Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T06:56:50.170Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Tell me a story in English or Swedish: Narrative production and comprehension in bilingual preschoolers and first graders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 December 2015

UTE BOHNACKER*
Affiliation:
Uppsala University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Ute Bohnacker, Department of Linguistics and Philology, Uppsala University, Box 635, Uppsala SE-75126, Sweden. E-mail: ute.bohnacker@lingfil.uu.se

Abstract

This study examines macrostructural aspects of narrative skills in 52 bilingual Swedish- and English-speaking children age 5–7. Elicited fictional story production and comprehension tasks were administered in parallel fashion in both Swedish and English (Multilingual Assessment Instrument for Narratives; Gagarina et al., 2012). Scores on the Multilingual Assessment Instrument for Narratives were compared across languages; moreover, story structure components in the narratives and answers to probe questions were qualitatively analyzed. Age effects (5-year-olds vs. 6- to 7-year-olds) for macrostructure production and narrative comprehension were evident, but no effect for language (Swedish/English). The results suggest that story structure is invariant across a bilingual child's two languages at a given age, with similar awareness of the intentions and goal-directed behavior of the story protagonists, irrespective of language.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Akinci, M.-A., Jisa, H., & Kern, S. (2001). Influence of L1 Turkish on L2 French narratives. In Verhoeven, L. & Strömqvist, S. (Eds.), Narrative development in a multilingual context (pp. 189208). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A. (2001). Narrative development in multilingual contexts: A cross-linguistic perspective. In Verhoeven, L. & Strömqvist, S. (Eds.), Narrative development in a multilingual context (pp. 419428). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Narrative structure. In Berman, R. A. & Slobin, D. I. (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: A cross-linguistic developmental study (pp. 3984). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fiestas, C. E., & Peña, E. D. (2004). Narrative discourse in bilingual children: Language and task effects. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 155168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Kunnari, S., Tantele, K., Välimaa, T., Balčiūnienė, I., et al. (2012). Multilingual Assessment Instrument for Narratives (MAIN). ZAS papers in linguistics 56. Berlin: ZAS. Retrieved from http://www.zas.gwz-berlin.de/zaspil56.htmlGoogle Scholar
Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Kunnari, S., Tantele, K., Välimaa, T., Balčiūnienė, I., et al. (2015). Assessment of narrative abilities in bilingual children. In Armon-Lotem, S., de Jong, J., & Meir, N. (Eds.), Assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 243276). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Tsimpli, I. M., & Walters, J. (2016). Narrative abilities in bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 11–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heilmann, J., Miller, J. F., & Nockerts, A. (2010). Sensitivity of narrative organization measures using narrative retells produced by young school-age children. Language Testing, 27, 603626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M. (1991). The development of discourse cohesion: Some functional and cross-linguistic issues. In Piérait-Le Bonniec, G. & Dolitsky, M. (Eds.), Language bases . . . discourse bases: Some aspects of contemporary French-language psycholinguistics research (pp. 157185). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M., & Schneider, P. (2000). Cohesion and coherence anomalies and their effects on children's referent introduction in narrative retell. In Perkins, M. & Howard, S. (Eds.), New directions in language development and disorders (pp. 251261). New York: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, D., McGillvray, L., & Schmidek, M. (1997). Guide to narrative language: Procedures for assessments. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications.Google Scholar
Iluz-Cohen, P., & Walters, J. (2012). Telling stories in two languages: Narratives of bilingual preschool children with typical and impaired language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 5874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Justice, L. M., Bowles, R. P., Kaderavek, J. N., Ukrainetz, T. A., EisenbergS. L,. S. L,. & Gillam, R. B. (2006). The Index of Narrative Microstructure: A clinical tool for analyzing school-age children's narrative performances. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 15, 177191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupersmitt, J., & Berman, R. A. (2001). Linguistic features of Spanish–Hebrew children's narratives. In Verhoeven, L. & Strömqvist, S. (Eds.), Narrative development in a multilingual context (pp. 277371). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lanza, E. (2001). Temporality and language contact in narratives by children bilingual in Norwegian and English. In Verhoeven, L. & Strömqvist, S. (Eds.), Narrative development in a multilingual context (pp. 1550). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk: Vol. 1. Transcription format and programs (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Books.Google Scholar
Möller, C. (2010). Coherence and cohesion in early immersion students’ L2 narratives: Implications for cognitive and linguistic development. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Universität Paderborn.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A., & Richner, E. R. (2007). From actors to agents to persons: The development of character representation in young children's narratives. Child Development, 78, 412429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J., Genesee, F., & Crago, M. B. (2011). Dual language development and disorders (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul. H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Paradis, J., & Schneider, P. (2008). Distinguishing bilingual children from monolinguals with SLI: Profile effects on the ENNI. Poster presented at the Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders, University of Wisconsin.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z. (2002). Narrative competence among monolingual and bilingual school children in Miami. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.), Child language and child development: Vol. 2. Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 135174). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, P., & Hayward, D. (2010). Who does what to whom: Introduction of referents in children's storytelling from pictures. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 459473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, M., & Shaul, Y. (2013). Narrative development among language-minority children: The role of bilingual versus monolingual preschool education. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 26, 3651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, L. R., & Hudson, J. A. (1991). Tell me a make-believe story: Coherence and cohesion in young children's picture-elicited narratives. Developmental Psychology, 27, 960974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soodla, P. (2011). Picture-elicited narratives of Estonian children at the kindergarten school: Transition as a measure of language competence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Tartu University.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L., & Albro, E. R. (1997). Building complexity and coherence: Children's use of goal-structured knowledge in telling stories. In Bamberg, M. (Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches (pp. 544). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. O. (Ed.), Advances in discourse processing: Vol. 2. New directions in discourse processing (pp. 53120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L., & Policastro, M. (1984). The concept of a story: A comparison between children's and teacher's viewpoints. In Mandl, H., Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text (pp. 113155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T., & Nickels, M. (1992). The development of goal plans of action in the narration of a picture story. Discourse Processes, 15, 249275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., & Rodkin, P. C. (1994). Knowledge of goal/plans: A conceptual basis for narrating “Frog where are you?” In Berman, R. A. & Slobin, D. I. (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: A cross-linguistic developmental study (pp. 85106). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T., & Stein, N. L. (1994). Using goal–plan knowledge to merge the past with the present and the future in narrating events on line. In Haith, M. M., Benson, J. B., Roberts, R. J. Jr., & Pennington, B. F. (Eds.), The development of future-oriented processes (pp. 323349). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Viberg, Å. (2001). Age-related and L2-related features in bilingual narrative development in Sweden. In Verhoeven, L. & Strömqvist, S. (Eds.), Narrative development in a multilingual context (pp. 87128). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westby, C. E. (2012). Assessing and remediating text comprehension problems. In Kamhi, A. G. & Catts, H. W. (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (3rd ed., pp. 163225). Boston: Pearson.Google Scholar