Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T00:05:42.169Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Conversational skills and modeling in learning disabled boys

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Mavis Donahue*
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Chicago
Tanis Bryan
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Chicago
*
Mavis Donahue, College of Education, Box 4348, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, Illinois 60680

Abstract

Learning disabled (LD) children in the role of interviewer have been found to be less skilled than nondisabled children at initiating and sustaining a dialogue with a classmate. This study tested the effects of modeling on these conversational skills and on metaconversational knowledge. LD and nondisabled boys in grades 2 through 8 listened to either a dialogue of a child interviewer modeling open-ended questions, conversational devices and contingent comments and responses, or a monologue presenting only the interviewee's responses. Each subject was then videotaped interviewing a classmate. Although the dialogue model increased LD children's production of open-ended questions and comments, these strategies appeared more difficult for their listeners to understand and expand. Results of the metaconversational responses suggest that LD children are aware of their difficulties in conversational interaction.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bachara, G. H.Empathy in learning disabled children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1976, 43, 541542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A.Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.Google Scholar
Bates, E.Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press, 1976.Google Scholar
Bryan, J., Bryan, T., & Sonnefeld, J.Being known by the company one keeps: The contagion of first impressions. Learning Disability Quarterly, 1982, 5, 288294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, T.Peer popularity of learning disabled children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1974, 7, 621625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, T.Peer popularity of learning disabled children: A replication. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1976, 9, 307311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, T., & Bryan, J.Social interactions of learning disabled children. Learning Disability Quarterly, 1978, 1, 3338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, T., Donahue, M., & Pearl, R.Learning disabled children's peer interactions during a small-group problem-solving task. Learning Disability Quarterly, 1981, 4, 1322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, T., Donahue, M., Pearl, R., & Herzog, A. Conversational interactions between mothers and their learning disabled children during a problem-solving task. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, in press.Google Scholar
Bryan, T., Donahue, M., Pearl, R., & Sturm, C.Learning disabled children's conversational skills: The “TV Talk Show.” Learning Disability Quarterly, 1981, 4, 250259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryan, T., & Pflaum, S.Social interactions of learning disabled children: A linguistic, social and cognitive analysis. Learning Disability Quarterly, 1978, 4, 7079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H., Caldwell, C., & Christian, W.Classroom training of conversational skills and remote programming for the practice of these skills in another setting. Child Behavior Therapy, 1979, 1, 219228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J.A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1960, 20, 3746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J.A Comprehensive Test of Basic Skills: Form T Monterey, CA.: CTB/McGraw-Hill, 1975.Google Scholar
Conover, W., & Iman, R.Rank transformations as a bridge between parametric and non-parametric statistics. American Statistican, 1981, 35, 124129.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. Sociolinguistic patterns in adult-child interaction. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press, 1979.Google Scholar
Cosgrove, J., & Patterson, C.Generalization of training for children's listener skills. Child Development, 1978, 49, 513516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Courtright, J., & Courtright, I.Imitative modeling as a theoretical base for instructing language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1976, 19, 655663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtright, J., & Courtright, I.Imitative modeling as a language intervention strategy: The effects of two mediating variables. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1979, 22, 389402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, M., & Rudel, R.Naming of object drawings by dyslexic and other learning disabled children. Brain and Language, 1976, 3, 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donahue, M.Requesting strategies of learning disabled children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1981, 2, 213234.(a)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donahue, M. Conversational competence in learning disabled children: An attempt to activate the inactive listener. Proceedings from the Second Annual University of Wisconsin Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders, 1981.(b)Google Scholar
Donahue, M., Pearl, R., & Bryan, T.Conversational competence in learning disabled children: Responses to inadequate messages. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1980, 1, 387403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donahue, M., Pearl, R., & Bryan, T. Communicative competence in learning disabled children. In Bialer, H. & Gadow, K. (Eds.), Advances in learning and behavioral disabilities, Vol. 2. Greenwich, CT.: J.A.I. Press, 1983.Google Scholar
Dore, J., Gearheart, M., & Newman, D. The structure of nursery school conversations. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Children's language, Vol. 1. New York: Gardner Press, 1978.Google Scholar
Dunn, L.Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, 1965.Google Scholar
Durost, W., Bixler, H., Wrightstone, J., Prescott, G., & Balow, I.Metropolitan Achievement Test. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1971.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S., & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.) Child discourse. New York: Academic Press, 1977.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S., & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.) Federal Register, 1977, p. 65083.Google Scholar
Gamica, O., & King, M. (Eds.) Language, children and society: The effects of social factors on learning to communicate. Oxford: Pergamon Press, 1979.Google Scholar
