Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 6
  • Print publication year: 2003
  • Online publication date: December 2009

18 - Action recognition in normal and schizophrenic subjects

from Part III - Disturbances of the self: the case of schizophrenia

Summary

Abstract

The ability to attribute an action to its proper agent and to understand its meaning when it is produced by someone else are basic aspects of human social communication. Several psychiatric symptoms, such as those of schizophrenia, relate to a dysfunction of the awareness of one's own action as well as of recognition of actions performed by others. Such syndromes thus offer a framework for studying the determinants of the sense of agency, which ultimately allows one to attribute correctly actions to their veridical source. This chapter will report a series of experiments in normal subjects and schizophrenic patients dealing with the recognition of actions. The basic paradigm used in these experiments was to present the subject with simple actions which may or may not correspond to those they currently execute. Systematic distortions have been introduced, such that the threshold for accepting an action as one's own could be determined. In normal subjects, this threshold is relatively high, indicating the existence of a specific mode of processing for action signals, independent from visual processing used in other perceptual activities. In schizophrenic patients, this threshold is further increased, with a strong tendency to self-attribute actions which do not correspond to those they have performed. The results reveal a clear distinction between patient groups with and without hallucinations and/or delusions of influence. Influenced patients show a higher rate of self-attributions. These results point to schizophrenia and related disorders as a paradigmatic alteration of a ‘who?’ system for action monitoring and self-consciousness.

REFERENCES
Andreasen, N. C. (1983). The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa
Andreasen, N. C. (1984). The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa
Andreasen, N. C. (1999). A unitary model of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 781–7
Baddeley, A. (1998). Recent developments in working memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 234–8
Bain, A. (1855). The Senses and the Intellect. London: Parker
Bernard, D., Lancon, C. & Bougerol, T. (1997). Attention models in evaluating schizophrenia. Encephale, 23, 113–18
Cadenhead, K. S., Geyer, M. A., Butler, R. W. et al. (1997). Information visual processing deficits of schizophrenia patients: relationship to clinical ratings, gender and medication status. Schizophrenia Research, 28, 51–62
Chadwick, P. & Birchwood, M. (1994). The omnipotence of voices. A cognitive approach to auditory hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 190–201
Chen, Y., Palafox, G. P., Nakayama, K. et al. (1999). Motion perception in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 149–54
Daprati, E., Franck, N., Georgieff, N. et al. (1997). Looking for the agent: an investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophrenic patients. Cognition, 65, 71–86
David, A. S. (1994). The neuropsychological origin of auditory hallucinations. In The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, ed. A. S. David & J. C. Cutting, pp. 269–313. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum
Dierks, T., Linden, D. E. J., Jandl, M. et al. (1999). Activation of the Heschl's gyrus during auditory hallucniations. Neuron, 22, 615–21
Dominey, P. & Georgieff, N. (1997). Schizophrenic learn surface but not abstract structure in a serial reaction time task. Neuroreport, 8, 2877–82
Duchenne de Boulogne (1855). De l'Éléctrisation Localisée et de son application à la Physiologie et à la Médecine. Paris: Baillère
Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G. & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation. A magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2608–11
Farrer, C., Franck, N., Georgieff, N. et al. (2002). Confusing the self and the other. Impaired attribution of actions in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin (in press)
Fletcher, P. (1999). Abnormal cingulate modulation of fronto-temporal connectivity in schizophrenia. Neuroimage, 9, 337–42
Fourneret, P. & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1133–40
Franck, N., Farrer, C., Georgieff, N. et al. (2001). Defective recognition of one's own actions in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 454–9
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. & Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 357–63
Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 14–21
Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 493–501
Georgieff, N. & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Beyond consciousness of external reality: a “who” system for consciousness of action and self-consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 465–77
Gepnery, B., Mestre, D., Masson, G. & Schonen, S. (1995). Postural effects of motion vision in young autistic children. Neuroreport, 6, 1211–14
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 6, 348–57
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. & Selemon, L. D. (1997). Functional and anatomical aspects of prefrontal pathology in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 437–58
Gould, L. N. (1949). Auditory hallucinations in subvocal speech: objective study in a case of schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 109, 418–27
Gray, J. A., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J. N. P., Hemsley, D. R. & Smith, A. D. (1991). The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 1–84
Grèzes, J. & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, and verb generation of actions: a meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 1–19
Janet, P. (1937). Les troubles de la personnalité sociale. Annales Médico-Psychologiques, II, 149–200
Jeannerod, M. (1990). The representation of the goal of an action and its role in the control of goal-directed movements. In Computational Neuroscience, ed. E. Schwartz, pp. 352–65. Cambridge: MIT Press
Jeannerod, M. (1993). A theory of representation driven actions. In The perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Sources of Self-knowledge, ed. U. Neisser, pp. 89–101. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain. Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–245
Jeannerod, M. (1997). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Action. Oxford: Blackwell
Jeannerod, M. (1999). To act or not to act: perspectives on the representation of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A52, 1–29
Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage, 14, S103–9
Jeannerod, M. & Frak, V. (1999). Mental imaging of motor activity in humans. Currrent Opinions in Neurobiology, 9, 735–9
Johansson, G. (1977). Studies on visual perception of locomotion. Perception, 6, 365–76
Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S. & Grafman, J. (1999). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature, 399, 148–51
Kuperberg, G. & Heckers, S. (2000). Schizophrenia and cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 205–10
Kuperberg, G. R., McGuire, P. K. & David, A. S. (1998). Reduced sensitivity to linguistic context in schizophrenic thought disorder: evidence from on-line monitoring for words in linguistically anomalous sentences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 8423–34
Laws, K. R. (1999). A meta analytic review of Wisconsin card sort studies in schizophrenia: general intellectual deficit in disguise? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 4, 1–35
Lewes, G. H. (1878). Motor feelings and the muscular sense. Brain, 1, 14–28
Manoach, D. S., Press, D. Z., Thangaraj, V. et al. (1999). Schizophrenic participants activate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a working memory task, as measured by fMRI. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1128–37
McGuire, P. K., Silbersweig, D. A. & Frith, C. D. (1996). Functional neuroanatomy of verbal self-monitoring. Brain, 119, 907–17
Minkowski, E. (1927). La Schizophrénie. Psychopathologie des Schizoïdes et des Schizophrènes. Lausanne: Editions Payot & Rivages
Nielsen, T. (1963). Volition: a new experimental approach. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 4, 225–30
Posada, A., Franck, N., Georgieff, N. & Jeannerod, M. (2001). Anticipating incoming events: an impaired cognitive process in schizophrenia. Cognition, 81, 209–25
Prabhakaran, V., Narayanan, K., Zhao, Z. & Gabrieli, D. E. (2000). Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 85–9
Ramachandran, V. S., Armel, C., Foster, C. & Stoddard, R. (1998). Object recognition can drive motion perception. Nature, 395, 852–3
Riddoch, M. J. & Humphreys, G. W. (1993). BORB: Birmingham Object Recognition Battery. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum
Roth, M., Decety, J., Raybaudi, M. et al. (1996). Possible involvement of primary motor cortex in mentally simulated movement. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroreport, 7, 1280–4
Schneider, K. (1955). Klinische Psychopathologie. Stuttgart: Thieme Verlag
Schwartz, B. D., Maron, B. A., Evans, W. J. & Winstead, D. K. (1999). High velocity transient visual processing deficits diminish ability of patients with schizophrenia to recognize objects. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioural Neurology, 12, 170–7
Servan-Schreiber, D., Cohen, J. D. & Steingard, S. (1996). Schizophrenic deficits in the processing of context. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 1105–12
Shiffrar, M. & Freyd, J. (1990). Apparent motion in the human body. Psychological Science, 1, 257–64
Silbersweig, D. A., Stern, E., Frith, C. et al. (1995). A functional neuroanatomy of hallucinations in schizophrenia. Nature, 378, 176–9
Spence, S. A., Brooks, D. J., Hirsch, S. R. et al. (1997). A PET study of voluntary movements in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain, 120, 1997–2011
Stevens, A. A., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., Gore, J. C., Fulbright, R. K. & Wexler, B. E. (1998). Cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia during auditory word and tone working memory demonstrated by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 1097–103
Stevens, J. A., Fonlupt, P., Shiffrar, M. & Decety, J. (2000). New aspects of motion perception: selective neural encoding of apparent human movements. NeuroReport, 11, 109–15
Weinberger, D. R. & Berman, K. F. (1996). Prefrontal function in schizophrenia: confounds and controversies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 351, 1495–503
Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z. & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–2
Zakzanis, K. K. & Heinrichs, R. W. (1999). Schizophrenia and the frontal brain: a quantitative review. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 556–66