Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-c9gpj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T06:30:23.428Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

28 - Children’s Scientific Reasoning Skills in Light of General Cognitive Development

from Part III - Education and School-Learning Domains

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 February 2022

Olivier Houdé
Affiliation:
Université de Paris V
Grégoire Borst
Affiliation:
Université de Paris V
Get access

Summary

The desire to understand and explain patterns and regularities in terms of causal laws is part of human nature. This search for knowledge contributes to making the world predictable as well as controllable, and it enables the invention and production of technologies that help to improve quality-of-life. Although debates about criteria for good causal explanations and conclusive evidence are still a controversial topic of epistemology and theory of science, some widely accepted standards for scientific reasoning have emerged. The distinction between explanation and observation, and the control of variables strategy (CVS) for designing and evaluating experiments are two widely accepted concepts. Mastering the CVS means to vary only the levels of the focal variable in experimental designs while keeping all other potentially confounding variables constant across conditions.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arain, M., Haque, M., Johal, L., Mathur, P., Nel, W., Rais, A., … Sharma, S. (2013). Maturation of the adolescent brain. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 9, 449.Google ScholarPubMed
Barrouillet, P., & Lecas, J.-F. (1999). Mental models in conditional reasoning and working memory. Thinking & Reasoning, 5, 289302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1956). A Study of Thinking. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Bullock, M., & Ziegler, A. (2009). Scientific reasoning: Developmental and individual differences. In Weinert, F. E., & Schneider, W. (eds.), Individual Development from 13 to 22: Findings from the Munich Longitudinal Study (pp. 3854). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.Google Scholar
Chen, Z., & Klahr, D. (1999). All other things being equal: Acquisition and transfer of the control of variables strategy. Child Development, 70, 10981120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1998). An empirical test of a taxonomy of responses to anomalous data in science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 35, 623654.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, A. O., Breiner, K., Steinberg, L., Bonnie, R. J., Scott, E. S., Taylor-Thompson, K., … Silverman, M. R. (2016). When is an adolescent an adult? Assessing cognitive control in emotional and nonemotional contexts. Psychological Science, 27, 549562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, I. J., Whalley, L. J., Lemmon, H., Crawford, J. R., & Starr, J. M. (2000). The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: Follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence, 28, 4955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelsbrunner, P. A., & Dablander, F. (2019). The psychometric modeling of scientific reasoning: A review and recommendations for future avenues. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelsbrunner, P. A., Schalk, L., Schumacher, R., & Stern, E. (2019). Variable control and conceptual change: A large-scale quantitative study in elementary school. Learning and Individual Differences, 66, 3853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelmann, K., Neuhaus, B. J., & Fischer, F. (2016). Fostering scientific reasoning in education – meta-analytic evidence from intervention studies. Educational Research and Evaluation, 22, 333349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 11661186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofer, B. K. (2004). Epistemological understanding as a metacognitive process: Thinking aloud during online searching. Educational Psychologist, 39, 4355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofer, B. K., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). The development of epistemological theories: Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review of Educational Research, 67, 88140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofer, S. I., Schumacher, R., & Rubin, H. (2017). The test of basic mechanics conceptual understanding (bMCU): Using Rasch analysis to develop and evaluate an efficient multiple-choice test on Newton’s mechanics. International Journal of STEM Education, 4, 120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofer, S. I., Schumacher, R., Rubin, H., & Stern, E. (2018). Enhancing physics learning with cognitively activating instruction: A quasi-experimental classroom intervention study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110, 11751191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houdé, O. (2000). Inhibition and cognitive development: Object, number, categorization, and reasoning. Cognitive Development, 15, 6373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houdé, O. (2019). 3-System Theory of the Cognitive Brain: A Post-Piagetian Approach. New York: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The Growth of Logical Thinking: From Childhood to Adolescence. Trans. A. Parsons, & S. Milgram. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitchner, K. S. (1983). Cognition, metacognition, and epistemic cognition. Human Development, 26, 222232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klahr, D., & Chen, Z. (2003). Overcoming the positive‐capture strategy in young children: Learning about indeterminacy. Child Development, 74, 12751296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klahr, D. & Dunbar, K. (1988). Dual space search during scientific reasoning. Cognitive Science, 12, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koerber, S., Mayer, D., Osterhaus, C., Schwippert, K., & Sodian, B. (2014). The development of scientific thinking in elementary school: A comprehensive inventory. Child Development, 86, 327336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koerber, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Nett, U. (2005). Scientific reasoning in young children: Preschoolers’ ability to evaluate covariation evidence. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 64, 141152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. In Neurath, O., Carnap, R., & Morris, C. (eds.), International Encyclopedia of Unified Science. Foundations of the Unity of Science (vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 1210). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96, 674689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 178181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D. (2006). Do cognitive changes accompany developments in the adolescent brain? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 5967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, D. (2007). Reasoning about multiple variables: Control of variables is not the only challenge. Science Education, 91, 710726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., Black, J., Keselman, A., & Kaplan, D. (2000). The development of cognitive skills to support inquiry learning. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 495523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., Iordanou, K., Pease, M., & Wirkala, C. (2008). Beyond control of variables: What needs to develop to achieve skilled scientific thinking? Cognitive Development, 23, 435451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D. & Pease, M. (2008). What needs to develop in the development of inquiry skills? Cognition and Instruction, 26, 512559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., Ramsey, S., & Arvidsson, T. S. (2015). Developing multivariable thinkers. Cognitive Development, 35, 92110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lederman, N. G. (1992). Students’ and teachers’ conceptions of the nature of science: A review of the research. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29, 331359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, X., & Lehman, J. D. (1999). Supporting learning of variable control in a computer-based biology environment: Effects of prompting college students to reflect on their own thinking. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36, 837858.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, R. F. Jr, Lorch, E. P., Calderhead, W. J., Dunlap, E. E., Hodell, E. C., & Freer, B. D. (2010). Learning the control of variables strategy in higher and lower achieving classrooms: Contributions of explicit instruction and experimentation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 90101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, R. F. Jr, Lorch, E. P., Freer, B. D., Dunlap, E. E., Hodell, E. C., & Calderhead, W. J. (2014). Using valid and invalid experimental designs to teach the control of variables strategy in higher and lower achieving classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 1835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, D., Sodian, B., Körber, S., & Schwippert, K. (2014). Scientific reasoning in elementary school children: Assessment and relations with cognitive abilities. Learning and Instruction, 29, 4355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, B. J., Croker, S., M., A., & Zimmerman, C. (2012). The emergence of scientific reasoning. In Current Topics in Children’s Learning and Cognition. Rijeka, Croatia: InTech.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1979). The concept of intelligence. Intelligence, 3, 217227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osterhaus, C., Koerber, S., & Sodian, B. (2017). Scientific thinking in elementary school: Children’s social cognition and their epistemological understanding promote experimentation skills. Developmental Psychology, 53, 450462CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1958). The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence: An Essay on the Construction of Formal Operational Structures. Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Piekny, J., Grube, D., & Maehler, C. (2013). The relation between preschool children’s false-belief understanding and domain-general experimentation skills. Metacognition and Learning, 8, 103119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rey-Mermet, A., Gade, M., & Oberauer, K. (2018). Should we stop thinking about inhibition? Searching for individual and age differences in inhibition ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 501526.Google ScholarPubMed
Ross, J. A. (1988). Controlling variables: A meta-analysis of training studies. Review of Educational Research, 58, 405437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Olson, D. R., & Doherty, M. (1993). Reflecting on scientific thinking: Children’s understanding of the hypothesis‐evidence relation. Child Development, 64, 16171636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schalk, L., Edelsbrunner, P. A., Deiglmayr, A., Schumacher, R., & Stern, E. (2019). Improved application of the control-of-variables strategy as a collateral benefit of inquiry-based physics education in elementary school. Learning and Instruction, 59, 3445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 3157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schauble, L. (1996). The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts. Developmental Psychology, 32, 102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schauble, L., Klopfer, L. E., & Raghavan, K. (1991). Students’ transition from an engineering model to a science model of experimentation. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28, 859882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W., & Bullock, M. (Hrsg.). (2009). Human Development from Early Childhood to Early Adulthood: Findings from a 20 year Longitudinal Study. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Schommer, M., Calvert, C., Gariglietti, G., & Bajaj, A. (1997). The development of epistemological beliefs among secondary students: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 3740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwichow, M., Croker, S., Zimmerman, C., Höffler, T., & Härtig, H. (2015). Teaching the control-of-variables strategy: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 39, 3763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siler, S. A., & Klahr, D. (2012). Detecting, classifying, and remediating: Children’s explicit and implicit misconceptions about experimental design. In Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (eds.), Psychology of Science: Implicit and Explicit Processes (p. 137180). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siler, S. A., Klahr, D., Magaro, C., Willows, K., & Mowery, D. (2010). Predictors of transfer of experimental design skills in elementary and middle school children. In International Conference on Intelligent Tutoring Systems (pp. 198208). Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sodian, B., Zaitchik, D., & Carey, S. (1991). Young children’s differentiation of hypothetical beliefs from evidence. Child Development, 62, 753766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Somerville, S. C. (1974). The pendulum problem: Patterns of performance defining developmental stages. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 44, 266281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, E. (2005). Knowledge restructuring as a powerful mechanism of cognitive development: How to lay an early foundation for conceptual understanding in formal domains. BJEP Monograph Series II, Number 3-Pedagogy-Teaching for Learning, 155, 155170.Google Scholar
Stern, E. (2017). Individual differences in the learning potential of human beings. NPJ Science of Learning, 2, 2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strand-Cary, M., & Klahr, D. (2008). Developing elementary science skills: Instructional effectiveness and path independence. Cognitive Development, 23, 488511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Graaf, J., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). Scientific reasoning abilities in kindergarten: Dynamic assessment of the control of variables strategy. Instructional Science, 43, 381400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Graaf, J., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). Scientific reasoning in kindergarten: Cognitive factors in experimentation and evidence evaluation. Learning and Individual Differences, 49, 190200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Graaf, J., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2018). Individual differences in the development of scientific thinking in kindergarten. Learning and Instruction, 56, 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verschueren, N., Schaeken, W., & d’Ydewalle, G. (2005). A dual-process specification of causal conditional reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 11, 239278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vosniadou, S. (2014). Examining cognitive development from a conceptual change point of view: The framework theory approach. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 645661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vosniadou, S. & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagensveld, B., Segers, E., Kleemans, T., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). Child predictors of learning to control variables via instruction or self-discovery. Instructional Science, 43, 365379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P., Müller, U., Frye, D., Marcovitch, S., Argitis, G., Boseovski, J., … Carlson, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, I151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, C. (2000). The development of scientific reasoning skills. Developmental Review, 20, 99149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, C. (2007). The development of scientific thinking skills in elementary and middle school. Developmental Review, 27, 172223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zohar, A. & Dori, Y. J. (eds.). (2012). Metacognition in Science Education. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zohar, A., & Peled, B. (2008). The effects of explicit teaching of metastrategic knowledge on low- and high-achieving students. Learning and Instruction, 18, 337353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×