Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-jbqgn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-06T05:51:20.288Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children's picture naming difficulty and errors: Effects of age of acquisition, uncertainty, and name generality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Carla J. Johnson*
Affiliation:
University of Western Ontario
James M. Clark
Affiliation:
University of Western Ontario
*
C. Johnson, Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada

Abstract

On the basis of prior research and a theoretical analysis of picture naming, we examined three correlates of child picture naming difficulty: age of name acquisition, picture-to-name uncertainty, and name generality (instance or category names). Study 1 demonstrated that late age of acquisition and high adult name uncertainty were associated with increased naming difficulty for both single instance and category items from the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (EOWPVT) (Gardner, 1979). Age of acquisition was a particularly strong predictor for instance items. Moreover, category pictures were more difficult to name than single instance pictures with names of comparable age of acquisition. In Study 2, analysis of incorrect responses to category pictures demonstrated that children made more instance errors than adults, but not more errors of other kinds. Study 2 also showed that a child uncertainty measure correlated more strongly with EOWPVT item difficulty than the adult measure of Study 1. The findings suggest that picture naming scores reflect both the availability of the names in lexical memory and their accessibility, which, in turn, partly depend on the amount of interference from competing responses. Some implications for the interpretation and remediation of picture naming deficits are suggested.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object, and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1958). How shall a thing be called? Psychological Review, 65, 1421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterfield, G. B., & Butterfield, E. C. (1977). Lexical codability and age. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 113118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & White, M. N. (1973a). Age-of-acquisition norms for 220 picturable nouns. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 563576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B., (1973b). Word frequency and age of acquisition as determiners of picture-naming latency. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 8595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, M. B., Rudel, R. G., & Broman, M. (1981). Tests that discriminate between dyslexic and other learning-disabled boys. Brain and Language, 13, 118129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Fey, M. E. (1986). Language intervention with young children. San Diego, CA: College-Hill.Google Scholar
Fried-Oken, M. (1987). Qualitative examination of children's naming skills through test adaptations. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 18, 206216.Google Scholar
Gardner, M. F. (1979). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Gardner, M. F. (1983). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Upper Extension. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
German, D. (1984). Diagnosis of word-finding disorders in children with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 353358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geschwind, N. (1967). The varieties of naming errors. Cortex, 3, 97112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Goodglass, H., Theurkauf, J. C., & Wingfield, A. (1984). Naming latencies as evidence for two modes of lexical retrieval. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 135146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, G. A., Taweel, S. S., Green, R. Z., & Flaugher, J. (1978). Effects of instructions on children's attention to stimulus components. Developmental Psychology, 14, 499506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, D. I., & Lupker, S. J. (1983). Semantic priming of pictures and words: A levels of processing approach. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 4560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jansky, J., & DeHirsch, K. (1972). Preventing reading failure. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Johnson, C. J. (1986). Dual coding of tactually presented forms by young children: Effects of strategy instructions and individual differences. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario.Google Scholar
Katz, R. B. (1986). Phonological deficiencies in children with reading disability: Evidence from an object-naming task. Cognition, 22, 222257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kneedler, R. D., & Hallaban, D. P. (1984). Self-monitoring as an attentional strategy for academic tasks with learning disabled children. In Gholson, B. & Rosenthal, T. (Eds.), Applications of cognitive-developmental theory (pp. 173248). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Kohn, S. E., & Goodglass, H. (1985). Picture-naming in aphasia. Brain and Language, 24, 266283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lachman, R. (1973a). Subject and stimulus data for research on the internal lexicon. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 3, 40.Google Scholar
Lachman, R. (1973b). Uncertainty effects on time to access the internal lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 99, 199208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachman, R., & Lachman, J. L. (1980). Picture naming: Retrieval and activation of long-term memory. In Poon, L. W., Fozard, J. L., Cermak, L. S., Grenberg, D., & Thompson, L. W. (Eds.), New directions in memory and aging (pp. 313343). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lachman, R., Shaffer, J. P., & Hennrikus, D. (1974). Language and cognition: Effects of stimulus codability, name-word frequency, and age of acquisition on lexical reaction time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 613625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Nippold, M. A., Kail, R., & Hale, C. A. (1983). Picture naming in language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 609615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things: A study of human learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McKeown, M. G., & Curtis, M. E. (1987). The nature of vocabulary acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Milianti, F. J., & Cullinan, W. L. (1974). Effects of age and word frequency on object recognition and naming in children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 17, 373385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mills, R. H., Knox, A. W., Juola, J. F., & Salmon, S. J. (1979). Cognitive loci of impairments in picture naming by aphasie subjects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 22, 7387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morin, R. E., & Forrin, B. (1965). Information processing: Choice reaction times of first- and third-grade students for types of associations. Child Development, 36, 713720.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: The acquisition of shared meaning. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Newcombe, F., Oldfield, R. C., Ratcliff, G. G., & Wingfield, A. (1971). Recognition and naming of object-drawings by men with focal brain wounds. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 34, 329340.Google ScholarPubMed
Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1964). The time it takes to name an object. Nature, 202, 10311032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldfield, R. C., (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 273281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paivio, A., Clark, J. M., Digdon, N., & Bons, T. (in press). Referential processing: Correlates of naming pictures and imaging to words. Memory & Cognition.Google Scholar
Pressley, M., Johnson, C. J., & Symons, S. (1987). Elaborating to learn and learning to elaborate. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 7691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochford, G. (1971). A study of naming errors in dysphasic and in demented patients. Neuropsychologia, 9, 437443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochford, G., & Williams, M. (1962). Studies in the development and breakdown of the use of names. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 25, 222233.Google ScholarPubMed
Rochford, G., (1965). Studies in the development and breakdown of the use of names: The effects of word frequency. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 28, 407413.Google ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudel, R. G., Denckla, M. B., & Broman, M. (1981). The effect of varying stimulus context on word-finding ability: Dyslexia further differentiated from other learning disabilities. Brain and Language, 13, 130144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudel, R. G., Denckla, M. B., Broman, M., & Hirsch, S. (1980). Word-finding as a function of stimulus context: Children compared with aphasic adults. Brain and Language, 10, 111119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174215.Google ScholarPubMed
Wales, R., Colman, M., & Pattison, P. (1983). How a thing is called – a study of mothers' and children's naming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, T. G. (1982). Naming practices, typicality, and underextension in child language. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 324346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiig, E. H., & Becker-Caplan, L. (1984). Linguistic retrieval strategies and word-finding difficulties among children with language disabilities. Topics in Language Disorders, 4(3), 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, S. E., & Canter, G. J. (1982). The influence of situational context on naming performance in aphasic syndromes. Brain and Language, 17, 92106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M. (1982). The word-retrieval process and reading in children and aphasies. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Children's language: Volume 3 (pp. 437493). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wolf, M., Bally, H., & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57, 9881000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M., & Goodglass, H. (1986). Dyslexia, dysnomia, and lexical retrieval: A longitudinal investigation. Brain and Language, 28, 154168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed