Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T09:25:26.077Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Short-term memory problems for phonemes’ serial order in adults with dyslexia: Evidence from a different analysis of the Nonword repetition task

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2019

Kirsten Schraeyen*
Affiliation:
Thomas More University of Applied Sciences University of Leuven University of Antwerp
Wim Van der Elst
Affiliation:
Hasselt University
Astrid Geudens
Affiliation:
Thomas More University of Applied Sciences
Pol Ghesquière
Affiliation:
University of Leuven
Dominiek Sandra
Affiliation:
University of Antwerp
*
*Corresponding author. Email: kirsten.schraeyen@thomasmore.be

Abstract

Many studies show that poor readers make more errors in nonword repetition than better readers. Although this finding is generally linked to the lower quality of poor readers’ phonological representations in verbal short-term memory, the nature of this poor performance remains unclear. We addressed this issue by focusing on two types of phoneme-related performance in a nonword repetition task: (a) recall of phonemes irrespective of their serial order (phoneme identity) and (b) recall of correctly reproduced phonemes’ serial order (serial order). We tested 91 young adults with and without dyslexia. Generalized linear mixed-effects models demonstrated that controls outperformed individuals with dyslexia in the recall of phonemes’ serial order but failed to detect a difference in the recall of phonemes’ identity. These findings are discussed not only in terms of the nature of or access to phonological representations but also in terms of another concept that has recently been advanced in the literature: a specialized serial order mechanism in verbal short-term memory. We also consider the possibility that individuals with dyslexia may be less sensitive to phonotactic constraints.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2019. 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Attout, L., & Majerus, S. (2015). Working memory deficits in developmental dyscalculia: The importance of serial order. Child Neuropsychology, 21, 432450. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.922170 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baird, G., Slonims, V., Simonoff, E., & Dworzynski, K. (2011). Impairment in non-word repetition: A marker for language impairment or reading impairment? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 53, 711716. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.03936 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., & Dai, B. (2008). lme4: Linear mixed effects models using S4 classes (R Package Version 0.999375-25). [Software]. Retrieved from http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html Google Scholar
Boada, R., & Pennington, B. F. (2006). Deficient implicit phonological representations in children with dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 153193. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.04.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boets, B., Op de Beeck, H. P., Vandermosten, M., Scott, S. K., Gillebert, C. R., Mantini, D.Ghesquière, P. (2013). Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science, 342, 12511254. doi: 10.1126/science.1244333 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogaerts, L., Szmalec, A., Hachmann, W. M., Page, M. P. A., & Duyck, W. (2015). Linking memory and language: Evidence for a serial-order learning impairment in dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 43–44, 106122. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.06.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonte, M. L., Poelmans, H., & Blomert, L. (2007). Deviant neurophysiological responses to phonological regularities in speech in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia, 45, 14271437. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booij, G. (1995). The phonology of Dutch. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Language and phonological skills in children at high-risk of reading difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 631640. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00252 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91, 77111. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00164-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 335359. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80014-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N., Hogan, T., Green, S., Cabbage, K., Brinkley, S., & Gray, S. (2017). Short-term memory in childhood dyslexia: Deficient serial order in multiple modalities. Dyslexia, 23, 209233. doi: 10.1002/dys.1557 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Smedt, B. (2006). Mathematical disabilities in children with velo-cardio-facial syndrome: a further delineation of the cognitive phenotype (Unpublished PhD thesis, KU Leuven, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences).Google Scholar
De Smedt, B., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2004, April). Working memory and arithmetic abilities in seven and eleven year old children. Poster presented at the Second Working Memory Symposium. Beaune, France.Google Scholar
Dietrich, J. A., & Brady, S. A. (2001). Phonological representations of adult poor readers: An investigation of specificity and stability. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 383418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbro, C. (1996). Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 8, 453485. doi: 10.1007/BF00577023 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbro, C. (1998). When reading is “readn” or somthn. Distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items in normal and disabled readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39, 149153. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.393070 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elbro, C., Borstrøm, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten. The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 3660. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.33.1.3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbro, C., & Jensen, M. N. (2005). Quality of phonological representations, word learning, and phoneme awareness in dyslexic and normal readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 375384. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2005.00468 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. (2001). Does strength of phonological representations predict phonological awareness? Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 301325. doi: 10.1017/S0142716401003022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., & Emslie, H. (1994). The children’s test of nonword repetition: A test of phonological working memory. Memory, 2, 103127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geudens, A. (2006). Phonological awareness and learning to read a first language: Controversies and new perspectives. LOT Occasional Series, 6, 2543.Google Scholar
Geudens, A., & Sandra, D. (2003). Beyond implicit phonological knowledge: No support for an onset–rime structure in children’s explicit phonological awareness. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 157182. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00036-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gupta, P. (2003). Examining the relationship between word learning, nonword repetition, and immediate serial recall in adults. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 12131236. doi: 10.1080/02724980343000071 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gupta, P. (2009). A computational model of nonword repetition, immediate serial recall, and nonword learning. In Thorn, A. & Page, M. (Eds.), Interactions between short-term and long-term memory in the verbal domain (pp. 108135). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Gupta, P., Lipinski, J., Abbs, B., & Lin, P.-H. (2005). Serial position effects in nonword repetition. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 141162. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.12.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hachmann, W. M., Bogaerts, L., Szmalec, A., Woumans, E., Duyck, W., & Job, R. (2014). Short-term memory for order but not for item information is impaired in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 64, 121136. doi: 10.1007/s11881-013-0089-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaroslawska, A. J., Gathercole, S. E., Logie, M. R., & Holmes, J. (2016). Following instructions in a virtual school: Does working memory play a role? Memory & Cognition, 44, 580589. doi: 10.3758/s13421-015-0579-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (1994). Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630645. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laasonen, M., Virsu, V., Oinonen, S., Sandbacka, M., Salakari, A., & Service, E. (2012). Phonological and sensory short-term memory are correlates and both affected in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 25, 22472273. doi: 10.1007/s11145-011-9356-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majerus, S., & Cowan, N. (2016). The nature of verbal short-term impairment in dyslexia: The importance of serial order. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1522. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01522 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Greffe, C., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Relations between vocabulary development and verbal short-term memory: The relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and item information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 95119. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.07.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martens, V. E., & de Jong, P. F. (2006). The effect of word length on lexical decision in dyslexic and normal reading children. Brain and Language, 98, 140149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez Perez, T., Majerus, S., Mahot, A., & Poncelet, M. (2012). Evidence for a specific impairment of serial order short-term memory in dyslexic children. Dyslexia, 18, 94109. doi: 10.1002/dys.1438 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez Perez, T., Majerus, S., & Poncelet, M. (2012). The contribution of short-term memory for serial order to early reading acquisition: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 708723. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.11.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez Perez, T., Majerus, S., & Poncelet, M. (2013). Impaired short-term memory for order in adults with dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 22112233. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.04.005 Google Scholar
McDougall, S., & Hulme, C. (1994). Short-term memory, speech rate and phonological awareness as predictors of learning to read. In Hulme, C. & Snowling, M. (Eds.), Reading development and dyslexia (pp. 3144). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
McKelvie, S. J. (1987). Learning and awareness in the Hebb digits task. Journal of General Psychology, 114, 7588. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1987.9711057 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molenberghs, G., & Verbeke, G. (2005). Models for discrete longitudinal data. Springer Series in Statistics. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Munson, B., Swenson, C. L., & Manthei, S. C. (2005). Lexical and phonological organization in children: Evidence from repetition tasks. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 108123. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/009)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nairne, J., & Kelley, M. (2004). Separating item and order information through process dissociation. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 113133. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.09.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nation, K., & Hulme, C. (2011). Learning to read changes children’s phonological skills: Evidence from a latent variable longitudinal study of reading and nonword repetition. Developmental Science, 14, 649659. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212218. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00035-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841865. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg076 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., & Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 129141. doi: 10.1080/17470210701508822 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rispens, J., & Parigger, E. (2010). Non-word repetition in Dutch-speaking children with specific language impairment with and without reading problems. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 177188. doi: 10.1348/026151009X482633 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, E. K., & Joanisse, M. F. (2010). Spoken sentence comprehension in children with dyslexia and language impairment: The roles of syntax and working memory. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 141165. doi: 10.1017/S0142716409990208 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheltinga, F. (2003). The Dutch nonword repetition test (Unpublished manuscript, University of Amsterdam).Google Scholar
Schraeyen, K., Geudens, A., Ghesquière, P., Van der Elst, W., & Sandra, D. (2017). Poor performance on the retention of phonemes’ serial order in short-term memory reflects young children’s poor reading skills. Mental Lexicon, 12, 129158. doi: 10.1075/ML.12.1.06SCH CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google ScholarPubMed
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2006). Reading acquisition and developmental dyslexia. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Staels, E., & Van den Broeck, W. (2014). Order short-term memory is not impaired in dyslexia and does not affect orthographic learning. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 116. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00732 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 13211337. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/103) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szenkovits, G., Darma, Q., Darcy, I., & Ramus, F. (2016). Exploring dyslexics’ phonological deficit II: Phonological grammar. First Language, 36, 316337. doi: 10.1177/0142723716648841 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szmalec, A., Loncke, M., Page, M., & Duyck, W. (2011). Order or dis-order? Impaired Hebb learning in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 37, 12701279. doi: 10.1037/a0023820 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgesen, J. K. (1985). Memory processes in reading disabled children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 18, 350357. doi: 10.1177/002221948501800608 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Broeck, W., & Geudens, A. (2012). Old and new ways to study characteristics of reading disability: The case of the nonword-reading deficit. Cognitive Psychology, 65, 414456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychiatry, 45, 240. doi: 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbeke, G., & Molenberghs, G. (2000). Linear mixed models for longitudinal data. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar