Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-06T07:54:16.346Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effectiveness of Association Splitting in Reducing Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts in a Nonclinical Sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 July 2012

Boris C. Rodríguez-Martín*
Affiliation:
Central University of Las Villas (UCLV), Cuba
Steffen Moritz
Affiliation:
University Medical Center, Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany
Osana Molerio-Pérez
Affiliation:
Central University of Las Villas (UCLV), Cuba
Patricia Gil-Pérez
Affiliation:
Central University of Las Villas (UCLV), Cuba
*
Reprint requests to Boris C. Rodríguez-Martín, Faculty of Psychology, Central University “Marta Abreu” of Las Villas (UCLV), Carretera de Camajuaní Km 5½, Santa Clara, Villa Clara, CubaCP: 54830. E-mail: borisc@uclv.edu.cu

Abstract

Background: Association Splitting (AS) is a novel cognitive technique, which has shown some promise for the reduction of obsessive thoughts in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Its effect on unwanted intrusive thoughts (UITs) in the general population is yet unknown. Method: Our experimental study tested the effect of AS in 49 participants who reported UITs as a regular problem. Participants were randomly allocated either to an AS versus waitlist control (WL) condition. The White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI) was used for measuring changes over time. Results: Significant group differences were found across time. Relative to WL, AS exerted a positive effect on the reduction of UITs yielding a large effect size. According to self-report AS exerted a positive effect 6 days after the participants had initiated the exercises. All subjects in the AS condition judged the technique as effective Conclusion: Results suggest that AS could be a suitable intervention to help people with a potential vulnerability to a clinical problem to control their UITs.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aldao, A. and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: a transdiagnostic examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 974983.Google Scholar
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S. and Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belloch, A., Morillo, C. and García-Soriano, G. (2009). Strategies to control unwanted intrusive thoughts: which are relevant and specific in obsessive-compulsive disorder? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33, 7589.Google Scholar
Belloch, A., Morillo, C., Luciano, J. V., García-Soriano, G., Cabedo, E. and Carrió, C. (2010). Dysfunctional belief domains related to obsessive-compulsive disorder: a further examination of their dimensionality and specificity. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13, 376388.Google Scholar
Berry, L-M., May, J., Andrade, J. and Kavanagh, D. (2010). Emotional and behavioural reaction to intrusive thoughts. Assessment, 17, 126137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabedo, E., Belloch, A., Morillo, C., Jiménez, A. and Carrió, C. (2004). Intensidad de las creencias disfuncionales en relación con el incremento de la obsesividad. Internacional Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 4, 465479.Google Scholar
Davies, M. I. and Clark, D. M. (1998). Predictors of analogue post-traumatic intrusive cognitions. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 26, 303314.Google Scholar
Ehlers, A. and Steil, R. (1995). Maintenance of intrusive memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: a cognitive approach. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 217249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Soriano, G. (2008). Pensamientos intrusos obsesivos y obsesiones clínicas: contenidos y significado personal. Universitat de València: Servei de Publications. [Doctoral Thesis]Google Scholar
Hazlett-Stevens, H. and Graske, M. G. (2003). The catastrophizing worry process in generalized anxiety disorder: a preliminary investigation of an analog population. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 31, 387401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyland, M. E. (2011). Motivation and placebos: do different mechanisms occur in different contexts? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 18281837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iijima, Y. and Tanno, Y. (in press). The rebound effect in the unsuccessful suppression of worrisome thoughts. Personality and Individual Differences. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.03.023Google Scholar
Luciano, J. V., Belloch, A., Algarabel, S., Tomás, J. M., Morillo, C. and Lucero, M. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis of the White Bear Suppression Inventory and the Thought Control Questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22, 250258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magee, J. C., Harden, K. P. and Teachman, B. A. (2012). Psychopathology and thought suppression: a quantitative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 189201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marino-Carper, T., Negy, C., Burns, G. and Lunt, R. A. (2010). The effects of psychoeducation on thought-action fusion, thought suppression, and responsibility. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 289296.Google Scholar
Markowitz, L. J. and Borton, J. L. S. (2002). Suppression of negative self-referent and neutral thoughts: a preliminary investigation. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30, 271277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masuda, A., Feinstein, A. B., Wendell, J. W. and Sheehan, S. T. (2010). Cognitive Defusion versus Thoughts Distraction: a clinical rationale training and experiential excercise in altering psychological impact of negative self-referential thoughts. Behaviour Modification, 34, 520538.Google Scholar
Masuda, A., Hayes, S. C., Twohig, M. P., Drossel, C., Lillis, J. and Washio, Y. (2009). A parametric study of Cognitive Defusion and the believability and discomfort of negative self-relevant thoughts. Behavior Modification, 33, 250256.Google Scholar
Moritz, S. and Jelinek, L. (2006). Association Splitting: self-help guide to reduce obsessive thoughts. Available from http://www.uke.de/assoziationsspaltungGoogle Scholar
Moritz, S. and Jelinek, L. (2011). Further evidence for the efficacy of Association Splitting as a self-help technique for reducing obsessive thoughts. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 574581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S., Jelinek, L., Klinge, R. and Naber, D. (2007). Fight fire with fireflies! Association Splitting: a novel cognitive technique to reduce obsessive thoughts. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 37, 15.Google Scholar
Moritz, S. and Pohl, R. F. (2009). Biased processing of threat-related information rather than knowledge deficits contributes to overestimation of threat in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Modification, 33, 763777.Google Scholar
Moritz, S., Wahl, K., Ertle, A, Jelinek, L., Hauschildt, M., Klinge, K. and Hand, I. (2009). Neither saints nor wolves in disguise: ambivalent interpersonal attitudes and behaviors in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Modification, 33, 274292.Google Scholar
Najmi, S., Reese, H., Wilhelm, S., Fama, J., Beck, C. and Wegner, D. M. (2010). Learning the futility of the thought suppression enterprise in normal experience and in obsessive compulsive disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 38, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Najmi, S., Riemann, B. C. and Wegner, D. M. (2009). Managing unwanted intrusive thoughts in obsessive-compulsive disorder: relative effectiveness of suppression, focused distraction, and acceptance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 494503.Google Scholar
Petrie, K. J., Booth, R. J. and Pennebaker, J. W. (1998). The immunological effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychologist, 75, 12641272.Google Scholar
Rodríguez-Martín, B. C. and Molerio-Pérez, O. (2011). Disociación de Asociaciones: una guía de autoayuda para reducir la presencia de los pensamientos intrusos en la vida cotidiana. Available from http://www.uke.de/assoziationsspaltungGoogle Scholar
Sink, C. A. and Mvududu, N. H. (2010). Statistical power, sampling, and effect sizes: three keys to research relevancy. Counseling Outcome Research and Evaluation, 1, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, C. and Purdon, C. (2008). Attention training in the reduction and reappraisal of intrusive thoughts. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36, 6170.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 3452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M. (1997). When the antidote is the poison. Ironic mental control processes. Psychological Science, 8, 148150.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M. and Erskine, J. A. K. (2003). Voluntary involuntariness: thought suppression and the regulation of the experience of will. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 684694.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M. and Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychologist, 6, 615640.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.