Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-wpx84 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-07T11:33:03.452Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

53 - The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Jonathan Haidt
Affiliation:
University of Virginia
Jonathan E. Adler
Affiliation:
Brooklyn College, City University of New York
Lance J. Rips
Affiliation:
Northwestern University, Illinois
Get access

Summary

Julie and Mark are brother and sister. They are traveling together in France on summer vacation from college. One night they are staying alone in a cabin near the beach. They decide that it would be interesting and fun if they tried making love. At the very least it would be a new experience for each of them. Julie was already taking birth control pills, but Mark uses a condom too, just to be safe. They both enjoy making love, but they decide not to do it again. They keep that night as a special secret, which makes them feel even closer to each other. What do you think about that? Was it OK for them to make love?

Most people who hear the above story immediately say that it was wrong for the siblings to make love, and they then begin searching for reasons (Haidt, Bjorklund, & Murphy, 2000). They point out the dangers of inbreeding, only to remember that Julie and Mark used two forms of birth control. They argue that Julie and Mark will be hurt, perhaps emotionally, even though the story makes it clear that no harm befell them. Eventually, many people say something like, “I don't know, I can't explain it, I just know it's wrong.” But what model of moral judgment allows a person to know that something is wrong without knowing why?

Type
Chapter
Information
Reasoning
Studies of Human Inference and its Foundations
, pp. 1024 - 1052
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albright, L., Kenny, D. A., & Malloy, T. E. (1988). Consensus in personality judgments at zero acquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 387–395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 256–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 1032–1037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asch, S. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70 (9, Whole No. 416).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. (1963). Social learning and personality development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, efficiency, intention, and control in social cognition. In Wyer, J. R. S. & Srull, T. K. (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1996). The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditionally automatic activation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 185–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (1995). Myside bias in thinking about abortion. Thinking and Reasoning, 1, 221–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (1997). The illusion of morality as self-interest. Psychological Science, 8, 330–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (1998). Judgment misguided: Intuition and error in public decision making. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron, J. (2000). Thinking and deciding (3rd ed.). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bastick, T. (1982). Intuition: How we think and act. Chichester, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic?Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 65–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D., Engel, C. L., & Fridell, S. R. (1999). Value judgments: Testing the somatic-marker hypothesis using false physiological feedback. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1021–1032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D., O'Quinn, K., Fulty, J., Vanderplass, M., & Isen, A. M. (1983). Influence of self-reported distress and empathy on egoistic versus altruistic motivation to help. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 706–718.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Newman, L. S. (1994). Self-regulation of cognitive inference and decision processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 3–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. (1991). Connectionism and the mind: An introduction to parallel processing in networks. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Benedict, R. (1959). Patterns of culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. (Original work published in 1934.)Google Scholar
Berger, P. L., & Luckman, T. (1967). The social construction of reality. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Black, J. E., Jones, T. A., Nelson, C. A., & Greenough, W. T. (1998). Neuronal plasticity and the developing brain. In Alessi, N. E., Coyle, J. T., Harrison, S. I., & Eth, S. (Eds.), Handbook of child and adolescent psychiatry (pp. 31–53). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A critical review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 1–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1960). The process of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R., & Whiten, A. (Eds.). (1988). Machiavellian intelligence. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752–766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In Zanna, M. P., Olson, J. M., & Herman, C. P. (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario symposium (pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chaiken, S., Giner-Sorolla, R., & Chen, S. (1996). Beyond accuracy: Defense and impression motives in heuristic and systematic information processing. In Gollwitzer, P. M. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 553–578). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, S., & Chaiken, S. (1999). The heuristic–systematic model in its broader context. In Chaiken, S. & Trope, Y. (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 73–96). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chen, S., Shechter, D., & Chaiken, S. (1996). Getting at the truth or getting along: Accuracy- versus impression-motivated heuristic and systematic processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 262–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cialdini, R. B. (1991). Altruism or egoism? That is (still) the question. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 124–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cialdini, R., Schaller, M., Houlihan, D., Arps, K., Fultz, J., & Beaman, A. (1987). Empathy-based helping: Is it selflessly or selfishly motivated?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 749–758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (1999). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cleckley, H. (1955). The mask of sanity. St. Louis, MO: C. V. Mosby.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In Wyer, R. S. & Srull, T. K. (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp. 323–417). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Colby, A., & Damon, W. (1992). Some do care: Contemporary lives of moral commitment. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1990). Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behavioral Brain Research, 41, 81–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damon, W. (1975). Early conceptions of positive justice as related to the development of logical operations. Child Development, 46, 301–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
D'Andrade, R. G. (1984). Cultural meaning systems. In Shweder, R. A. & LeVine, R. A. (Eds.), Culture theory (pp. 88–119). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Darley, J. (1993). Research on morality: Possible approaches, actual approaches. Psychological Science, 4, 353–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darley, J. M., & Berscheid, E. (1967). Increased liking as a result of anticipation of personal contact. Human Relations, 20, 29–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1998). The descent of man. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books. (Original work published 1874.)Google Scholar
Davis, J. L., & Rusbult, C. E. (2001). Attitude alignment in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 65–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waal, F. (1982). Chimpanzee politics. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Waal, F. (1991). The chimpanzee's sense of social regularity and its relation to the human sense of justice. American Behavioral Scientist, 34, 335–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waal, F. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Waal, F., & Lanting, F. (1997). Bonobo: The forgotten ape. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 207–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Edwards, K., & Hippel, W. (1995). Hearts and minds: The priority of affective versus cognitive factors in person perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 996–1011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Shea, C. L., Carlo, G., & Knight, G. P. (1991). Empathy-related responding and cognition: A “chicken and the egg” dilemma. In Kurtines, W. M. & Gewirtz, J. L. (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development: Vol. 1. Theory (pp. 63–88). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic evaluation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (1991). Structures of social life. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (1992). Four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99, 689–723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, A. P. (1999). Learning culture the way informants do: Observing, imitating, and participating. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, R. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1976). The interpretation of dreams (J. Strachey, Trans.). New York: Norton. (Original work published 1900).Google Scholar
Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Galotti, K. M. (1989). Approaches to studying formal and everyday reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 331–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985). The social brain. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S., Bogen, J. E., & Sperry, R. W. (1962). Some functional effects of sectioning the cerebral commissures in man. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 48, 1765–1769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, J. C. (1991). Toward an integration of Kohlberg's and Hoffman's theories of morality. In Kurtines, W. M. & Gewirtz, J. L. (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development: Vol. 1. Advances in theory, research, and application (pp. 183–222). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650–669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures: And other reports from my life with autism. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A. V. M., Kirkland, S., & Lyon, D. (1990). Evidence for terror management theory II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 308–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI study of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science,293, 2105–2108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, D. W., & Ross, L. (1991). Subjective construal, social inference, and human misunderstanding. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 319–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001). The moral emotions. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852–870). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Bjorklund, F., & Murphy, S. (2000). Moral dumbfounding: When intuition finds no reason. Unpublished manuscript, University of Virginia.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Hersh, M. (2001). Sexual morality: The cultures and reasons of liberals and conservatives. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 191–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S., & Dias, M. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 613–628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J., Rozin, P., McCauley, C. R., & Imada, S. (1997). Body, psyche, and culture: The relationship between disgust and morality. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9, 107–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1993). Without conscience. New York: Pocket Books.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D., & Quinn, M. J. (1971). Psychopathy and autonomic conditioning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77, 223–235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, R. M. (1981). Moral thinking: Its levels, method, and point. London: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458–489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, J. (1967). Ethical objectivism. In Edwards, P. (Ed.), The encyclopedia of philosophy (Vols. 3–4, pp. 71–75). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hart, D., & Fegley, S. (1995). Prosocial behavior and caring in adolescence: Relations to self-understanding and social judgment. Child Development, 66, 1346–1359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N. (1997). Four grammars for primate social relations. In Simpson, J. A. & Kenrick, D. T. (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 297–316). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hermans, D., Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1994). The affective priming effect: Automatic evaluation of evaluative information in memory. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 515–533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social cognition and social perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 369–425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirschi, T., & Hindelang, M. J. (1977). Intelligence and delinquency: A revisionist view. American Sociological Review, 42, 571–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation: Empathy and guilt. In Eisenberg, N. (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 218–231). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure. Journal of Physiology, 206, 419–436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hume, D. (1960). An enquiry concerning the principles of morals. La Salle, IL: Open Court. (Original work published 1777.)Google Scholar