Gleason, J. Code-switching in children's language. In Moore, T. E. (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press, 1973.Google Scholar
Grice, H. Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. (Eds.), Syntax and semantics, Vol. 3: Speech acts. New York: Seminar Press, 1975.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Rogers-Warren, A. A milieu approach to teaching language. In Schiefelbusch, R. (Ed.), Language intervention strategies. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press, 1978.Google Scholar
Huitema, B.The analysis of covariance statistics. New York: Wiley Press, 1980.Google Scholar
Hunt, K.Grammatical structures written at three grade levels. National Council of Teachers of English. Research Report No. 3. Champaign, III.: National Council of Teachers of English, 1965.Google Scholar
Ironsmith, M., & Whitehurst, G.How children learn to listen: The effects of modeling feedback styles on children's performance in referential communication. Developmental Psychology, 1978, 14, 546554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, E.Conversational competence in children. Journal of Child Language, 1974, 1, 163183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, E., & Schieffelin, B. Topic as a discourse notion: A study of topic in the conversations of children and adults. In Li, C. (Ed.), Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press, 1976.Google Scholar
Kelly, J., Furman, W., Phillips, J., Hathom, S., & Wilson, T.Teaching conversational skills to retarded adolescents. Child Behavior Therapy, 1979, 1, 8597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L.Developmental considerations in the management of language disabled children. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1975, 8, 232237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L.Facilitating linguistic skills in children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1981, 2, 89118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucas, E.Semantic and pragmatic language disorders. Rockville, Maryland: Aspen Systems Corporation, 1980.Google Scholar
Minkin, N., Braukmann, C. J., Minkin, B. L., Timbers, G. D., Timbers, B. J., Fixsen, D. L., Phillips, E. L., & Wolf, M. M.The social validation and training of conversational skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1976, 9, 127139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mishler, E.Studies in dialogue and discourse: I. An exponential law of successive questioning. Language in Society, 1975, 4, 3152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noel, M. N.Referential communication abilities of learning disabled children. Learning Disability Quarterly, 1980, 3, 7075.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press, 1979.Google Scholar
Otis, A., & Lennon, R.Otis-Lennon Mental Ability Test: Technical handbook. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Jovanovich, 1969.Google Scholar
Pearl, R., Donahue, M., & Bryan, T. Learning disabled children's tactfulness. Paper presented at Council for Learning Disabilities, Houston, Texas, October 1981. (a)Google Scholar
Pearl, R., Donahue, M., & Bryan, T.Learning disabled and normal children's responses to non-explicit requests for clarification. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1981, 53, 919925. (b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prelock, P., & Panagos, J.Mimicry vs. imitative modeling: Facilitating sentence production in the speech of the retarded. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1980, 9, 565578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rees, N. Pragmatics of language: Applications to normal and disordered language development. In Schiefelbusch, R. (Ed.), Bases of language intervention. Baltimore, MD: University Park, 1978.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, T., & Zimmerman, B.Social learning and cognition. New York: Academic Press, 1978.Google Scholar
Scranton, T. R., & Ryckman, D. B.Learning disabled children in an integrative program: So-ciometric status. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1979, 12, 402407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, S., Speed, F., & Millikan, G.Population marginal means in the linear model: An alternative to least square means. The American Statistician, 1980, 34, 216221.Google Scholar
Semel, E., & Wiig, E.Clinical evaluation of language functions. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill, 1980.Google Scholar
Shantz, C., & Wilson, K.Training communication skills in young children. Child Development, 1972, 43, 693698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M. Learning the rules of the game: Four views of the relation between social interaction and syntax acquisition. In Deutsch, W. (Ed.), The child's construction of language. New York: Academic Press, 1982.Google Scholar
Sonnenschein, S., & Whitehurst, G.The development of communication: When a bad model makes a good teacher. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1980, 3, 371390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spekman, N.Dyadic verbal communication abilities of learning disabled and normally achieving fourth and fifth grade boys. Learning Disability Quarterly, 1981, 4, 139151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, E., Clark, W., & Tiegs, E.Short Form Test of Academic Aptitude. Monterey, CA.: CTB/McGraw-Hill, 1970.Google Scholar
Vogel, S.Syntactic abilities in normal and dyslexic children. Baltimore: University Park Press, 1975.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G., & Sonnenschein, S. The development of informative messages in referential communication: Knowing when vs. knowing how. In Dickson, W. P. (Ed.), Children's oral communication skills. New York: Academic Press, 1981.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G., & Vasta, R.Is language acquired through imitation? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1975, 4, 3759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G.The development of communication: Changes with age and modeling. Child Development, 1976, 47, 473482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G., & Merkur, A.The development of communication modeling and contrast failure. Child Development, 1977, 48, 9931001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiig, E., & Semel, E.Language disabilities in children and adolescents. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill, 1976.Google Scholar
Wiig, E., & Semel, E.Language assessment and intervention for the learning disabled. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill, 1980.Google Scholar
Wilcox, M., & Leonard, L.Experimental acquisition of wh-questions in language disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1978, 21, 220239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed