senior engineering students when additional dimensions of sustainability were required. In other words, the additional dimension of sustainability only had an effect on first-year engineering students’ ability to generate solutions. However, more years of training in engineering did not facilitate senior engineering students to generate more sustainable solutions to engineering problems than first-year students.

5.4. Both first-year and senior engineering students use more cognitive resources when generating solutions to meet additional dimensions of sustainability

The repeated ANOVA indicates that the cortical activation of all the engineering students was significantly higher in the left vlPFC and dlPFC when the additional dimensions of sustainability were added. Figure 6 illustrates the cognitive difference in two channels located in the dlPFC and vlPFC when generating solutions for both types of tasks. As mentioned before, the left vlPFC is associated with self-reflection. The left dlPFC is generally associated with playing an active role during convergent thinking and making analytic judgements (Luft et al. Reference Luft, Zioga, Banissy and Bhattacharya2017). Higher activation in these two regions in the left hemisphere means more cognitive resources are recruited when additional dimensions of sustainability are added to the concept generation task.

Figure 6. Higher cortical activation in the vlPFC and dlPFC among first-year and senior engineering students when including additional dimensions of sustainability.

Further analysis of students’ neurocognition when focusing on tasks with additional dimensions of sustainable between the two groups of students shows the underlying mechanism that differs between them. When generating solutions for the problems with the additional dimension of sustainability, first-year engineering students recruited significantly (p < 0.002) higher levels of activation in 10 channels located in the left and right dlPFC with large effect sizes. This increase in activation occurs without first-year students generating an equal number of solutions as the tasks without the added dimension of sustainability.

Averaged over all channels in the PFC, first-year engineering students recruited more cognitive resources than senior engineering students. The first-year and senior engineering students produced a similar number of solutions when the additional dimension of sustainability was present. The senior engineering students were more cognitively efficient in their ability to generate sustainable solutions compared to the first-year engineering students. Meaning, senior engineering students required less cognitive resources to generate each solution compared to the first-year engineering students. Within the subregions of the PFC, first-year engineering students recruited more cognitive activation specifically in the dlPFC, but this group required significantly (p < 0.002) less cognitive resources in two channels located in the left vlPFC compared to the senior engineering students. An increase in activation in the left vlPFC among senior engineering students might suggest seniors recruited more cognition for self-reflection than first-year students (Fiez et al. Reference Fiez, Raife, Balota, Schwarz, Raichle and Petersen1996; Ranganath et al. Reference Ranganath, Johnson and D’Esposito2003; Deppe et al. Reference Deppe, Schwindt, Kugel, Plassmann and Kenning2005).

6. Discussion

Quantity is one of the critical metrics measuring the effectiveness of design ideation (Shah et al. Reference Shah, Smith and Vargas-Hernandez2003). Ball et al. (Reference Ball, Maskill and Ormerod1998) point out that during the ideation phase of engineering design, generating more design alternatives is beneficial to increase the chances of a better final design. Theorists and educational professionals teach ideation during engineering design by generating a wide range of alternative solutions (Osborn Reference Osborn1993; Cross Reference Cross2004; Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Kohn and Arditti2011). Ideally, engineering education is supposed to enhance the ability of students to generate more solutions to engineering design problems (Yilmaz et al. Reference Yilmaz, Jablokow, Daly, Silk and Berg2014). However, the results presented in this paper seem to suggest the opposite. In the design tasks without added dimensions of sustainability, senior engineering students, with more years of engineering education, failed to generate a larger range of solutions compared with first-year engineering students.

The fewer solutions produced by senior engineering students might be explained by concept fixation. Usually, designers with more expertise have the tendency to fixate on their early solutions and are reluctant to generate more alternatives (Genco et al. Reference Genco, Hölttä‐Otto and Seepersad2012). On the other hand, generating a wide range of alternative concepts may not always benefit design quality, therefore generating a limited number of alternatives can be an appropriate strategy (Cross Reference Cross2004).

The cortical activation captured by fNIRS offers neurocognitive explanations about the difference in the number of solutions generated by first-year and senior engineering students. First-year students recruited more activation in the brain region associated with working memory, cognitive flexibility and abstract reasoning (dlPFC). Senior engineering students recruited more activation in the brain regions associated with uncertainty processing and self-reflection in decision-making (vlPFC).

A possible explanation for the higher activation among senior engineering students in the vlPFC is that the senior engineering students felt uncertain about their ideas and applied a filter to evaluate and reflect if their answer was acceptable prior to verbally providing the solution. This result seems consistent with the findings in previous research, which found that undergraduate engineering students spend more cognitive effort analyzing their solutions (Lammi & Gero Reference Lammi and Gero2011) while high-school students generate more ideas without further evaluation about their ideas (Kavakli & Gero Reference Kavakli and Gero2002). The increased activation among first-year students in the right and left dlPFC provides supporting neurocognitive evidence for why they generated significantly more solutions. First-year students increased involvement of their right dlPFC. This region is usually associated with divergent thinking in creative tasks (Moore et al. Reference Moore, Bhadelia, Billings, Fulwiler, Heilman, Rood and Gansler2009; Zmigrod et al. Reference Zmigrod, Colzato and Hommel2015). First-year students also recruited more activation in their left dlPFC. This region is generally associated with convergent thinking in creative tasks (Luft et al. Reference Luft, Zioga, Banissy and Bhattacharya2017).

6.1. The effects of adding dimensions of sustainability during ideation

Sustainability is generally regarded as a design constraint limiting the engineering design processes (Collado-Ruiz & Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi Reference Collado-Ruiz and Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi2010). Concerns about the ability of engineering students to contribute to sustainable design continue to persist (Carew & Mitchell Reference Carew and Mitchell2008; Davidson et al. Reference Davidson, Hendrickson, Matthews, Bridges, Allen, Murphy, Allenby, Crittenden and Austin2010; Khalili et al. Reference Khalili, Duecker, Ashton and Chavez2015). The results presented in this paper find that requiring students to meet additional dimensions of sustainability increased the difficulty of design concept generation. Students had to recruit more cognitive resources to complete the tasks when additional sustainability requirements were presented.

First-year engineering students were unable to generate as many solutions to the problems with additional dimension of sustainability as they generated when sustainability was not a requirement. These first-year engineering students may not have adequate knowledge about sustainability or design methods to manage this added complexity. Senior engineering students were less affected by the additional dimensions of sustainability. They recruited more cognitive resources but were able to generate a similar number of solutions compared to the tasks without the added dimensions of sustainability. The difference in outcomes between first-year and senior engineering students offers new evidence that engineering education may support the objective of enhancing student capabilities to cognitively manage the added complexities required for sustainable design.

The neurocognitive evidence suggests that both first-year and senior engineering students recruited significantly more cognitive resources to complete the ideation tasks when sustainability was a requirement. This result is consistent with prior studies that find increasingly difficult tasks produce increasingly higher activation in the PFC (Petkar et al. Reference Petkar, Dande, Yadav, Zeng and Nguyen2010; Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2017). However, first-year and senior engineering students appear to use different neurocognitive strategies to handle the design constrains associated with sustainable design. The differences in neurocognitive activation between first-year and senior engineering students were observed when sustainability was not a requirement but becomes more pronounced when sustainability was added to the concept generation tasks.

Adding dimensions of sustainability triggered more neurocognitive activation in the dlPFC for first-year students. Senior engineering students recruited more neurocognitive resources to the vlPFC, the brain region generally associated with self-reflection. A possible explanation for the contradiction between first-year students’ higher neurocognitive activation and fewer solutions is that they continue to cognitively search for solutions but do not have the knowledge basis or experience to meet that added design constraints (She & MacDonald Reference She and MacDonald2018; Maccioni et al. Reference Maccioni, Borgianni and Basso2019). A related explanation is first-year students may have become overwhelmed with the added complexity and cognitive overload led to the decrease in productivity (Collado-Ruiz & Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi Reference Collado-Ruiz and Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi2010). Another possible explanation is first-year students allocated more cognitive resources to processing the problem, and the problem space, as a result, less cognitive resources remained to generate a large number of solutions.

The increased neurocognitive activation in brains of the senior engineering students occurred in the vlPFC, the region associated with self-reflection. Prior research finds this region critical for performance in sustainable design and design participants elicited higher activation in the vlPFC when they showed more preference for sustainable products (Goucher-Lambert et al. Reference Goucher-Lambert, Moss and Cagan2017).

6.2. Implications for sustainable design teaching in engineering education

The senior engineering students who participated in the experiment had taken sustainability-related courses. These senior engineering students likely had more knowledge about sustainable design. Unsurprisingly, they appear more prepared to cognitively manage additional requirements for sustainable design compared to the first-year students. Prior research suggests students who learn about broad dimensions of sustainability were subsequently better prepared to produce novel engineering design solutions (McWhirter & Shealy Reference McWhirter and Shealy2020). The results presented here provide new supporting evidence for the potential benefit of education for sustainability to enhance engineering design.

In addition to courses about sustainability, senior engineering students had more experience with the engineering design process. They had practice with managing design requirements like sustainability and this likely had an effect on the outcome. Experience is a significant factor in design cognition (Kavakli & Gero Reference Kavakli and Gero2002, Reference Kavakli, Gero and Lindemann2003). Cognitive differences from experience are observable in the brain. For example, air traffic controllers with decades of experience required significantly less cognitive resources to manage a higher number of airplanes than novice air traffic controllers (Harrison et al. Reference Harrison, Izzetoglu, Ayaz, Willems, Hah, Ahlstrom, Woo, Shewokis, Bunce and Onaral2014).

Teaching design ideation techniques like Theory of Inventive Problem Solving (TRIZ) or the use of concept mapping may help improve students’ ability to cognitively manage the complexity of sustainability (Shealy et al. Reference Shealy, Grohs, Hu, Maczka and Panneton2017; Shealy et al. Reference Shealy, Hu and Gero2018). For example, students that use concept maps during design ideation produce significantly more concepts and also require less cognitive resources (Hu et al. Reference Hu, Shealy, Grohs and Panneton2019). Future research could explore the effects of teaching first-year students design techniques like concept mapping prior to design ideation tasks and measure the effect on design cognition.

The use of concept maps may also help senior engineering students. Concept mapping was previously demonstrated to direct more cognitive resources to the dlPFC (Hu et al. Reference Hu, Shealy, Grohs and Panneton2019). This is the region of the brain associated with divergent thinking and was less recruited among senior engineering students compared to first-year engineering students during the concept generation tasks. Senior engineering students produced fewer design solutions than first-year students when sustainability was not a requirement.

The observed neurocognitive changes from the study presented in this paper offers potential opportunities for interventions to help improve sustainable design outcomes. For example, future research could explore whether priming the recruitment of activation in the dlPFC, or through transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to the dlPFC, can improve senior engineering students’ ability to generate more novel design solutions (Sahar et al. Reference Sahar, Farb and Shu2020). Designers who first completed the Stroop test (requiring participants to quickly and accurately name font color that is mismatched with word text) subsequently produced more creative solutions than designers who did not complete the Stroop test. The increased level of creativity was equal to designers that received a transcranial shock (Sahar et al. Reference Sahar, Farb and Shu2020). This type of intervention, through priming or tDCS, may also help overcome design fixation. For example, tDCS can help enhance cognitive control (Feeser et al. Reference Feeser, Prehn, Kazzer, Mungee and Bajbouj2014) and reduce attention bias (Heeren et al. Reference Heeren, Baeken, Vanderhasselt, Philippot and de Raedt2015).

In addition to future research exploring the effects of interventions to improve sustainable design, future research should also begin to explore if added dimensions of sustainability produce unique mental activities or if the observed mental activities are similar to concept generation process with other types of design constraints or requirements. For instance, a future study could compare the activation patterns between sustainable design and empathic design. Empathetic design may elicit higher activation in the mPFC (Shealy et al. Reference Shealy, Gero, Hu and Milovanovic2020) and may lead to a shift of cognitive functions between divergent and convergent thinking in the right and left PFC (Milovanovic et al. Reference Milovanovic, Hu, Shealy and Gero2020).

7. Limitations

There are several limitations to this study that are worth mentioning. fNIRS data from this study only include the change of oxygenated hemoglobin in the PFC. Other brain regions (e.g., parietal cortex) likely contribute to concept generation. This is a limitation common to all neuroimaging studies that do not capture whole-brain activation (Ayaz et al. Reference Ayaz, Shewokis, İzzetoğlu, Çakır and Onaral2012; Cazzell et al. Reference Cazzell, Li, Lin, Patel and Liu2012). The time constraint for each design task is another limitation. It may have contributed to the number of solutions students generated. A consistent amount of time for each concept generation task was necessary so the neurocognition within and between subjects could be averaged and compared. Future research can begin to explore not just the initial patterns of neurocognition among designers (in the first 60 seconds) but longer periods of time and with more subjects (Shealy et al. Reference Shealy, Hu and Gero2018). Another limitation is that this study focused on neurocognitive differences and the number of unique solutions engineering students generated. Future research can begin to explore the uniqueness of solutions rather than just the fluency of solutions.

The quality of each solution was not included as an evaluation metric because this is beyond the initial phase of concept generation, which focuses primarily on the quantity of solutions. Future studies can use more metrics, such as quality, novelty and variety, to measure the effectiveness of ideation to address sustainable design tasks. The 20-person sample size is another limitation (Schönbrodt & Perugini Reference Schönbrodt and Perugini2013). Although, the number of participants is above the average of seven subjects in a systematic review of conceptual design cognition (Hay et al. Reference Hay, Duffy, McTeague, Pidgeon, Vuletic and Grealy2017b) and similar to the average sample size of 27 in other fNIRS studies for problem solving and decision-making (Hu & Shealy Reference Hu and Shealy2019). Future research should replicate the results with a larger sample size (Shrout & Rodgers Reference Shrout and Rodgers2018).

Another limitation was the design problems. The use of 10 previously developed engineering design problems was intentional to control for potential experimental error between each individual problem. Future research could begin by replicating the experiment with the two sets of problems in counter-balance to achieve better parity. The list was also inherently limited. Future research could also replicate the study with an additional new set of design tasks.

Another future study could ask students to narrate how they developed solutions or if they felt uncertain, and to reflect about their solutions before verbally saying them out loud. More descriptions about how and why students generated concepts would give additional insights into the cognitive processes observed in the brain. Another line of research stemming from this study is how the use of mnemonics, specific training or design methods and tools related to sustainability may influence where and how engineers access information in their brain. For instance, prompting students with design heuristics (Daly et al. Reference Daly, Christian, Yilmaz, Seifert and Gonzalez2012) or priming exercises (She et al. Reference She, Seepersad, Holtta-Otto, MacDonald, Plattner, Meinel and Leifer2018) may lead to more targeted ideas or refocus their solutions to options previously not considered.

8. Conclusion

The interdisciplinary study reported in this paper explores how engineering students cognitively produce solutions to engineering design problems and cognitively manage added dimensions of sustainability. The results indicate that first-year students generate more solutions and elicit higher cortical activation associated with cognitive flexibility than seniors when additional dimensions of sustainability are not included. Seniors recruited higher activation in the region of their brain primarily associated with uncertainty processing and self-reflection, suggesting a possible tendency of evaluation and design fixation. The inclusion of additional sustainability dimensions in the brainstorming task required higher cognitive activation associated with cognitive flexibility for both first-year students and seniors, however, only first-year students, showed a decrease in the number of solutions when dimensions of sustainability were included.

Many empirical studies have investigated the cognitive processes during brainstorming or ideation for sustainability by observation or self-report (Cross Reference Cross2001; Coley et al. Reference Coley, Houseman and Roy2007; Daly et al. Reference Daly, Christian, Yilmaz, Seifert and Gonzalez2012). A key limitation is the subjectivity and lack of reliability that comes with observation and self-report. The study presented in this paper provides a novel objective measure of design cognition through neuroimaging and demonstrates the potential of using fNIRS to triangulate design cognition and outcomes.

This interdisciplinary study, combining sustainable design and neurocognition, contributes to the science of sustainable engineering by demonstrating the effects of sustainability requirements on design behaviour and design cognition. Better understanding the activated brain regions required for sustainable design and how additional dimensions of sustainability affect students’ ability to produce outcomes begins to shed light on the current gap and potential need of engineering education for sustainability. Future studies can begin to test the effectiveness of novel design methods and tools, for example, concept mapping or priming, to overcome fixation and enhance engineering students’ ability to develop engineering design solutions.

Acknowledgments

This material is based in part on work supported by The National Science Foundation, through Grant EEC-1929892. Any opinions, findings and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.

References

Allen, M. S. 1962 Morphological Creativity. Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Allenby, B. 2011 Rethinking engineering education. In Proceedings of the 2011 IEEE International Symposium on Sustainable Systems and Technology, pp. 1–5.Google Scholar
ASEE. 1999 ASEE statement on sustainable development education, (February 12th 2009) http://www.asee.org/about/Sustainable_Development.cfm.Google Scholar
Ashford, N. A. 2004 Major challenges to engineering education for sustainable development: what has to change to make it creative, effective, and acceptable to the established disciplines? International Journal of Sustainability in Higher Education 5 (3), 239250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayaz, H., Shewokis, P. A., İzzetoğlu, M., Çakır, M. P. & Onaral, B. 2012 Tangram solved? Prefrontal cortex activation analysis during geometric problem solving. In 2012 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, pp. 47244727, IEEE.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azapagic, A. 2005 Integrating sustainability into engineering curriculum: case study-based learning. In 05AIChE: 2005 AIChE Annual Meeting and Fall Showcase, Oct 30-Nov 4 2005, AIChE Annual Meeting, Conference Proceedings, American Institute of Chemical Engineers, New York, NY 10016-5991, United States, 13121.Google Scholar
Azapagic, A., Perdan, S. & Shallcross, D. 2005 How much do engineering students know about sustainable development? The findings of an international survey and possible implications for the engineering curriculum. European Journal of Engineering Education 30 (1), 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aziz-Zadeh, L., Liew, S. L. & Dandekar, F. 2013 Exploring the neural correlates of visual creativity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8 (4), 475480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ball, L. J., Maskill, L. & Ormerod, T. C. 1998 Satisficing in engineering design: causes, consequences and implications for design support. Automation in Construction, Models of Design 7 (2), 213227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barth, M., Godemann, J., Rieckmann, M. & Stoltenberg, U. 2007 Developing key competencies for sustainable development in higher education. International Journal of Sustainability in Higher Education (eds. Adomssent, M., Godemann, J. & Michelsen, G.) 8 (4), 416430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F. & Neubauer, A. C. 2014 To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. NeuroImage 88, 125133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkowitz, A. L. & Ansari, D. 2010 Expertise-related deactivation of the right temporoparietal junction during musical improvisation. NeuroImage 49 (1), 712719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhikhoo, N., Hashemi, A. & Cruickshank, H. 2017 Improving thermal comfort of low-income housing in Thailand through passive design strategies. Sustainability, Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute 9 (8), 1440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bielefeldt, A. R. 2013 Pedagogies to achieve sustainability learning outcomes in civil and environmental engineering students. Sustainability 5 (10), 44794501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blizzard, J., Klotz, L., Potvin, G., Hazari, Z., Cribbs, J. & Godwin, A. 2015 Using survey questions to identify and learn more about those who exhibit design thinking traits. Design Studies 38, 92110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blizzard, J. 2013 Design Thinkers Can Save the World: How Understanding Their Interests, Goals, and Motivations Can Inform Engineering Educators. Clemson University.Google Scholar
Blizzard, J. L. & Klotz, L. E. 2012 A framework for sustainable whole systems design. Design Studies 33 (5), 456479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borgianni, Y. & Maccioni, L. 2020 Review of the use of neurophysiological and biometric measures in experimental design research. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing 34, 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremer, M. H. Gonzalez, E. & Mercado, E. 2010 Teaching creativity and innovation using sustainability as driving force. International Journal of Engineering Education 26 (2), 430437.Google Scholar
Brundtland, G. H. 1987 Our Common Future. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bucciarelli, L. L. 1988 An ethnographic perspective on engineering design. Design Studies 9 (3), 159168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunce, S. C., Izzetoglu, K., Ayaz, H., Shewokis, P., Izzetoglu, M., Pourrezaei, K. & Onaral, B. 2011 Implementation of fNIRS for monitoring levels of expertise and mental workload. In Foundations of Augmented Cognition. Directing the Future of Adaptive Systems, pp. 1322. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. 2000 Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12 (1), 147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardenas, C., Sosa, R., Moysen, R. & Martinez, V. 2010 Sustaining sustainable design through systemic thinking. International Journal of Engineering Education 29 (2), 287292.Google Scholar
Carew, A. L. & Mitchell, C. A. 2008 Teaching sustainability as a contested concept: capitalizing on variation in engineering educators’ conceptions of environmental, social and economic sustainability. Journal of Cleaner Production (Netherlands) 16 (1), 105115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cazzell, M., Li, L., Lin, Z.-J., Patel, S. J. & Liu, H. 2012 Comparison of neural correlates of risk decision-making between genders: an exploratory fNIRS study of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). NeuroImage 62 (3), 18961911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cervero, R. 2003 Road expansion, urban growth, and induced travel: a path analysis. Journal of the American Planning Association 69 (2), 145163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ceschin, F. 2014 How the design of socio-technical experiments can enable radical changes for sustainability. International Journal of Design 8 (3).Google Scholar
Chau, K. W. 2007 Incorporation of sustainability concepts into a civil engineering curriculum. Journal of Professional Issues in Engineering Education and Practice 133 (3), 188191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chitchyan, R., Becker, C., Betz, S., Duboc, L., Penzenstadler, B., Seyff, N., & Venters, C. C. 2016 Sustainability design in requirements engineering: state of practice. In Proceedings of the 38th International Conference on Software Engineering Companion, ICSE’16, pp. 533–542. Association for Computing Machinery.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coley, F., Houseman, O. & Roy, R. 2007 An introduction to capturing and understanding the cognitive behaviour of design engineers. Journal of Engineering Design 18 (4), 311325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collado-Ruiz, D. & Ostad-Ahmad-Ghorabi, H. 2010 Influence of environmental information on creativity. Design Studies 31 (5), 479498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, N. 2001 Design cognition: results from protocol and other empirical studies of design activity. Design Knowing and Learning: Cognition in Design Education, Elsevier, 10(3)79103.Google Scholar
Cross, N. (2004) Expertise in design: an overview. Design Studies, Expertise in Design 25 (5), 427441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, N. 2011 Design Thinking: Understanding How Designers Think and Work. Berg Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, N. & Cross, A. C. 1998 Expertise in engineering design. Research in Engineering Design 10 (3), 141149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cucuzzella, C. 2016 Creativity, sustainable design and risk management. Journal of Cleaner Production 135, 15481558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curran, M. A. 2009 Wrapping our brains around sustainability. Sustainability 1 (1), 513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, S. R., Christian, J. L., Yilmaz, S., Seifert, C. M. & Gonzalez, R. 2012 Assessing design heuristics for idea generation in an introductory engineering course. International Journal of Engineering Education 28 (2), 463473.Google Scholar
Daly, S. R., Mosyjowski, E. A., & Seifert, C. M. (2014). Teaching Creativity in Engineering Courses. Journal of Engineering Education, 103(3), 417449. https://doi.org/10.1002/jee.20048CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, C. I., Hendrickson, C. T., Matthews, H. S., Bridges, M. W., Allen, D. T., Murphy, C. F., Allenby, B. R., Crittenden, J. C. & Austin, S. 2010 Preparing future engineers for challenges of the 21st century: Sustainable engineering. Journal of Cleaner Production, Going beyond the rhetoric: system-wide changes in universities for sustainable societies 18 (7), 698701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deppe, M., Schwindt, W., Kugel, H., Plassmann, H. & Kenning, P. 2005 Nonlinear responses within the medial prefrontal cortex reveal when specific implicit information influences economic decision-making. Journal of Neuroimaging: Official Journal of the American Society of Neuroimaging 15 (2), 171182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorst, K. & Cross, N. 2001 Creativity in the design process: co-evolution of problem–solution. Design Studies 22 (5), 425437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dresler, T., Obersteiner, A., Schecklmann, M., Vogel, A. C. M., Ehlis, A.-C., Richter, M. M., Plichta, M. M., Reiss, K., Pekrun, R. & Fallgatter, A. J. 2009 Arithmetic tasks in different formats and their influence on behavior and brain oxygenation as assessed with near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS): a study involving primary and secondary school children. Journal of Neural Transmission (Vienna, Austria: 1996) 116 (12), 16891700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dym, C. L., Agogino, A. M., Eris, O., Frey, D. D. & Leifer, L. J. 2005 Engineering design thinking, teaching, and learning. Journal of Engineering Education 94 (1), 103120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
EPA. 2007 What Is Green Engineering (March 1st 2009) http://www.epa.gov/oppt/greenengineering/pubs/whats_ge.html.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. & Simon, A. H. 1984 Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Euston, D. R., Gruber, A. J. & McNaughton, B. L. 2012 The role of medial prefrontal cortex in memory and decision making. Neuron 76 (6), 10571070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, M. W. & Keane, M. T. 2015 Cognitive Psychology: A Student’s Handbook. Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feeser, M., Prehn, K., Kazzer, P., Mungee, A. & Bajbouj, M. 2014 Transcranial direct current stimulation enhances cognitive control during emotion regulation. Brain Stimulation 7 (1), 105112. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2013.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, M. & Quaresima, V. 2012 A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. NeuroImage 63 (2), 921935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiez, J. A., Raife, E. A., Balota, D. A., Schwarz, J. P., Raichle, M. E. & Petersen, S. E. 1996 A positron emission tomography study of the short-term maintenance of verbal information. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience 16 (2), 808822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fini, E. H., Awadallah, F., Parast, M. M. & Abu-Lebdeh, T. 2018 The impact of project-based learning on improving student learning outcomes of sustainability concepts in transportation engineering courses. European Journal of Engineering Education, Taylor & Francis 43 (3), 473488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funahashi, S. 2017 Working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Brain Sciences 7 (5), 49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genco, N., Hölttä‐Otto, K. & Seepersad, C. C. 2012 An experimental investigation of the innovation capabilities of undergraduate engineering students. Journal of Engineering Education 101 (1), 6081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gero, J. S. & Mc Neill, T. 1998 An approach to the analysis of design protocols. Design Studies 19 (1), 2161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gero, J. S. & Milovanovic, J. 2020 A framework for studying design thinking through measuring designers’ minds, bodies and brains. Design Science 6 (e19),.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervásio, H., Santos, P., Martins, R. & Simões da Silva, L. 2014 A macro-component approach for the assessment of building sustainability in early stages of design. Building and Environment 73, 256270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, C., Folley, B. S. & Park, S. 2009 Enhanced divergent thinking and creativity in musicians: a behavioral and near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain and Cognition 69 (1), 162169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glimcher, P. W. & Fehr, E. 2013 Neuroeconomics: Decision Making and the Brain. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Glotzbach, E., Mühlberger, A., Gschwendtner, K., Fallgatter, A. J., Pauli, P. & Herrmann, M. J. 2011 Prefrontal brain activation during emotional processing: a functional near infrared spectroscopy study (fNIRS). The Open Neuroimaging Journal 5, 3339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goel, V. 2014 Creative brains: designing in the real world †. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldenberg, J., Mazursky, D. & Solomon, S. 1999 Toward identifying the inventive templates of new products: a channeled ideation approach. Journal of Marketing Research, 200210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldschmidt, G. 2016 Linkographic evidence for concurrent divergent and convergent thinking in creative design. Creativity Research Journal 28 (2), 115122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goucher-Lambert, K. & Cagan, J. 2015 The impact of sustainability on consumer preference judgments of product attributes. Journal of Mechanical Design 137 (8), 081401 (11 pages).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goucher-Lambert, K., Moss, J. & Cagan, J. 2017 Inside the mind: using neuroimaging to understand moral product preference judgments involving sustainability. Journal of Mechanical Design 139 (4), 041103 (11 pages).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grattan, L. M., Bloomer, R. H., Archambault, F. X. & Eslinger, P. J. 1994 Cognitive flexibility and empathy after frontal lobe lesion. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology 7 (4), 251259.Google Scholar
