Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T10:12:30.126Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How does language proficiency affect children’s iconic gesture use?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 January 2019

MEGHAN ZVAIGZNE
Affiliation:
McGill University
YURIKO OSHIMA-TAKANE*
Affiliation:
McGill University and University of Victoria
MAKIKO HIRAKAWA
Affiliation:
Chuo University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Yuriko Oshima-Takane, Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, P. O. Box 1700 STN CSC, V8W 2Y2, Canada. E-mail: yuriko.oshima-takane@mcgill.ca

Abstract

Previous research investigating the relationship between language proficiency and iconic gesture use has produced inconsistent findings. This study investigated whether a linear relationship was assumed although it is a quadratic relationship. Iconic co-speech gesture use by 4- to 6-year-old French–Japanese bilinguals with two levels of French proficiency (intermediate and low) but similar levels of Japanese proficiency was compared with that of high-proficiency French monolinguals (Study 1) and Japanese monolinguals with similar proficiency to the bilinguals (Study 2). To control the information participants communicated, a dynamic referential communication task was used; a difference between two cartoons had to be communicated to an experimenter. Study 1 showed a significant quadratic relationship between proficiency and iconic gesture use in French; the intermediate-proficiency bilinguals gestured least among the three proficiency groups. The monolingual and bilingual groups with similar Japanese proficiency in Study 2 gestured at similar rates. It is suggested that children gestured for different reasons depending on their language proficiency and the cognitive resources available for the task.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abdalla, F. (2015). Language dominance and gesture production in Arabic-English bilingual children. Paper presented at the International Conference on Bilingualism, Malta.Google Scholar
Academic Therapy Publications. (2000). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (2nd ed.). Novato: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. J. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 593613.Google Scholar
Colletta, J.-M., Pellenq, C., & Guidetti, M. (2010). Age-related changes in co-speech gesture and narrative: Evidence from French children and adults. Speech Communication, 52, 565576.Google Scholar
Crossley, S. A., Salsbury, T., McNamara, D. S., & Jarvis, S. (2010). Predicting lexical proficiency in language learner texts using computational indices. Language Testing, 28, 561580.Google Scholar
Efron, D. (1972). Gesture, race and culture; a tentative study of the spatio-temporal and “linguistic” aspects of the gestural behavior of eastern Jews and southern Italians in New York City, living under similar as well as different environmental conditions. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Ferguson, G. A., & Takane, Y. (1989). Multiple-comparison procedures. In G. A. Ferguson & Y. Takane (Eds.), Statistical analysis in psychology and education (6th ed., pp. 321329). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Frick-Horbury, D., & Guttentag, R. E. (1998). The effects of restricting hand gesture production on lexical retrieval and free recall. American Journal of Psychology, 111, 4362.Google Scholar
Gershkoff-Stowe, L., & Thelen, E. (2004). U-shaped changes in behavior: A dynamic systems perspective. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 1136.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787814.Google Scholar
Gregersen, T., Olivares-Cuhat, G., & Storm, J. (2009). An examination of L1 and L2 gesture use: What role does proficiency play? Modern Language Journal, 93, 195208.Google Scholar
Groupe Coopératif en orthophonie région Laval-Laurentides-Lanaudière. (1995). Épreuve de compréhension de Carrow-Woolfolk et Épreuve de dénomination EO-WPVT-R: Adaptation et normalisation. Unpublished manuscript, Montréal.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M. (1998). Gesture as a communication strategy in second language discourse. A study of Learners of French and Swedish. Lund: Lund University Press.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M. (1999). Communication strategies, gestures, and grammar. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère, 2, 6171.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M. (2012). Bilingualism and gesture. In T. K. Bhatia & W. C. Ritchie (Eds.), The handbook of bilingualism (2nd ed., pp. 417437). New York: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hadar, U., Dar, R., & Teitelman, A. (2001). Gesture during speech in first and second language. Implications for lexical retrieval. Gesture, 1, 151165.Google Scholar
Holler, J., & Beattie, G. (2003). Pragmatic aspects of representational gestures. Do speakers use them to clarify verbal ambiguity for the listener? Gesture, 3, 127154.Google Scholar
Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 297315.Google Scholar
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2007). Raise your hand if you’re spatial: Relations between verbal and spatial skills and gesture production. Gesture, 7, 7395.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In M. R. Key (Ed.), The Relationship of verbal and nonverbal communication (pp. 207227). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1994). Do gestures communicate? A review. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 27, 175200.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2001). Gesture as communication strategy. Semiotica, 135, 191209.Google Scholar
Kidd, E., & Holler, J. (2009). Children’s use of gesture to resolve lexical ambiguity. Developmental Science, 12, 903913.Google Scholar
Kita, S. (2000). How representational gestures help speaking. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 162185). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kita, S., & Özyurek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 1632.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. M., & Hadar, U. (1999). The role of speech-related arm/hand gestures in word retrieval. In L. S. Messing & R. Campbell (Eds.), Gesture, speech, and sign (pp. 93116). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analysis talk. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Marcos, L. R. (1979). Nonverbal behavior and thought processing. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36, 940943.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R., & Nicoladis, E. (2000). Gesture reflects language development: Evidence from bilingual children. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 192196.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D., & Duncan, S. D. (2000). Growthpoints in thinking-for-speaking. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 141161). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nagpal, J., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P. (2011). Predicting individual differences in L2 speakers’ gestures. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 205214.Google Scholar
Namy, L. L., Campbell, A. L., & Tomasello, M. (2004). The changing role of iconicity in non-verbal symbol learning: A u-shaped trajectory in the acquisition of arbitrary gestures. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 3757.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E. (2002). Some gestures develop in conjunction with spoken language development and others don’t: Evidence from bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 26, 241266.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E. (2007). The effect of bilingualism on the use of manual gestures. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 441454.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Mayberry, R. I., & Genesee, F. (1999). Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 514526.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Pika, S., & Marentette, P. (2009). Do French-English bilingual children gesture more than monolingual children? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38, 573585.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Pika, S., Yin, H., & Marentette, P. (2007). Gesture use in story recall by Chinese–English bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 721735.Google Scholar
Oshima-Takane, Y., MacWhinney, B., Sirai, H., Miyata, S., & Naka, N. (1998). CHILDES manual for Japanese (2nd ed.). Nagoya: JCHAT Project.Google Scholar
Pettenati, P., Sekine, K., Congestrì, E., & Volterra, V. (2012). A comparative study on representational gestures in Italian and Japanese children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 36, 149164.Google Scholar
Pika, S., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P. F. (2006). A cross-cultural study on the use of gestures: Evidence for cross-linguistic transfer? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 319327.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1993). From rote learning to system building: Acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition, 48, 2169.Google Scholar
Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M., & Chen, Y. (1996). Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7, 226231.Google Scholar
Sekine, K., Stam, G., Yoshioka, K., Tellier, M., & Capirci, O. (2015). Cross-linguistic views of gesture usage. Vigo International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 12, 91105.Google Scholar
Sherman, J., & Nicoladis, E. (2004). Gestures by advanced Spanish-English second-language learners. Gesture, 4, 143156.Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (2004). U-shaped interest in u-shaped development—And what it means. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 110.Google Scholar
So, W. C., Demir, O. E., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). When speech is ambiguous, gesture steps in: Sensitivity to discourse-pragmatic principles in early childhood. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 209224.Google Scholar