Hume, D. (1969). A treatise of human nature. London: Penguin. (Original work published 1739–1740.)Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R. (1994). Synaptogenesis, synapse elimination, and neural plasticity in human cerebral cortex. In Nelson, C. A. (Ed.), Threats to optimal development: Integrating biological, psychological, and social risk factors. The Minnesota Symposia on Child Development, Vol. 27 (pp. 35–54). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jensen, L. A. (1997). Different worldviews, different morals: America's culture war divide. Human Development, 40, 323–344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39, 341–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, I. (1959). Foundation of the metaphysics of morals (L. W. Beck, Trans.). Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill. (Original work published 1785.)Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67, 490–507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive–developmental approach to socialization. In Goslin, D. A. (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347–480). Chicago: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1971). From is to ought: How to commit the naturalistic fallacy and get away with it in the study of moral development. In Mischel, T. (Ed.), Cognitive development and epistemology (pp. 151–235). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L., Levine, C., & Hewer, A. (1983). Moral stages: A current formulation and a response to critics. Basel, Switzerland: Karger.Google Scholar
Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96, 674–689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, D. (1991). The skills of argument. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557–558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurtines, W. M., & Gewirtz, J. L. (Eds.). (1991). Handbook of moral behavior and development (Vols. 1–3). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1996). Moral politics: What conservatives know that liberals don't. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lapsley, D. K. (1996). Moral psychology. Boulder, CO: Westview.Google Scholar
Laupa, M., & Turiel, E. (1986). Children's conceptions of adult and peer authority. Child Development, 57, 405–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lemer, M. J. (1965). Evaluation of performance as a function of performer's reward and attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 355–360.Google Scholar
Lerner, M. J., & Miller, D. T. (1978). Just world research and the attribution process: Looking back and ahead. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 1030–1051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109–137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, E. S. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2098–2109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahapatra, M. (1981). Traditional structure and change in an Orissan temple. Calcutta, India: Punthi Pustak.Google Scholar
Margolis, H. (1987). Patterns, thinking, and cognition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mednick, S. A., Pollock, V., Volavka, J., & Gabrielli, W. F. (1982). Biology and violence. In Wolfgang, M. & Weiner, N. A. (Eds.), Criminal violence (pp. 21–80). Beverly Hills: Sage.Google Scholar
Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, 54, 1053–1060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., & Mischel, H. N. (1976). A cognitive social-learning approach to morality and self-regulation. In Lickona, T. (Ed.), Moral development and behavior: Theory, research, and social issues (pp. 84–107). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Minoura, Y. (1992). A sensitive period for the incorporation of a cultural meaning system: A study of Japanese children growing up in the United States. Ethos, 20, 304–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moskowitz, G. B., Skurnik, I., & Galinsky, A. D. (1999). The history of dual process notions, and the future of pre-conscious control. In Chaiken, S. & Trope, Y. (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 12–36). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newcomb, T. M. (1943). Personality and social change: Attitude formation in a student community. New York: Dryden.Google Scholar
Nickerson, R. S. (1994). The teaching of thinking and problem solving. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Thinking and problem solving (pp. 409–449). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Schacter, S. (1966). Cognitive manipulation of pain. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 227–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L., & Turiel, E. (1978). Social interactions and the development of social concepts in preschool children. Child Development, 49, 400–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perkins, D. N., Allen, R., & Hafner, J. (1983). Difficulties in everyday reasoning. In Maxwell, W. (Ed.), Thinking: The frontier expands (pp. 177–189). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Perkins, D. N., Farady, M., & Bushey, B. (1991). Everyday reasoning and the roots of intelligence. In Voss, J. F., Perkins, D. N., & Segal, J. W. (Eds.), Informal reasoning and education (pp. 83–105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 123–205). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgement of the child (M. Gabain, Trans.). New York: Free Press. (Original work published 1932.)Google Scholar
Plato, . (1949). Timaeus (B. Jowen, Trans.). Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill. (Original work published 4th century B.C.)Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In Solso, R. L. (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Power, F. C., Higgins, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1989). Lawrence Kohlberg's approach to moral education. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1995). Origins of human social competence. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 205–218). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Toward in integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypothesis-testing model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 297–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raine, A. (1997). Antisocial behavior and psychophysiology: A biosocial perspective and a prefrontal dysfunction hypothesis. In Stoff, D. M., Breiling, J., & Maser, J. D. (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior (pp. 289–304). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Raine, A., Buchsbaum, M. S., Stanley, J., Lottenberg, S., Abel, L., & Stoddard, J. (1994). Selective reductions in pre-frontal glucose metabolism in murderers. Biological Psychiatry, 36, 365–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rest, J. R. (1979). Development in judging moral issues. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Robinson, R. J., Keltner, D., Ward, A., & Ross, L. (1995). Actual versus assumed differences in construal: “Naive realism” in intergroup perception and conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 404–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681–690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, W. D. (1930). The right and the good. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2000). Disgust. In Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 637–653). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schooler, J. W., Fiore, S. M., & Brandimonte, M. A. (1997). At a loss from words: Verbal overshadowing of perceptual memories. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 37, 291–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Information and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selman, R. (1971). The relation of role taking to the development of moral judgment in children. Child Development, 42, 79–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shavitt, S. (1990). The role of attitude objects in attitude formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 124–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherif, M. (1935). A study of some social factors in perception. Archives of Psychology, 27(187).Google Scholar
Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26, 978–986.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Goodnow, J., Hatano, G., LeVine, R. A., Markus, H., & Miller, P. (1998). The cultural psychology of development: One mind, many mentalities. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Lerner, R. M. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 865–937). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., & Haidt, J. (1993). The future of moral psychology: Truth, intuition, and the pluralist way. Psychological Science, 4, 360–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The “big three” of morality (autonomy, community, and divinity), and the “big three” explanations of suffering. In Brandt, A. & Rozin, P. (Eds.), Morality and Health (pp. 119–169). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Review, 74, 29–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, H. A. (1992). What is an “explanation” of behavior?Psychological Science, 3, 150–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, P. (1994). Rethinking life and death. New York: St. Martin's Press.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. (1978). Hypothesis testing processes in social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1202–1212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, R. C. (1993). The philosophy of emotions. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 3–15). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1991). A terror management theory of social behavior. In Zanna, M. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 93–159). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Spear, L. P. (2000). Neurobehavioral changes in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 111–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spellman, B. A., & Holyoak, K. J. (1992). If Saddam is Hitler then who is George Bush? Analogical mapping between systems of social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 913–933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stein, R., & Nemeroff, C. J. (1995). Moral overtones of food: Judgments of others based on what they eat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 480–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tappan, M. (1997). Language, culture, and moral development: A Vygotskian perspective. Developmental Review, 17, 78–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tetlock, P. E., Kristel, O. V., Elson, B., Green, M., & Lerner, J. (2000). The psychology of the unthinkable: Taboo trade-offs, forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 853–870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thoma, S. J., Narvaez, D., Rest, J., & Derryberry, P. (1999). Does moral judgment development reduce to political attitudes or verbal ability? Evidence using the defining issues test. Educational Psychology Review, 11, 325–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1920). A constant error in psychological ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 4, 25–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y., & Bassok, M. (1983). Information-gathering strategies in hypothesis-testing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 560–576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 863–932). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Turiel, E., Hildebrandt, C., & Wainryb, C. (1991). Judging social issues: Difficulties, inconsistencies, and consistencies. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56, 1–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turiel, E., Killen, M., & Helwig, C. C. (1987). Morality: Its structure, function, and vagaries. In Kagan, J. & Lamb, S. (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 155–243). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D., & Bargh, J. (1998). Control and automaticity in social life. In Gilbert, D. T., Fiske, S. T., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 446–496). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Werker, J., & Tees, R. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2001). Hypnotically induced disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Manuscript in preparation, University of Virginia.Google Scholar
Whiting, J., & Child, I. (1953). Child training and personality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Wicklund, R. A., & Brehm, J. W. (1976). Perspectives on cognitive dissonance. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Williams, B. (1967). Rationalism. In Edwards, P. (Ed.), The encyclopedia of philosophy (Vols. 7–8, pp. 69–75). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Wilson, J. Q. (1993). The moral sense. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Self-insight and the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wundt, W. (1969). Outlines of psychology (C. H. Judd, Trans.). St Clair Shores, MI: Scholarly Press. (Original work published in 1897.)Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimbardo, P. G., LaBerge, S., & Butler, L. D. (1993). Psychophysiological consequences of unexplained arousal: A posthypnotic suggestion paradigm. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 466–473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×