Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-cnmwb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T15:50:34.783Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A cage without a view increases stress and impairs cognitive performance in rats

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

AP Harris*
Affiliation:
47 Little France, Queens Medical Research Institute, University of Edinburgh EH16 4TJ, UK
RB D’Eath
Affiliation:
Scottish Agricultural College, Sir Stephen Watson Building, Bush Estate, Penicuik EH26 0PH, UK
SD Healy
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews KY16 9JP, UK
*
* Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints: Anjie.Harris@ed.ac.uk

Abstract

Single housing is believed to be chronically stressful and to have a negative impact on welfare and cognition in rats (Rattus norvegicus). However, single housing does not consistently evoke stress-like responses nor does it consistently impair cognitive performance. In an experiment in which all cages were separated by an opaque barrier, single- and pair-housed pigmented (dark-eyed) rats performed equally in a cognitive test and displayed similar levels of anxiety during testing. Additionally, bar biting in the home cage did not differ between the two groups. Stress levels both during cognitive testing and in the home cage were higher than those we have previously reported when rats were housed without opaque barriers between the cages. We conclude that visual interactions between rats in different cages may be of sufficient significance that single housing in a cage with a view to neighbouring rats and to the rest of the laboratory holding room may be preferable to pair housing in a cage without this view.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2010 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baenninger, LP 1967 Comparison of behavioural development in socially isolated and grouped rats. Animal Behaviour 15: 312323CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balcombe, JP 2006 Laboratory environments and rodents’ behavioural needs: a review. Laboratory Animals 40: 217235CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, DR, Wilcox, ZC and Bayloss, RC 1995 Impact of differential housing on humoral immunity following exposure to an acute stressor in rats. Physiology and Behavior 57: 649653CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beiko, J, Lander, R, Hampson, E, Boon, F and Cain, DP 2004 Contribution of sex differences in the acute stress response to sex differences in water maze performance in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research 151: 239253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festing, MFW, Overend, P, Das, RG, Borja, MC and Berdoy, M 2002 The Design of Animal Experiments, No 14 Edition. The Royal Society of Medicine Press Ltd: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Harris, AP, D’Eath, RB and Healy, SD 2008a Sex differences in spatial cognition are not caused by isolation housing. Behaviour 145: 757778Google Scholar
Harris, AP, D’Eath, RB and Healy, SD 2008b Sex differences, or not, in spatial cognition: acute stress is the key. Animal Behaviour 76: 15791589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, AP, D’Eath, RB and Healy, SD 2009 Environmental enrichment enhances spatial cognition in rats by reducing thigmotaxis (wall hugging) during testing. Animal Behaviour 77: 14591464CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatch, A, Wiberg, GS, Balaz, T and Grice, HC 1963 Longterm isolation stress in rats. Science 142: 507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurlbert, SH 1984 Pseudoreplication and the design of ecological field experiments. Ecological Monographs 54: 187211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurst, JL, Barnard, CJ, Nevison, CM and West, CD 1997 Housing and welfare in laboratory rats: welfare implications of isolation and social contact among caged males. Animal Welfare 6: 329347Google Scholar
Hurst, JL, Barnard, CJ, Nevison, CM and West, CD 1998 Housing and welfare in laboratory rats: welfare implications of isolation and social contact among caged females. Animal Welfare 7: 121136Google Scholar
Krohn, TC, Hansen, AK and Dragsted, N 2003 The impact of cage ventilation on rats housed in IVC systems. Laboratory Animals 37: 8593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krohn, TC, S⊘rensen, DB, Ottesen, JL and Hansen, AK 2006 The effects of individual housing on mice and rats: a review. Animal Welfare 15: 343352Google Scholar
Lewis, RS and Hurst, JL 2004 The assessment of bar chewing as an escape behaviour in laboratory mice. Animal Welfare 13: 1925Google Scholar
Morinan, A and Leonard, BE 1980 Some anatomical and physiological correlates of social isolation in the young rat. Physiology and Behavior 24: 637640CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nevison, CM, Hurst, JL and Barnard, CJ 1999 Why do male ICR(CD-1) mice perform bar-related (stereotypic) behaviour? Behavioural Processes 47: 95111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nyska, A, Hester, SD, Cooper, RL, Goldman, JM, Stoker, TE, House, D and Wolf, DC 2002 Single or group housing altered hormonal physiology and affected pituitary and interstitial cell kinetics. The Journal of Toxicological Sciences 27: 449457CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson-Kane, EG 2004 Enrichment for laboratory rats: a review. Animal Welfare 13: S209S214Google Scholar
Patterson-Kane, EG, Hunt, M and Harper, D 1999 Behavioural indexes of poor welfare in laboratory rats. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2: 97110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson-Kane, EG, Hunt, M and Harper, D 2002 Rats demand social contact. Animal Welfare 11: 327332Google Scholar
Perrot-Sinal, TS, Kostenuik, MA, Ossenkopp, KP and Kavaliers, M 1996 Sex differences in performance in the Morris water maze and the effects of initial nonstationary hidden platform training. Behavioural Neuroscience 110: 13091320CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinn, GP and Keough, MJ 2002 Experimental Design and Data Analysis for Biologists. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renstrom, A, Bjoring, G and Hoglund, AU 2001 Evaluation of individually ventilated cage systems for laboratory rodents: occupational health aspects. Laboratory Animals 35: 4250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, WMS and Burch, RL 1959 The Principles of Humane Experimental Techinique, Reprinted 1992, Edition. Universities Federation for Animal Welfare: Wheathampstead, Herts, UKGoogle Scholar
Sandstrom, NJ and Hart, SR 2005 Isolation stress during the third postnatal week alters radial arm maze performance and corticosterone levels in adulthood. Behavioural Brain Research 156: 289296Google ScholarPubMed
Snihur, AWK, Hampson, E and Cain, DP 2008 Estradiol and corticosterone independently impair spatial navigation in the morris water maze in adult female rats. Behavioural Brain Research 187: 5666CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treit, D and Fundytus, M 1989 Thigmotaxis as a test for anxiolytic activity in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 31: 959962CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verwer, CM, van der Ven, LTM, van den Bos, R and Hendriksen, CFM 2007 Effects of housing condition on experimental outcome in a reproduction toxicity test. Regulatory Toxicology and Pharmacology 48: 184193CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, DL 2009 Sensory stimulation as environmental enrichment for captive animals: a review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 118: 1-11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcoxon, JS, Nadolski, GJ, Samarut, J, Chassande, O and Redei, EE 2007 Behavioural inhibition and impaired spatial learning and memory in hypothyroid mice lacking thyroid hormone receptor α. Behavioural Brain Research 177: 109116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wongwitdecha, N and Marsden, CA 1996 Effects of social isolation on learning in the Morris water maze. Brain Research 715: 119124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed