Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-k7p5g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T11:25:06.087Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Two Views of Emotional Perception: Some Empirical Suggestions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2020

Get access

Extract

One stream in contemporary philosophical and psychological study of the emotions argues that they are perceptual capacities (e.g., Prinz 2004). For instance, Jesse Prinz has recently defended the view that emotions are perceptions of bodily changes and, via these, of “core relational themes” (2004, 224-25). Core relational themes are, roughly, relations an individual has to his/her environment that pertain to that individual's welfare (2004, 15-16). The present project is to compare and contrast two possible models of emotional perception. The central difference between these models is the notion of modularity that they use, and the corresponding overall view of the nature of the mind. I will suggest some empirical tests that might adjudicate between these different kinds of emotional modularity, and hence between these two models of emotional perception. I will conclude with some remarks about the extent of the relevance of this issue.

Type
3. The Analogy with Perception
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Batson, C. D., J. S., Coke, F., Chard, D., Smith, and A., Talisaferro. 1979. Generality of the ‘glow of goodwill': Effects of mood on helping and information acquisition. Social Psychology Quarterly 42(2): 176-79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blevins, G., and T., Murphy. 1974. Feeling good and helping: Further phone booth findings. Psychological Reports 34: 326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
H. C, Breiter, N. L., Etcoff, P. J., Whalen, W. A., Kennedy, S. L., Rauch, R. L., Buckner, M. M., Strauss, S. E., Hyman, and B. R., Rosen. 1996. Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron 17: 875-87.Google Scholar
R. J, Davidson, and W., Irwin. 1999. The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3(1): 1121.Google Scholar
de Sousa, R. 1987. The Rationality of Emotion. Cambridge (Mass.): MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Vignemont, F., and T., Singer. 2006. The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(10): 435-41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doris, J. 1998. Persons, situations, and virtue ethics. Nous 32(4): 504-30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doris, J. 2002. Lack of Character. Cambridge: Cambridge University.Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. 1983. The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge (Mass.): MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V., C., Keysers, and G., Rizzolatti. 2004. A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(9): 396403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, R. 1995. Sympathy, simulation, and the impartial spectator. Ethics 105(4): 727-42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harman, G. 1999. Moral philosophy meets social psychology. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 99: 315-32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurley, S. L. 1998. Consciousness in Action. Cambridge (Mass.): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1992. Beyond Modularity. Cambridge (Mass.): MIT Press.Google Scholar
Keysers, C., E., Kohler, M.A., Umilta, L., Nanetti, L., Fogassi, and V., Gallese. 2003. Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition. Experimental Brain Research 153: 628-36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, P., and A., Isen. 1975. Further studies on the effect of feeling good on helping. Sociometry 38: 141-47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, C. 2003. Social psychology and virtue ethics. Journal of Ethics 7: 365-92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, J. S., C. D., Frith, D. I., Perrett, D., Rowland, A. W., Young, A. J., Calder, R. J., Dolan. 1996. A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature 383: 812-15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
J. S, Morris, K. J., Friston, C., Buche!, C. D., Frith, A. W., Young, A. J., Calder, and R. J., Dolan. 1998. A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain 121: 4257.Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., S. A., Bunge, J. J., Gross, and J.D.E., Gabriele. 2002. Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14(8): 12151229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelps, E. 2004. The human amygdala and awareness: Interactions between emotion and cognition. In The Cognitive Neurosciences, III, ed. Gazzaniga, M., 10051016. Cambridge (Mass.): MIT Press.Google Scholar
M. L, Phillips, A. W., Young, C., Senior, M., Brammer, C., Andrew, A. J., Calder, E. T., Bullmore, D. I., Perrett, D., Rowland, S.C., Williams, J. A., Gray, and A. S., David. 1997. A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature 389: 495-98.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. 2004. Gut Reactions: A Perceptual Theory of Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University.Press.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., L., Fadiga, V., Gallese, and L., Fogassi. 1996. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research 3: 131-41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
G. E, Schafe, and J. E., LeDoux. 2004. The neural basis of fear. In The Cognitive Neurosciences, III, ed. Gazzaniga, M., 9871004. Cambridge (Mass.): MIT Press.Google Scholar
S. M, Schaefer, D. C., Jackson, R. J., Davidson, D. Y., Kimberg, and S. L., Thompson-Schill. 2002. Modulation of amygdalar activity by the conscious regulation of negative emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14(6): 913-21.Google Scholar
Singer, T. 2006. The neural basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 855-63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sneddon, A. Forthcoming. The depths and shallows of psychological externalism. Philosophical Studies. Published online on March 6, 2007: http: I I www.springerlink.com I content I 38k27p3010550173 IGoogle Scholar
Weyant, J., and R., Clark. 1977. Dimes and helping: The other side of the coin. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 3: 107-10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
P. J, Whalen, S. L., Rauch, N. L., Etcoff, S. C., Mcinerny, M. B., Lee, and M. A., Jenike. 1998. Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience 18: 411-18.Google Scholar
Wicker, B., C., Keysers, J., Plailly, J.-P., Royet, V., Gallese, and G., Rizzolatti. 2003. Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common.neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40: 655-64.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R. A. 2001. Two views of realization. Philosophical Studies 104(1): 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. A. 2004. Boundaries of the Mind: The Individual in the Fragile Sciences. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar