Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T13:47:34.127Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cross-sectional and prospective relationships of passive and mentally active sedentary behaviours and physical activity with depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 March 2019

Mats Hallgren*
Affiliation:
Assistant Professor, Department of Public Health Sciences, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
Thi-Thuy-Dung Nguyen
Affiliation:
Research Assistant, Department of Public Health Sciences, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
Neville Owen
Affiliation:
Professor and Head of the Behavioural Epidemiology Laboratory, Baker Heart and Diabetes Institute, Swinburne University of Technology, Australia
Brendon Stubbs
Affiliation:
Clinical Lecturer, Physiotherapy Department, South London and Maudsley National Health Service Foundation Trust; and Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Davy Vancampfort
Affiliation:
Lecturer, Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Leuven; and University Psychiatric Center, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Belgium
Andreas Lundin
Affiliation:
Project Coordinator, Department of Public Health Sciences, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
David Dunstan
Affiliation:
Professor and Head of the Physical Activity Laboratory, Baker Heart and Diabetes Institute; and Mary MacKillop Institute for Health Research, Australian Catholic University, Australia
Rino Bellocco
Affiliation:
Professor, Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden; and Department of Statistics and Quantitative Methods, University of Milano Bicocca, Italy
Ylva Trolle Lagerros
Affiliation:
Associate Professor, Obesity Center, Academic Specialist Center, Stockholm Health Services; and Clinical Epidemiology Unit, Department of Medicine, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
*
Correspondence: Mats Hallgren, Karolinska Institutet, Solnavägen 1e, Level 6, 113 65 Solna, Sweden. Email: mats.hallgren@ki.se
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Sedentary behaviour can be associated with poor mental health, but it remains unclear whether all types of sedentary behaviour have equivalent detrimental effects.

Aims

To model the potential impact on depression of replacing passive with mentally active sedentary behaviours and with light and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. An additional aim was to explore these relationships by self-report data and clinician diagnoses of depression.

Method

In 1997, 43 863 Swedish adults were initially surveyed and their responses linked to patient registers until 2010. The isotemporal substitution method was used to model the potential impact on depression of replacing 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with equivalent durations of mentally active sedentary behaviour, light physical activity or moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. Outcomes were self-reported depression symptoms (cross-sectional analyses) and clinician-diagnosed incident major depressive disorder (MDD) (prospective analyses).

Results

Of 24 060 participants with complete data (mean age 49.2 years, s.d. 15.8, 66% female), 1526 (6.3%) reported depression symptoms at baseline. There were 416 (1.7%) incident cases of MDD during the 13-year follow-up. Modelled cross-sectionally, replacing 30 min/day of passive sedentary behaviour with 30 min/day of mentally active sedentary behaviour, light physical activity and moderate-to-vigorous activity reduced the odds of depression symptoms by 5% (odds ratio 0.95, 95% CI 0.94–0.97), 13% (odds ratio 0.87, 95% CI 0.76–1.00) and 19% (odds ratio 0.81, 95% CI 0.93–0.90), respectively. Modelled prospectively, substituting 30 min/day of passive with 30 min/day of mentally active sedentary behaviour reduced MDD risk by 5% (hazard ratio 0.95, 95% CI 0.91–0.99); no other prospective associations were statistically significant.

Conclusions

Substituting passive with mentally active sedentary behaviours, light activity or moderate-to-vigorous activity may reduce depression risk in adults.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2019

Physical activity, sedentary behaviour and depression

Low levels of physical activity are consistently associated with an elevated risk of depression,Reference Schuch, Vancampfort, Firth, Rosenbaum, Ward and Silva1 whereas structured forms of physical activity and exercise programmes can be effective in the treatment of mood disorders.Reference Cooney, Dwan, Greig, Lawlor, Rimer and Waugh2 When adults are not being physically active, they can spend a great deal of their time in sedentary behaviours (sitting). People with depression spend significantly more time sitting than do those without depression.Reference Vancampfort, Firth, Schuch, Rosenbaum, Mugisha and Hallgren3 Conceptually and practically, sitting time may be considered to be distinct from being physically inactive (i.e. engaging in <150 min of moderate-intensity physical activity per week), with the term sedentary behaviour referring to any waking activity characterised by an energy expenditure of ≤1.5 metabolic equivalents in a seated or reclining posture.Reference Tremblay, Aubert, Barnes, Saunders, Carson and Latimer-Cheung4 Examples of these ubiquitous daily behaviours include watching TV, office work and driving. Extended periods of time spent in sedentary behaviour have been linked to increased risk of diabetes, cardiovascular disease and premature mortality,Reference Young, Hivert, Alhassan, Camhi, Ferguson and Katzmarzyk5,Reference Dempsey, Owen, Yates, Kingwell and Dunstan6 and these associations have been observed after controlling for time spent in leisure-time, moderate-to-vigorous physical activity.Reference Ekelund, Steene-Johannessen, Brown, Fagerland, Owen and Powell7 There are also relationships between sedentary behaviour and adverse mental health outcomes. In a meta-analysis, the risk of depression from sedentary behaviour was 31% higher over 13 cross-sectional studies, and 14% higher over 11 prospective studies.Reference Zhai, Zhang and Zhang8 Two recent trials demonstrated that experimentally induced sedentary behaviour can have adverse effects on mood and depression.Reference Edwards and Loprinzi9,Reference Endrighi, Steptoe and Hamer10 In one trial, a 32 min/day increase in sedentary behaviour over 2 weeks resulted in mood disturbances that were independent of changes in physical activity.Reference Endrighi, Steptoe and Hamer10

Passive and mentally-active sedentary behaviours

However, it remains unclear whether all types of sedentary behaviour can have equivalent detrimental effects on mental health. Some sedentary behaviours are characterised by cognitive effort (e.g. desk-based office work), whereas others primarily involve more passive mental activity (e.g. watching TV). This distinction has been made previouslyReference Kikuchi, Inoue, Sugiyama, Owen, Oka and Nakaya11 but not in the context of depressive symptoms or disorders. Given the nature of depression, which is associated with cognitive deficits, behavioural inactivation and higher than average durations of physical inactivity,Reference Schuch, Vancampfort, Firth, Rosenbaum, Ward and Reichert12 it is plausible that some sedentary behaviours – particularly those that are passive – may increase the risk of depression more than others. In a 2-year prospective study examining associations of watching TV, internet use and reading with mental health, TV-watching time at baseline (≥6 v. <2 h) was associated with more depressive symptoms and worse global cognitive functioning, whereas internet use and reading were associated with fewer depressive symptoms.Reference Hamer and Stamatakis13 Using the same data reported here, we previously examined longitudinal relationships of passive and mentally active sedentary behaviours with incident major depressive disorder (MDD) in 37 504 adults.Reference Hallgren, Owen, Stubbs, Zeebari, Vancampfort and Schuch14 After adjustment for relevant covariates, including physical activity, engaging in mentally active sedentary behaviours for ≥3 h/day (compared with <3 h/day) was associated with significantly reduced hazards of developing a depressive illness over 13 years. Conversely, a non-significant inverse (i.e. detrimental) association was found for time spent in passive sedentary behaviours, suggesting possible differential effects on depression. Although our previous findings on associations of sedentary behaviours with depression are informative, they do not specifically identify the benefits that might arise if other activities are substituted. Whereas several studies have independently shown the harms associated with too much sitting,Reference Young, Hivert, Alhassan, Camhi, Ferguson and Katzmarzyk5,Reference Dempsey, Owen, Yates, Kingwell and Dunstan6 none have examined the effects of replacing passive with mentally active sedentary behaviours in the context of depression. Revealing the intricacy of these relationships could have public health and clinical relevance.

Additional prospective studies using clinician diagnoses of depression are needed to reduce the misclassification bias that arises when depression is self-rated. Equally, however, self-report questionnaires are relevant as they may capture sub-threshold symptoms which are perceived as distressing and could precede the onset of major depression. Thus, comparing self-reported symptoms of depression with clinician diagnoses may provide a more complete and clinically relevant picture of these relationships. Our aim was to model the potential impact on depression of substituting short (30 min) durations of passive sedentary behaviour with equivalent durations of mentally active sedentary behaviour, light physical activity and moderate-to-vigorous activity. The primary outcomes were self-reported frequent symptoms of depression (cross-sectional analyses) and clinician-diagnosed incident MDD (prospective analyses). Associations were examined using both cross-sectional and longitudinal data.

