Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-m9kch Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-21T08:57:30.827Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Primates unleashed

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2023

Federica Amici
Affiliation:
Life Sciences, Institute of Biology, Leipzig University, 04103 Leipzig, Germany. amici@eva.mpg.de https://www.eva.mpg.de/pbe/people/federica-amici/index.html Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany. katja.liebal@uni-leipzig.de https://www.lw.uni-leipzig.de/institut-fuer-biologie/abteilungen/ag-humanbiologie-primatenkognition/team/katja-liebal
Katja Liebal
Affiliation:
Life Sciences, Institute of Biology, Leipzig University, 04103 Leipzig, Germany. amici@eva.mpg.de https://www.eva.mpg.de/pbe/people/federica-amici/index.html Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany. katja.liebal@uni-leipzig.de https://www.lw.uni-leipzig.de/institut-fuer-biologie/abteilungen/ag-humanbiologie-primatenkognition/team/katja-liebal

Abstract

Before claiming major differences between the communication systems of humans and other species, it is necessary to (1) overcome methodological limitations in the comparative study of communicative intentions; (2) account for mechanisms other than epistemic vigilance that may also sustain complex forms of communication; and (3) better differentiate between motivational and cognitive factors potentially affecting the emergence of open-ended communication.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amici, F., Oña, L., & Liebal, K. (2022). Compositionality in primate gestural communication and multimodal signal displays. International Journal of Primatology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-022-00316-9Google Scholar
Arnold, K., & Zuberbühler, K. (2006). Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441, 303.Google Scholar
Arnold, K., & Zuberbühler, K. (2008). Meaningful call combinations in a non-human primate. Current Biology, 18, 202203.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., & Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 142146.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354, 110114.Google Scholar
Liebal, K., Slocombe, K., & Waller, B. M. (2022). The language void 10 years on: Multimodal primate communication research is still uncommon. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 34, 274287.Google Scholar
Suzuki, T. N., Wheatcroft, D., & Griesser, M. (2016). Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls. Nature Communications, 7, 10986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, S. W., Koski, S. E., Byrne, R. W., Slocombe, K. E., Bickel, B., Boeckle, M., … Manser, M. B. (2017). Exorcising Grice's ghost: An empirical approach to studying intentional communication in animals. Biological Reviews, 92, 14271433.Google Scholar