Grohs, J., Shealy, T., Darren, M., Mo, H., Panneton, R. & Yang, X. 2017 Evaluating the Potential of fNIRS Neuroimaging to Study Engineering Problem Solving and Design. ASEE.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, J., Izzetoglu, K., Ayaz, H., Willems, B., Hah, S., Ahlstrom, U., Woo, H., Shewokis, P. A., Bunce, S. C. & Onaral, B. 2014 Cognitive workload and learning assessment during the implementation of a next-generation air traffic control technology using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems 44 (4), 429440. doi:10.1109/THMS.2014.2319822.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, L., Duffy, A. H. B., Gilbert, S. J., Lyall, L., Campbell, G., Coyle, D. & Grealy, M. A. 2019 The neural correlates of ideation in product design engineering practitioners. Design Science 5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, L., Duffy, A. H. B., McTeague, C., Pidgeon, L. M., Vuletic, T. & Grealy, M. 2017a. Towards a shared ontology: a generic classification of cognitive processes in conceptual design. In Design Science, p. 3. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hay, L., Duffy, A. H. B., McTeague, C., Pidgeon, L. M., Vuletic, T. & Grealy, M. 2017b. A systematic review of protocol studies on conceptual design cognition: design as search and exploration. In Design Science, p. 3. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Heeren, A., Baeken, C., Vanderhasselt, M.A., Philippot, P. & de Raedt, R. 2015 Impact of anodal and cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during attention bias modification: an eye-tracking study. PLOS ONE 10 (4), e0124182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herwig, U., Kaffenberger, T., Schell, C., Jäncke, L. & Brühl, A. B. 2012 Neural activity associated with self-reflection. BMC Neuroscience 13, 52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hocevar, D. 1979 Ideational fluency as a confounding factor in the measurement of originality. Journal of Educational Psychology 71 (2), 191196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howard, T. J., Culley, S. J. & Dekoninck, E. 2008 Describing the creative design process by the integration of engineering design and cognitive psychology literature. Design Studies 29 (2), 160180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, M. & Shealy, T. 2018. Systems versus linear thinking: measuring cognitive networks for engineering sustainability. In Construction Research Congress, Proceedings, American Society for Civil Engineering.Google Scholar
Hu, M. & Shealy, T. 2019 Application of functional near-infrared spectroscopy to measure engineering decision-making and design cognition: literature review and synthesis of methods. Journal of Computing in Civil Engineering 33 (6), 04019034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, M., Shealy, T., Grohs, J. & Panneton, R. 2019 Empirical evidence that concept mapping reduces neurocognitive effort during concept generation for sustainability. Journal of Cleaner Production 238, 117815.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, W.-L. & Reid, T. 2018 The effects of designers’ contextual experience on the ideation process and design outcomes. Journal of Mechanical Design, American Society of Mechanical Engineers Digital Collection 140 (10), 101101101118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, P., Qiu, L., Shen, L., Zhang, Y., Song, Z., Qi, Z., Gong, Q. & Xie, P. 2013 Evidence for a left-over-right inhibitory mechanism during figural creative thinking in healthy nonartists. Human Brain Mapping 34 (10), 27242732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huntzinger, D. N., Hutchins, M. J., Gierke, J. S. & Sutherland, J. W. 2007 Enabling sustainable thinking in undergraduate engineering education. International Journal of Engineering Education 23, 218230.Google Scholar
Huppert, T. J., Diamond, S. G., Franceschini, M. A. & Boas, D. A. 2009 HomER: a review of time-series analysis methods for near-infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics 48 (10), 280298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hymel, K. M., Small, K. A. & Dender, K. V. 2010 Induced demand and rebound effects in road transport. Transportation Research Part B: Methodological 44 (10), 12201241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, J. 2007 A song of the city|Video on TED.com.Google Scholar
Jonson, B. 2005 Design ideation: the conceptual sketch in the digital age. Design Studies 26 (6), 613624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrini, K., Pollick, F. E., Dahl, S., McAleer, P., McKay, L., Rocchesso, D., Haakon Waadeland, K., Love, S., Avanzini, F. & Puce, A. 2011 Action expertise reduces brain activity for audiovisual matching actions: an fMRI study with expert drummers. Neuroimage 56 (3), 14801492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kajzer Mitchell, I. & Walinga, J. 2017. The creative imperative: the role of creativity, creative problem solving and insight as key drivers for sustainability. Journal of Cleaner Production 140, 18721884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapalko, M. 2010 Avoid the greenwashing trap. Sustainability: The Journal of Record 3 (6), 317318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavakli, M. & Gero, J. 2003 Difference between expert and novice designers: an experimental study. In Human Behaviour in Design (ed. Lindemann, U), pp. 4251. Springer. CUMINCAD.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavakli, M. & Gero, J. S. 2002 The structure of concurrent cognitive actions: a case study on novice and expert designers. Design Studies 23 (1), 2540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khalili, N. R., Duecker, S., Ashton, W. & Chavez, F. 2015 From cleaner production to sustainable development: the role of academia. Journal of Cleaner Production, Integrating Cleaner Production into Sustainability Strategies 96, 3043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirilina, E., Yu, N., Jelzow, A., Wabnitz, H., Jacobs, A. M. & Tachtsidis, I. 2013 Identifying and quantifying main components of physiological noise in functional near infrared spectroscopy on the prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 7081.Google ScholarPubMed
Klotz, L., Pickering, J., Schmidt, R. & Weber, E. U. 2019 Design behaviour for sustainability. Nature Sustainability 2 (12), 10671069.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klotz, L., Weber, E., Johnson, E., Shealy, T., Hernandez, M. & Gordon, B. 2018. Beyond rationality in engineering design for sustainability. Nature Sustainability, 1 (5), 225233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klotz, L., Potvin, G., Godwin, A., Cribbs, J., Hazari, Z. & Barclay, N. 2014 Sustainability as a route to broadening participation in engineering. Journal of Engineering Education 103 (1), 137153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoll, S. W. & Horton, G. 2010 Changing the perspective: improving generate thinkLets for ideation. In System Sciences (HICSS), 2010 43rd Hawaii International Conference on, IEEE, 1–10.Google Scholar
Kroger, J. K., Sabb, F. W., Fales, C. L., Bookheimer, S. Y., Cohen, M. S. & Holyoak, K. J. 2002 Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. Cerebral Cortex 12 (5), 477485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudrowitz, B. M. & Wallace, D. 2013 Assessing the quality of ideas from prolific, early-stage product ideation. Journal of Engineering Design 24 (2), 120139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambrechts, W., Mulà, I., Ceulemans, K., Molderez, I. & Gaeremynck, V. 2013 The integration of competences for sustainable development in higher education: an analysis of bachelor programs in management. Journal of Cleaner Production, Environmental Management for Sustainable Universities (EMSU) 2010, 48, 6573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lammi, M. D. & Gero, J. S. 2011 Comparing design cognition of undergraduate engineering students and high school pre-engineering students. In 2011 Frontiers in Education Conference (FIE), F4F-1-F4F-6.Google Scholar
Lee, D., Klein, L. & Camus, G. 1999. Induced traffic and induced demand. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board 1659, 6875.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B. J. & Wagner, A. D. 2011 Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and action updating. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1224 (1), 4062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liang, C., Chang, C.-C. & Liu, Y.-C. 2019. Comparison of the cerebral activities exhibited by expert and novice visual communication designers during idea incubation. International Journal of Design Creativity and Innovation, Taylor & Francis 7 (4), 213236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindau, L. A., Hidalgo, D. & Facchini, D. 2010. Bus rapid transit in Curitiba, Brazil. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board 2193 (1), 1727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linke, B. S., & Moreno, J. (2015). New concepts for bio-inspired sustainable grinding. Journal of Manufacturing Processes, 19, 7380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmapro.2015.05.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litman, T. & Laube, F. 2002. Automobile dependency and economic development.Google Scholar
Lozano, R. 2010. Diffusion of sustainable development in universities’ curricula: an empirical example from Cardiff University. Journal of Cleaner Production, Going beyond the rhetoric: system-wide changes in universities for sustainable societies 18 (7), 637644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luft, C. D. B., Zioga, I., Banissy, M. J. & Bhattacharya, J. 2017. Relaxing learned constraints through cathodal tDCS on the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Scientific Reports 7 (1), 2916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maccioni, L., Borgianni, Y. & Basso, D. 2019. Value perception of green products: an exploratory study combining conscious answers and unconscious behavioral aspects. Sustainability 11 (5), 1226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccioni, L., Borgianni, Y. & Rotini, F. 2017. Sustainability as a value-adding concept in the early design phases? Insights from stimulated ideation sessions. In Sustainable Design and Manufacturing 2017, Smart Innovation, Systems and Technologies (eds. Campana, G., Howlett, R. J., Setchi, R. & Cimatti, B.), pp. 888897. Springer International Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maher, M. L. & Poon, J. 1996. Modeling design exploration as co-evolution. Computer-Aided Civil and Infrastructure Engineering 11 (3), 195209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milovanovic, J., Hu, M., Shealy, T. & Gero, J. 2020 Evolution of Brain Network Connectivity in the Prefrontal Cortex During Concept Generation Using Brainstorming for a Design Task. American Society of Mechanical Engineers Digital Collection.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McWhirter, N. & Shealy, T. 2020 Case-based flipped classroom approach to teach sustainable infrastructure and decision-making. International Journal of Construction Education and Research 16 (1), 323. doi:10.1080/15578771.2018.1487892CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, J. E. & Treagust, D. 2003. Engineering education, is problem-based or project-based learning the answer. Australasian Journal of Engineering Education 3, p216.Google Scholar
Moore, D. W., Bhadelia, R. A., Billings, R. L., Fulwiler, C., Heilman, K. M., Rood, K. M. J. & Gansler, D. A. 2009. Hemispheric connectivity and the visual–spatial divergent-thinking component of creativity. Brain and Cognition 70 (3), 267272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, R., Childs, P. & Hamilton, T. 2007 Sustainability by design: a reflection on the suitability of pedagogic practice in design and engineering courses in the teaching of sustainable design. European Journal of Engineering Education 32 (2), 135142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulder, K. F., Segalàs, J. & Ferrer‐Balas, D. 2012 How to educate engineers for/in sustainable development: ten years of discussion, remaining challenges. International Journal of Sustainability in Higher Education (ed. Svanström, M.) 13 (3), 211218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NAE. (2008) Changing the Conversation: Messages for Improving Public Understanding of Engineering. National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Naseer, N. & Hong, K.-S. 2015. fNIRS-based brain-computer interfaces: a review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.Google ScholarPubMed
National Science Foundation. 2009 Environmental sustainability, (March 1st 2009) http://www.nsf.gov/funding/pgm_summ.jsp?pims_id=501027&org=CBET.Google Scholar
Newstetter, W. C. & Michael McCracken, W. 2001 Chapter 4 – novice conceptions of design: implications for the design of learning environments. In Design Knowing and Learning: Cognition in Design Education, pp. 6377. Elsevier Science.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, T. A. & Zeng, Y. 2017 Effects of stress and effort on self-rated reports in experimental study of design activities. Journal of Intelligent Manufacturing 28 (7), 16091622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noland, R. B. 2001. Relationships between highway capacity and induced vehicle travel. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 35 (1), 4772.Google Scholar
Osborn, A. F. 1993 Applied Imagination: Principles and Procedures of Creative Problem-Solving 3rd Edition. Creative Education Foundation.Google Scholar
Pahl, G., Beitz, W., Feldhusen, J. & Grote, K. 2007 Engineering Design: A Systematic Approach. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulus, P. B., Kohn, N. W. & Arditti, L. E. 2011 Effects of quantity and quality instructions on brainstorming. The Journal of Creative Behavior 45 (1), 3846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pereira, T. 2009 Sustainability: an integral engineering design approach. Renewable and Sustainable Energy Reviews 13 (5), 11331137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petkar, H., Dande, S., Yadav, R., Zeng, Y. & Nguyen, T. A. 2010 A Pilot Study to Assess Designer’s Mental Stress Using Eye Gaze System and Electroencephalogram, pp. 899909. American Society of Mechanical Engineers Digital Collection.Google Scholar
Pieroni, M. P. P., McAloone, T. C. & Pigosso, D. C. A. 2019 Business model innovation for circular economy and sustainability: a review of approaches. Journal of Cleaner Production 215, 198216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinti, P., Aichelburg, C., Lind, F., Power, S., Swingler, E., Merla, A., Hamilton, A., Gilbert, S., Burgess, P. & Tachtsidis, I. 2015 Using fiberless, wearable fNIRS to monitor brain activity in real-world cognitive tasks. Journal of Visualized Experiments 106, e53336e53336.Google Scholar
Ranganath, C., Johnson, M. K. & D’Esposito, M. 2003 Prefrontal activity associated with working memory and episodic long-term memory. Neuropsychologia 41 (3), 378389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, D. S., Erickson, B., Kim, Y. E., Mirman, D., Hamilton, R. H. & Kounios, J. 2016 Anodal tDCS to right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex facilitates performance for novice jazz improvisers but hinders experts. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahar, A., Farb, N. & Shu, L. H. 2020 Mirroring neurostimulation outcomes through behavioral interventions to improve creative performance. In Proceedings of the ASME 2020 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference IDETC/CIE2020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salganik, L. H. & Rychen, D. S. 2003. Key Competencies for a Successful Life and a Well-functioning Society. Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
Schön, D. 1983 The Reflective Practitioner: How Professionals Think in Action. Temple Smith.Google Scholar
Schön, D. 1992 Designing as reflective conversation with the materials of a design situation. Research in Engineering Design 3 (3), 131147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schön, D. A. 1981 Learning a language, learning to design. In Architecture Education Study, Volume 1: The papers (eds. Porter, W. L. & Kilbridge, M. D., Andrew, W.). Mellon Foundation.Google Scholar
Schönbrodt, F. D. & Perugini, M. 2013 At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality 47 (5), 609612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seitamaa-Hakkarainen, P., Huotilainen, M., Mäkelä, M., Groth, C. & Hakkarainen, K. 2016 How can neuroscience help understand design and craft activity? The promise of cognitive neuroscience in design studies. FORMakademisk – forskningstidsskrift for design og designdidaktikk 9 (1), 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seitz, R. J., Nickel, J. & Azari, N. P. 2006 Functional modularity of the medial prefrontal cortex: involvement in human empathy. Neuropsychology, American Psychological Association, US 20 (6), 743751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, J. J., Smith, S. M. & Vargas-Hernandez, N. 2003 Metrics for measuring ideation effectiveness. Design Studies 24 (2), 111134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
She, J. & MacDonald, E. F. 2018 Exploring the effects of a product’s sustainability triggers on pro-environmental decision-making. Journal of Mechanical Design, American Society of Mechanical Engineers Digital Collection 140 (1), 011102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
She, J., Seepersad, C. C., Holtta-Otto, K. & MacDonald, E. F. 2018 Priming designers leads to prime designs. In Design Thinking Research: Making Distinctions: Collaboration versus Cooperation, Understanding Innovation (eds. Plattner, H., Meinel, C. & Leifer, L.), pp. 251273. Springer International Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shealy, T., Gero, J., Hu, M. & Milovanovic, J. 2020 Concept generation techniques change patterns of brain activation during engineering design. Design Science, p. 6. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shealy, T. & Gero, J. S. 2019 The neurocognition of three engineering concept generation techniques. Proceedings of the Design Society: International Conference on Engineering Design 1 (1), 18331842.Google Scholar
Shealy, T., Grohs, J. R., Hu, M., Maczka, D. K. & Panneton, R. 2017 Investigating design cognition during brainstorming tasks with freshmen and senior engineering students using functional near infrared spectroscopy. Proceedings of American Society for Engineering Education, American Society for Engineering Education.Google Scholar
Shealy, T., Hu, M. & Gero, J. 2018 Patterns of cortical activation when using concept generation techniques of brainstorming, morphological analysis, and TRIZ. ASME 2018 IDETC/CIE.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiu, E. (Ed.) 2014 Creativity Research: An Inter-Disciplinary and Multi-Disciplinary Research Handbook. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shrout, P. E. & Rodgers, J. L. 2018 Psychology, science, and knowledge construction: broadening perspectives from the replication crisis. Annual Review of Psychology 69 (1), 487510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siddiqui, S. V., Chatterjee, U., Kumar, D., Siddiqui, A. & Goyal, N. 2008. Neuropsychology of prefrontal cortex. Indian Journal of Psychiatry 50 (3), 202208.Google ScholarPubMed
Siller, T. J. 2001. Sustainability and critical thinking in civil engineering curriculum. Journal of Professional Issues in Engineering Education and Practice 127 (3), 104108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, H. A. 1973 The structure of ill structured problems. Artificial Intelligence 4 (3–4), 181201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smalley, R. 2003 Top ten problems of humanity for next 50 years. In Proceedings of Energy and Nano Technology Conference. May 3. Rice University.Google Scholar
Smith, G. F. 1998 Idea-generation techniques: a formulary of active ingredients. The Journal of Creative Behavior 32 (2), 107134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staniškis, J. K. & Katiliūtė, E. 2016 Complex evaluation of sustainability in engineering education: case & analysis. Journal of Cleaner Production 120, 1320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strait, M. & Scheutz, M. 2014 What we can and cannot (yet) do with functional near infrared spectroscopy. Frontiers in Neuroscience 8, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, L., Xiang, W., Chai, C., Wang, C. & Liu, Z. 2013 Impact of text on idea generation: an electroencephalography study. International Journal of Technology and Design Education 23 (4), 10471062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Someren, M. W., Barnard, Y. F. & Sandberg, J. A. C. 1994 The Think Aloud Method: A Practical Guide to Modelling Cognitive Processes. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wan Alwi, S. R., Manan, Z. A., Klemeš, J. J. & Huisingh, D. 2014 Sustainability engineering for the future. Journal of Cleaner Production, Special Volume: PSE Asia for Cleaner Production 71, 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, S., Rutgers, E., Daly, S., Jablokow, K. & Yilmaz, S. 2015 Exploring the effects of problem framing on solution shifts: a case study.Google Scholar
Yang, J. 2015 The influence of motor expertise on the brain activity of motor task performance: a meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 15 (2), 381394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yilmaz, S., Jablokow, K., Daly, S., Silk, E. & Berg, M. 2014 Investigating impacts on the ideation flexibility of engineers. In 2014 ASEE Annual Conference, American Society for Engineering Education.Google Scholar
Yücel, M. A., Selb, J., Copper, R. J. & Boas, D. A. 2014 Targeted principle component analysis: a new motion artifact correction approach for near-infrared spectroscopy. Journal of Innovative Optical Health Sciences 7 (2).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J. X., Leung, H.-C. & Johnson, M. K. 2003 Frontal activations associated with accessing and evaluating information in working memory: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 20 (3), 15311539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zmigrod, S., Colzato, L. S. & Hommel, B. 2015. Stimulating creativity: modulation of convergent and divergent thinking by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Creativity Research Journal 27 (4), 353360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Block design process.

Figure 1

Figure 2. fNIRS placement along the frontal cortex.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Average number of solutions generated by first-year students and seniors in the brainstorming tasks.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Higher cognitive activation in the dlPFC among first-year students engineering students when generating concepts.

Figure 4

Figure 5. Higher cognitive activation in the vlPFC among senior engineering students when generating concepts.

Figure 5

Figure 6. Higher cortical activation in the vlPFC and dlPFC among first-year and senior engineering students when including additional dimensions of sustainability.