Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-qks25 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-30T03:15:27.116Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessing Prelinguistic Communication in Children with Developmental Delay: Effects of Contextual Structure

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2016

Anna Bortoli*
Affiliation:
The University of Melbourne
P. Margaret Brown
Affiliation:
The University of Melbourne
*
Address for: Anna Bortoli, Early Intervention Unit, Department of Learning and Educational Development, Faculty of Education, The University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010 Australia. Phone: 61 3 8344 0980 Fax: 61 3 8344 0995 E-mail: a.bortoli@edfac.unimelb.edu.au

Abstract

This study compared the prelinguistic communicative behaviors of a boy with developmental delay (aged three years and three months) during structured and low structured interactions with his teacher. Using a videotaped analysis procedure developed by Tait and Wood (1987) four variables (eye contact to the speaker, eye contact to the referent, initiation and turntaking) were investigated. The analysis showed that, in the low structured condition, there was a greater degree of engagement, as measured by increased eye contact overall. More particularly, in this condition there was a greater percentage of eye contact to the referent. In addition to this, the participant took a greater percentage of the conversational turns offered to him by the adult in the low structured condition, although there was no difference between the types of turns taken (vocal, gestural and eye contact). The implications of these results for assessment and intervention are discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Australian Association of Special Education 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Development of language use in relation to content and form. In Bloom, L., & Lahey, M., (Eds). Language development and language and language disorders. Toronto: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Buzolich, M., & Wiemann, J. (1988). Turntaking in atypical conversations: The case of the speaker/augmented—communicator dyad. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coggins, T., Olswang, L., & Guthrie, J. (1987). Assessing communicative intents in young children: Low structured observations or elicitation tasks? Journal of Special and Hearing Disorders, 52, 4449.Google Scholar
Cook, R., Tessier, A., & Klein, M. (1996). Adapting early childhood curricula for children in inclusive settings. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Cunningham, C., Reuler, E., Blackwell, J., & Deck, J. (1981). Behavioural and linguistic developments in the interactions of normal and retarded children with their mothers. Child Development, 52, 6270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fewell, R. (1991). Trends in the assessment of infants and toddlers with disabilities. Exceptional Children, 58(2), 166174.Google Scholar
Fey, M. (1986). Language disorders and language development. Boston MA: College-Hill.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L. (1988). Developing dialogue skills: The efffects of a conversational model of language intervention. In Marfo, K. (ed.), Parent-child interaction and developmental disabilities. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Greenspan, S., & Meisels, S. (1994). Toward a new vision for the developmental assessment of infants and young children. Zero to Three, 14, 6, 18.Google Scholar
Guralnick, M. (1990). Peer interactions and the development of handicapped children’s social and communicative competence. In Foot, H., Morgan, M., & Shutel, R., (Eds.). Children helping children, (pp. 275305). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Kaye, K., & Charney, R. (1981). Conversational asymmetry between mothers and children. Journal of Child Language, 8, 3549.Google Scholar
Hecht, F., Levine, H., & Mastergeorge, B. (1993). Conversational roles of children with developmental delays and their mothers in natural and semi-structured situations. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97(4), 419429.Google Scholar
Holdgrafer, G., & Dunst, C. (1990). Use of low structured observation for assessing communicative intents in young children. First Language, 10, 243253.Google Scholar
Lahey, P. (1988). Language disorders and language development. New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
Lerner, J. (1997). Learning disability: Theories, diagnosis and teaching strategies. Boston MA: Houghton Mifflin Co.Google Scholar
MacDonald, J. (1985). Language through conversation: A model for intervention with language-delayed persons. In Warren, S.F. & Rogers-Warren, A.K., (Eds.), Teaching functional language: Generalisation and maintenance of language skills, (pp. 89122). Austin, TX: Pro-ed.Google Scholar
McCollum, J., & Stayton, V. (1988). Gaze patterns of mothers and infants as indicators of role integration during play and teaching with toys. In Marfo, K. (ed.), Parent-child interaction and developmental disabilities. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Owens, R. (1999). Language disorders. A functional approach to assessment and Intervention. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Rice, M., Sell, M., & Hadley, P. (1991). Social interaction of speech and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 12991307.Google Scholar
Ryba, K. (1998). Dynamic assessment and program planning for students with intellectual disability. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 27(2), 314.Google Scholar
Tait, D., & Wood, D. (1987). From communication to speech in deaf children. Child Language, Teaching and Therapy, 3(1), 117.Google Scholar
Tannock, R. (1988). Control and reciprocity in mothers’ interaction with down syndrome and normal children. In Marfo, K. (ed.), Parent-child interaction and developmental disability. New York: Praegar.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 14541463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoder, P., & Davies, B. (1990). Do parental questions and topic continuation elicit replies from developmentally delayed children? A sequential analysis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 363373.Google Scholar