Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-xq9c7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-16T07:42:50.178Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Commentary

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2010

Carol M. Worthman
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Paul M. Plotsky
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Daniel S. Schechter
Affiliation:
Hôpitaux Universitaires de Genève
Constance A. Cummings
Affiliation:
Foundation for Psychocultural Research, California
Get access

Summary

Animal models of stress-related mechanisms in psychopathology have enormous potential. Compared to human studies, advantages include that the nature and timing of stressors can be systematically varied; the effects over substantial proportions of the subjects' short lives and over generations can be studied within the duration of a single research grant; and hypothesized neuroanatomical and neurochemical pathways can be examined directly. The research described in Dr. Huhman's chapter realizes this potential in many respects. The challenge is to determine what we can learn from these models. Enumerating the differences between Syrian hamsters and humans might lead to the immediate answer “not much.” Equally, the consistency of findings from studies of the key neuronal circuits in fear conditioning – involving not only Syrian hamsters, but many other animal species and humans – would suggest we can learn a great deal. This review considers some of the issues.

THE INTERPLAY BETWEEN SOCIAL EXPERIENCE AND BIOLOGY

Over a relatively short time, animal studies have informed a transformation from the separate study of the social and the biological to integrated study of the two levels of analysis. It is now well established across several mammalian species that social experiences often have long-term behavioral and biological effects, many mediated by variations in gene expression, and that these occur only over particular, sensitive periods in development (see Wiedenmayer; Szyf et al., this volume).

Type
Chapter
Information
Formative Experiences
The Interaction of Caregiving, Culture, and Developmental Psychobiology
, pp. 443 - 450
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bjorkqvist, K. (2001). Social defeat as a stressor in humans. Physiology & Behavavior, 73, 435–442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. (2008). The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex: functional contributions and dysfunction in psychopathy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363, 2557–2565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolton, D. & Hill, J. (2004). Mind, meaning and mental disorder. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colvert, E., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Groothues, C., Hawkins, A. et al. (2008). Emotional difficulties in early adolescence following severe early deprivation: findings from the English and Romanian adoptees study. Developmental Psychopathology, 20, 547–567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, P. T., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (2007). The role of child adrenocortical functioning in pathways between interparental conflict and child maladjustment. Developmental Psycholology, 43, 918–930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., Glynn, L. M., Schetter, C. D., Hobel, C., Chicz-Demet, A., & Sandman, C. A. (2007). Prenatal exposure to maternal depression and cortisol influences infant temperament. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 737–746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P. (2006). Evolution and depression: issues and implications. Psychological Medicine, 36, 287–297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P. & Allan, S. (1998). The role of defeat and entrapment (arrested flight) in depression: An exploration of an evolutionary view. Psychological Medicine, 28, 585–598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P., Gilbert, J., & Irons, C. (2004). Life events, entrapments and arrested anger in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 79, 149–160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, J., Murray, L., Leidecker, V., & Sharp, H. (2008). The dynamics of threat, fear and intentionality in the conduct disorders: Longitudinal findings in the children of women with post-natal depression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363(1503), 2529–2541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, J., Pickles, A., Burnside, E., Byatt, M., Rollinson, L., Davis, R. et al. (2001). Child sexual abuse, poor parental care and adult depression: Evidence for different mechanisms. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 104–109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodges, J. & Tizard, B. (1989). Social and family relationships of ex-institutional adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 30, 77–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huhman, K. L. (2006). Social conflict models: Can they inform us about human psychopathology?Hormones & Behavior, 50, 640–646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Taylor, A. (2004). Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: Evidence of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 44–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., & Gardner, C. O. (2001). Genetic risk, number of previous depressive episodes, and stressful life events in predicting onset of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 582–586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B., Newcombe, R., Craig, I. W. et al. (2006). MAOA, maltreatment, and gene-environment interaction predicting children's mental health: New evidence and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 903–913.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meaney, M. J., Szyf, M., & Seckl, J. R. (2007). Epigenetic mechanisms of perinatal programming of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function and health. Trends in Molecular Medicine, 13, 269–277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer–Lindenberg, A., Buckholtz, J. W., Kolachana, B., Hariri, R., Pezawas, L., Blasi, G. et al. (2006). Neural mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and violence in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A, 103, 6269–6274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nemeroff, C. B. (1998). The neurobiology of depression. Scientific American, 278, 42–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, T. G., Heron, J., Golding, J., & Glover, V. (2003). Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioural/emotional problems in children: A test of a programming hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 44, 1025–1036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A. & LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48, 175–187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblum, L. A., Forger, C., Noland, S., Trost, R. C., & Coplan, J. D. (2001). Response of adolescent bonnet macaques to an acute fear stimulus as a function of early rearing conditions. Developmental Psychobiology, 39, 40–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanchez, M. M., Ladd, C. O., & Plotsky, P. M. (2001). Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: Evidence from rodent and primate models. Developmental Psychopathology, 13, 419–449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suomi, S. J. (2006). Risk, resilience, and gene x environment interactions in rhesus monkeys. Annuals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 52–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarullo, A. R. & Gunnar, M. R. (2006). Child maltreatment and the developing HPA axis. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 632–639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goozen, S. H., Fairchild, G., Snoek, H., & Harold, G. T. (2007). The evidence for a neurobiological model of childhood antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 149–182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×