Method

Participants

Data originate from the Swedish National March Cohort (http://ki.se/en/meb/the-swedish-national-march-cohort-nmc),Reference Lagerros, Hantikainen, Mariosa, Ye, Adami and Grotta15 a 4-day national fundraising event arranged by the Swedish Cancer Society in some 3600 Swedish cities and villages in September 1997. In total, 43 863 participants completed a 36-page survey with detailed questions about health behaviours and lifestyle, including specific questions on physical activity habits (type, frequency and duration). Reliability and validity findings for the activity questionnaire have been published previously and the survey has been used extensively.Reference Lagerros, Bellocco, Adami and Nyren16 Exclusion criteria included participants who were younger than 18 years at the beginning of the follow-up (n = 1741), those who emigrated (n = 465) or died (n = 8), or those who had a primary diagnosis of any mental disorder (ICD-8 [1967]: 290–315; ICD-9 [1978]: 290–319; ICD-10 [1992]: F00–F99) (n = 1089) before the beginning of the follow-up. After excluding these people, the sample eligible for follow-up was 40 569 participants. For the current analyses, only complete cases were included where participants provided data for all exposures and covariates (n = 24 060). In prospective analyses, to examine only incident cases we further excluded 1526 participants based on the presence of self-reported frequent symptoms of depression at baseline (n = 22 534). The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human participants were approved by the Research Ethics Vetting Board in Stockholm. Verbal informed consent was obtained from all participants (witnessed and formally recorded). The cohort and data collection method is described in a previous related paper.Reference Hallgren, Owen, Stubbs, Zeebari, Vancampfort and Schuch14

Study outcomes: self-reported frequent symptoms of depression and clinician-diagnosed MDD

Depression was assessed in two ways: in cross-sectional analyses, baseline depression was assessed using the self-rated question, ‘How often do you feel sad, low spirited, depressed?’ (with the possible responses ‘never, sometimes, often, always’). The last two response alternatives (often, always) were categorised as having frequent symptoms of depression (for brevity, also referred to as ‘symptoms of depression’). For prospective analyses, the occurrences of incident MDD (ICD codes: F32.0, F32.1, F32.2, F32.8, F32.9, F33.0, F33.1, F33.2, F33.4, F33.8 and F33.9) during the 13-year follow-up to 31 December 2010 were ascertained through linkages to existing nationwide, complete and continuously updated specialist medical registers, including in-patient and out-patient records. All diagnoses were made by a specialist clinician, often a psychiatrist or clinical psychologist. Accurate linkages, and thus essentially complete follow-up, were attained by using the individually unique National Registration Numbers assigned to all Swedish residents as identifiers both in the baseline questionnaire and in all registers. Currently, there is no nationwide primary healthcare register in Sweden.

Exposures: sedentary behaviours and physical activity

Four categories were assessed: (a) passive sedentary behaviours, (b) mentally active sedentary behaviours, (c) light physical activity and (d) moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. The last two activity categories were obtained from separate questions within the baseline questionnaire, as described below. For prospective analyses, total activity was calculated by summing time spent in these four activities, as described below. To facilitate interpretation of the results, and in line with previous studies,Reference Endrighi, Steptoe and Hamer10,Reference Yasunaga, Shibata, Ishii, Koohsari and Oka17 we examined the associations with depression of replacing 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with equivalent durations of mentally active sedentary behaviour, light physical activity and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (defined below). This duration (30 min) was also chosen for practical reasons; longer duration changes in activity could have stronger effects, but may not be feasible for most people.

Two questions assessed participation in passive and mentally active sedentary behaviours. Both were prefaced with the following: ‘How physically active are you on an ordinary weekday? Specifically, how much time per day/night do you devote to activities that require effort similar to: (a) watching TV, listening to music, sitting in the bathtub?’ (passive sedentary behaviours), and ‘(b) office work, sitting in a meeting, knitting/sewing?’ (mentally active sedentary behaviours). Each activity category was illustrated with a relevant visual image. For each question, eight response alternatives were provided to estimate the amount of time typically spent in each activity (in minutes): 0–4, 5–9, 10–19, 20–39, 40–89, 90–179, 180–359 and 360–720. The midpoint of each response was calculated and added to determine a continuous sedentary behaviour score for each activity.

The average weekly duration of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity was estimated by asking participants how much time per week they usually spent in ‘exercise, athletics and sports’, including (a) walking, (b) strenuous exercise (e.g. jogging, swimming) and (c) hard training/competition, each rated separately. The question implied that these activities were undertaken in a purposeful or structured manner; thus walking was included in the definition of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity, as previously recommended.18 For each question, there were six response alternatives: 0, 0–1, 2, 3, 4 and ≥5 h per week. Ratings were made separately for summer and winter and then averaged. Hours per week were converted into minutes: 0, 30, 120, 180, 240 and 300 min, respectively. After adding the total number of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity minutes, participants were categorised as ‘below’ (0–149 min), ‘achieving’ (150–299 min) or ‘exceeding’ (≥300 min) the World Health Organization (WHO)-recommended durations. The last category is recommended for attaining additional health benefits from physical activity,18 but has rarely been assessed in previous studies. The method used to calculate moderate-to-vigorous physical activity is comparable to recent studies using metabolic equivalents of task (METs)-minutes.Reference McDowell, Dishman, Vancampfort, Hallgren, Stubbs and MacDonncha19 Both the physical activity and sedentary behaviour questions have been validated and used extensively in previous studies.Reference Lagerros, Bellocco, Adami and Nyren16,Reference Lagerros, Mucci, Bellocco, Nyren, Balter and Balter20,Reference Bellocco, Jia, Ye and Lagerros21

Covariates

Based on previous evidence of association with sedentary behaviour and/or depression,Reference Zhai, Zhang and Zhang8 the following variables were included in the statistical models.

  1. (a) Body mass index (BMI): calculated from self-reported weight and height (kg/m2) and then categorised according to the WHO's BMI classification for adults, i.e. not overweight (<25), overweight (25 to <30) and obese (≥30). Because of the small number of cases of underweight (1.3%), this group was collapsed with the first category.

  2. (b) Education: assessed by a question about the type of education/school attended. Participants were grouped into four categories: compulsory school (year 9), upper secondary (years 10–12), tertiary, and vocational and other.

  3. (c) Smoking status: assessed by asking participants if they had ever smoked cigarettes for 6 months or more. Those answering yes were coded as ‘ever smoked’.

  4. (d) Comorbidities: assessed based on whether or not the following 12 self-reported conditions had been treated by a medical doctor: asthma, heart attack, high blood pressure, angina pectoris, angina pectoris in legs (claudication), lipid disturbance, stroke, rheumatoid arthritis, tuberculosis, cancer, diabetes and multiple sclerosis. A total score was determined by adding each condition.

  5. (e) Age and gender: age was categorised into three groups based on the distribution of data (<45, 45–59 and ≥60 years); gender was considered a confounder due to the reported gender differences in depression.

Statistical analyses

Baseline characteristics were calculated using descriptive statistics (mean, median, s.d.).

Isotemporal substitution modelling (ISM)Reference Mekary, Willett, Hu and Ding22 has been used previously to estimate the effects of replacing different durations of physical activity on body weight and the risk of chronic disease.Reference Mekary, Willett, Hu and Ding22Reference Ryan, Wellburn, McDonough, Martin and Batterham24 Compared with conventional regression modelling, ISM can provide a more accurate estimation of the potential effects of different activities. Within total waking hours, time spent in one type of activity usually occurs at the expense of time engaged in related activities; ISM uniquely enables examination of the potential impact on depression of substituting one type of activity with another. Given these advantages, ISM was used to assess the associations with depression of replacing 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with 30 min of (a) mentally active sedentary behaviour, (b) light physical activity and (c) moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. Associations were examined cross-sectionally by using self-reported frequent symptoms of depression at baseline, and longitudinally by using clinician-diagnosed MDD as outcome. For cross-sectional analyses, logistic regression was used to calculate odds ratios, 95% confidence intervals and P-values. For longitudinal analyses, Cox proportional hazards regression analyses were used to calculate hazard ratios and associated confidence intervals. Survival time was censored at the date of death from all causes or at the end of the follow-up for those who did not have MDD. For both sets of analyses, three models are reported: single, partition and substitution models. The three models are briefly explained below using logistic regression as an example. The same principles were applied for Cox regression models. For brevity, we use PA for physical activity and SB for sedentary behaviour in these equations.

Single models

Single models assessed the association of each type of activity with depression (as defined previously), adjusting for confounders: Log (odds depression) = β 0 + β 1*passive SB + β 2*covariates.

Partition models

Partition models assessed the association between each type of activity and depression adjusting for confounders, while keeping other activities constant: Log (odds depression) = β 0 + β 1*passive SB + β 2*mentally active SB + β 3*light PA + β 4*moderate-to-vigorous PA + β 6*covariates. As total activity is not controlled in the partition model, the beta-coefficient of each activity represents the additive effect of these activities on depression, not the substitutive effects.

Substitution models

Substitution models assessed the effect of replacing 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with 30 min of mentally active sedentary behaviour, light physical activity and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity: Log (odds depression) =  β 0 + β 2*mentally active SB + β 3*light PA + β 4* moderate-to-vigorous PA + β 5*total activity + β 6*covariates. In the substitution models, passive sedentary behaviour is dropped, but total activity (that is, all sedentary behaviour and physical activity) is retained. Because total activity is held constant, a 30 min increase in mentally active sedentary behaviour results in an equivalent decrease in activities not included in the model (i.e. passive sedentary behaviours), Thus, the beta-coefficients β 2, β 3 and β 4 can be interpreted as the effect on depression of replacing 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with the equivalent duration of mentally active sedentary behaviour, light physical activity and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity, respectively. Before running the prospective models (Cox regression) we used Schoenfeld residuals to test the assumption of proportional hazards for each covariate adjusting for other covariates in the model. There was no evidence for a violation of the assumption.

Results

Participant characteristics

Participant characteristics are shown in Table 1: 66% were female (mean age 49.2 years, s.d. 15.8), 30% had a tertiary education, 39% were overweight or obese, 39% reported having ever smoked cigarettes (≥6 months) and 30% had one or more comorbidity. Participants reported approximately 5.5 h/day in total sedentary behaviours and 37 min/day in light physical activity and moderate-to-vigorous activity combined. Of the total sample, 6.3% reported having frequent symptoms of depression at baseline and 1.7% was diagnosed with MDD over the 13-year follow-up. Among the 22 534 participants who did not report symptoms of depression at baseline, 320 (1.4%) developed MDD during the course of follow-up. There was a significant inverse association between the passive and mentally active sedentary behaviours (Spearman's rho = −0.071, P < 0.01). There were some differences between the analytic and excluded sample (i.e. those with missing data on covariates). Specifically, the excluded sample included more participants that were male, elderly, obese, smoked, had one or more comorbidity, less formal education and spent more time in passive sedentary behaviours. A detailed comparison is available in Supplementary Table 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.60.

Table 1 Characteristics of participants

a. Total could be over/below 100% due to rounding.

Cross-sectional associations with self-reported frequent symptoms of depression

Table 2 shows associations between self-reported symptoms of depression, with sedentary behaviours, light physical activity and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. The single models indicate the association between each type of activity and symptoms of depression adjusting for confounders (listed above). Passive sedentary behaviours significantly increased the odds of reporting depressive symptoms. A 30 min increase in light physical activity and moderate-to-vigorous activity reduced the odds of having symptoms of depression by 23% (odds ratio 0.87, 95% CI 0.77–0.99), and 28% (odds ratio 0.82, 95% CI 0.74–0.91), respectively. In the partition model, a 30 min increase in passive sedentary behaviour increased the odds of depressive symptoms by 6% (odds ratio 1.06, 95% CI 1.04–1.07); conversely, and a 30 min increase in mentally active sedentary behaviour reduced the odds of depressive symptoms by 14% (odds ratio 0.86, 95% CI 0.77–0.95). In the substitution model, replacing 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with 30 min of mentally active sedentary behaviour, light physical activity and moderate-to-vigorous activity significantly reduced the odds of depressive symptoms by 5% (odds ratio 0.95, 95% CI 0.94–0.97), 13% (odds ratio 0.87, 95% CI 0.76–1.00) and 19% (odds ratio 0.81, 95% CI 0.93–0.90), respectively. Thus there was a dose-response relationship where substituting passive sedentary behaviours with higher-intensity activities had a greater benefit on depressive symptoms.

Table 2 Odds ratios for self-reported depression when substituting 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with other types of activity (n = 24 060; cases of depression 1526)

a. Substituting passive sedentary behaviour (SB) with other activities: Log (odds depression symptom) = β 0 + β 2*mentally active SB + β 3*light activity + β 4*moderate-to-vigorous activity + β 5*total activity + β 6*covariates.

b. Additive effect of each activity on depression, holding other activities constant: Log (odds depression symptom) =  β 0 + β 1*passive SB + β 2*mentally active SB + β 3*light activity + β 4*moderate-to-vigorous activity + β 6*covariates.

c. Single effect of each activity on depression.

d. Log (odds depression symptom) = β 0 + β 1*passive SB + β 6*covariates.

e. Log (odds depression symptom) = β 0 + β 2*mentally active SB + β 6*covariates.

f. Log (odds depression symptom) = β 0 + β 3*light activity + β 6*covariates.

g. Log (odds depression symptom) = β 0 + β 4*moderate-to-vigorous activity + β 6*covariates.

h. Log (odds depression symptom) = β 0 + β 5*total activity + β 6*covariates).

* P < 0.05.

** P < 0.01.

*** P < 0.001.

Prospective associations with clinician-diagnosed MDD

In prospective analyses (Table 3) using substitution models, replacing 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with 30 min of mentally active sedentary behaviour reduced the risk of clinician-diagnosed MDD by 5% (hazard ratio 0.95, 95% CI 0.91–0.99). No other prospective associations were statistically significant.

Table 3 Hazard ratios for incident major depressive disorder when substituting 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with other types of activity (n = 22 534; cases of major depressive disorder 320)

a. Substituting passive sedentary behaviour (SB) with other activities: h(t) = h 0(t) exp (β 2*mentally active SB + β 3*light activity + β 4*moderate-to-vigorous activity + β 5*total activity + β 6*covariates).

b. Additive effect of each activity on depression, holding other activities constant: h(t) = h 0(t) exp (β 1*passive SB + β 2*mentally active SB + β 3*light activity + β 4*moderate-to-vigorous activity + β 6*covariates).

c. Single effect of each activity on depression.

d. h(t) = h 0(t) exp (β 1*passive SB + β 6*covariates).

e. h(t) = h 0(t) exp (β 2*mentally active SB + β 6*covariates).

f. h(t) = h 0(t) exp (β 3*light activity + β 6*covariates).

g. h(t) = h 0(t) exp (β 4*moderate-to-vigorous activity + β 6*covariates).

h. h(t) = h 0(t) exp (β 5*total activity + β 6*covariates).

* P < 0.05.

Discussion

In the context of emerging research demonstrating links between sedentary behaviour and mood disorders,Reference Hamer and Stamatakis25Reference Mekary, Lucas, Pan, Okereke, Willett and Hu27 this is the first study to examine the potential impact on depression of replacing passive with mentally active sedentary behaviours. In both cross-sectional and prospective analyses, substituting 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with 30 min of mentally active sedentary behaviour reduced the odds of depressive symptoms and clinician-diagnosed MDD by 5%, respectively. Cross-sectionally, compared with replacement with mentally active sedentary behaviours (5%), larger magnitude effects on depressive symptoms were observed when replacing passive sedentary behaviours with light physical activity (23% lower odds) and moderate-to-vigorous activity (28% lower odds). Consistent with our previous work,Reference Hallgren, Owen, Stubbs, Zeebari, Vancampfort and Schuch14 these findings suggest that passive sedentary behaviours may heighten the risk of depression in adults. The current study adds the observation that substituting common passive sedentary behaviours with mentally active sedentary behaviours, or (preferably) with light physical activity or moderate-to-vigorous activity, may reduce depressive symptoms in adults.

Previous longitudinal studies have consistently shown beneficial relationships between moderate-to-vigorous physical activity and lower risk of depression.Reference Schuch, Vancampfort, Firth, Rosenbaum, Ward and Silva1 Unlike the cross-sectional results, no significant associations were found with moderate-to-vigorous activity in the prospective substitution models. Differences between the cross-sectional and prospective models may be attributable to several factors, including the relatively low incidence rate of MDD in the current study. Diagnoses were obtained from specialist in-patient and out-patient healthcare registers, not from primary care, where some individuals with mildly severe depression (yet still meeting the criteria for MDD) could initially seek treatment. Thus the incidence rate of MDD may have been underestimated slightly, making it less likely to observe an association where one may exist. An alternative interpretation could be that the beneficial substitution effects seen here do not occur at higher ‘thresholds’ of depression; in this case MDD diagnosed by a specialist clinician. Despite these inconsistencies, the cross-sectional self-report data offers a relevant perspective by taking into account self-rated symptoms of depression, which are prevalent in the general population and frequently precede the onset of major depression.Reference Horwath, Johnson, Klerman and Weissman28 Therefore, the cross-sectional findings could be particularly relevant from a prevention perspective. Although the associations of replacing passive sedentary behaviours with moderate-to-vigorous physical activity were not statistically significant in longitudinal analyses, the direction of these relationships was as predicted (i.e. beneficial). Moreover, there is evidence from previous prospective studies and controlled trials indicating the benefits of moderate-intensity physical activity on both depression and general somatic health.Reference Cooney, Dwan, Greig, Lawlor, Rimer and Waugh2,Reference Schuch, Vancampfort, Firth, Rosenberg, Ward and Silva29 Taken together, and seen in the context of existing research, these findings suggest potential mental health benefits of replacing passive sedentary behaviours with moderate-to-vigorous physical activity.

Our findings are consistent with recent studies demonstrating beneficial associations of light physical activity and lower levels of sedentary behaviour with depression.Reference Zhai, Zhang and Zhang8,Reference Loprinzi30 Two previous studies have used ISM to explore relationships between sedentary behaviour and depression. In a recent cross-sectional study involving 276 older adults, using objective measures of activity, Yasunaga et al (2018)Reference Yasunaga, Shibata, Ishii, Koohsari and Oka17 found that replacing 30 min/day of sedentary behaviour with 30 min/day of light physical activity was negatively associated with self-rated depression (β = −0.131, 95% CI −0.260 to −0.002). Mekary et al (2013)Reference Mekary, Lucas, Pan, Okereke, Willett and Hu27 prospectively examined the associations of different activities with various activity displacements and depression risk among 32 900 women from the USA over 10 years. An isotemporal substitution gradient was found for watching TV, such that replacing 60 min/day of this activity with 60 min/day of brisk walking was associated with lower depression risk. However, a similar ‘protective’ association was not seen when watching TV was replaced with slow walking, which could indicate that a minimum physical activity ‘dose’ is required to elicit these effects.Reference Mekary, Lucas, Pan, Okereke, Willett and Hu27 In the current study, similar beneficial associations with self-reported symptoms of depression were seen for replacing passive sedentary behaviours with walking.

There are several plausible explanations for the differential effects of passive and mentally active sedentary behaviours on depression. One explanation relates to the context of these activities. Office work and ‘sitting in a meeting’ (both assessed here) usually occur in work environments. Employment is linked to better mental health – even when it involves sedentary behaviour – as it can promote a sense of autonomy, belonging and achievement. Work can also foster supportive social relationships. Thus, the negative mood states associated with passive sedentary behaviours could potentially heighten the risk of depression more than mentally active sedentary behaviours, despite equivalent energy expenditure. We also speculate that substituting passive with mentally active sedentary behaviours might reduce negative rumination which, in turn, may counteract the vicious cycle of maladaptive cognitions often seen in people with depression. Other physiological mechanisms could also underlie these relationships: sedentary behaviours adversely affect glycaemic control and evidence suggests that glycaemic variability may influence brain health and cognition.Reference Wheeler, Dempsey, Grace, Ellis, Gardiner and Green31 However, it remains to be seen whether or not this variability is linked to different types of sedentary behaviour.

Substituting passive sedentary behaviour with light or moderate physical activity could reduce depression through several related mechanisms. Physical activity has been shown to upregulate monoamine neurotransmission in the animal brain; changes which may be linked to mood disorders in humans.Reference Dishman32 Exercise also appears to regulate the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, leading to reductions in glucocorticoid stress hormones.Reference Portugal, Cevada, Monteiro, Guimaraes, Rubini and Lattari33 Research supports the role of inflammation, oxidative and nitrogen stress and neurotrophins as key mediators in the pathogenesis of mood disorders.Reference Moylan, Eyre, Maes, Baune, Jacka and Berk34 Some studies suggest that higher doses of physical activity are needed to elicit these biological mechanisms. However, in a recent 12-week, community-based, randomised controlled trial of exercise for mild-to-moderate depression in adults we observed equivalent magnitude effects of light, moderate and vigorous exercise on self-rated depression severity.Reference Hallgren, Helgadottir, Herring, Zeebari, Lindefors and Kaldo35 The largest absolute improvement in depressive symptoms was seen in the light exercise group,Reference Helgadottir, Hallgren, Ekblom and Forsell36 suggesting that low-intensity exercise can also have beneficial effects on depressive symptoms. Psychosocial factors are also relevant; exercise can act as a distraction from stressful life events, improve self-esteem and may reduce negative attentional biases.

Our distinction between passive and mentally active sedentary behaviours is relatively new, although its importance has been recognised in at least one previous study.Reference Kikuchi, Inoue, Sugiyama, Owen, Oka and Nakaya11 Kikuchi et al (2014) examined cross-sectional relationships of passive (TV time, listening or talking while sitting and sitting around) and mentally active (computer use and reading books or newspapers) leisure-time activities in older Japanese adults. Higher passive sedentary time was associated with greater odds of being overweight and engaging in lower levels of physical activity. Conversely, higher mentally active sedentary time was associated with lower odds of low physical activity.Reference Kikuchi, Inoue, Sugiyama, Owen, Oka and Nakaya11 Psychological outcomes were also examined. Higher passive sedentary time increased the odds of psychological distress (Kessler K6 Scale), but not after adjustment for moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. Other categories of sedentary behaviour have been explored. A meta-analysis examined possible differential effects of watching TV and computer or internet use on depression. Both outcomes were associated with a similar increased risk of depression. Unlike these previous investigations, the current study did not assess computer or mobile phone use which – although certainly relevant – could involve both passive and mentally active behaviours.

Strengths and limitations

This study has notable strengths. The analyses are based on a large participant sample and the comprehensive baseline survey enabled relevant covariates to be included in the fully adjusted models. The physical activity questionnaire has been validated in previous studiesReference Lagerros, Bellocco, Adami and Nyren16,Reference Lagerros, Mucci, Bellocco, Nyren, Balter and Balter20 and, importantly, included separate items assessing passive and mentally active sedentary behaviours. The analytic approach (ISM) might also be considered an advantage as it enables substitution effects to be examined. Some potential limitations are also acknowledged. The exposure was self-reported which may overestimate physical activity levels generally. One of the study outcomes (frequent symptoms of depression) was self-rated, based on a single item which has not been validated for this purpose, and cannot be regarded as equivalent to a clinician's diagnosis of major depression. The cohort displayed some characteristics which may not reflect the general Swedish adult population; for example, participants were more overweight than adults surveyed in national health surveys.37 Our reliance on clinician diagnoses of depression is a potential strength as it reduces misclassification bias but, as noted, the true incidence of MDD may have been underestimated slightly in prospective analyses. However, the specialist registers are widely used in Sweden for longitudinal research, including studies within psychiatry. To address the issue of reverse causality, those with indications of frequent depressive symptoms at baseline were removed from the prospective analyses. Mental ill health exists on a continuum and major depression is an episodic disorder, so our analytic approach could limit the generalisability of our findings to some degree. Finally, because of the age of the baseline data (1997) some relevant sedentary behaviours were not assessed (e.g. internet and smart phone use).

Clinical relevance and future research

These findings suggest that substituting passive with mentally active sedentary behaviours, light physical activity or moderate-to-vigorous activity may reduce feelings of depression in adults, which in turn could lower the risk of developing major depression. In the context of research showing that adults with depression are more sedentary than age- and gender-matched controlsReference Schuch, Vancampfort, Firth, Rosenbaum, Ward and Reichert12 and studies indicating detrimental links between sedentary behaviour and depression,Reference Zhai, Zhang and Zhang8 these results are also clinically relevant. They reinforce the notion that clinical interventions for adults reporting symptoms of depression may be enhanced by screening physical activity habits and promoting increased activity when levels fall below recommended guidelines.Reference Hallgren, Stubbs, Vancampfort, Lundin, Jaakallio and Forsell38 Such interventions should aim to increase total daily physical activity while also reducing sedentary behaviours, particularly passive behaviours.Reference Vancampfort, Firth, Schuch, Rosenbaum, Mugisha and Hallgren3,Reference Hallgren, Owen, Stubbs, Zeebari, Vancampfort and Schuch14 Currently, there is active discussion in the scientific literature regarding the optimal format of physical activity interventions and their structure within psychiatric settings.Reference Richardson, Faulkner, McDevitt, Skrinar, Hutchinson and Piette39,Reference Vancampfort, Stubbs, Ward, Teasdale and Rosenbaum40

There is an opportunity for future research to further elucidate the relationships that we have identified. Detrimental effects of sedentary behaviours on cardiovascular disease and mortality have been established, and emerging evidence of negative mental health consequences points to the need for further research in this context. Epidemiological investigations combining objective measurements of total sedentary time with self-report methods to identify which components are passive and mentally active would be informative. Intervention trials comparing the effects on depression of reducing passive sedentary behaviours versus increasing structured exercise would also be informative, as would trials in which physical activity is increased specifically by reducing both types of sedentary behaviour.

Funding

This work was supported by ICA AB, Telefonaktiebolaget LM Ericsson, the Swedish Cancer Society (grant CAN 2012/591), and funds from the regional agreement on medical training and clinical research between Stockholm County Council and Karolinska Institutet (Y.T.L.). The funders played no role in the design of the study, the data collection, the analysis, the decision to publish or the preparation of the manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We thank Statistics Sweden for scanning the questionnaires. We thank the Swedish Cancer Society and the volunteers who were involved in data collection.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.60.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest: None.

References

1Schuch, FB, Vancampfort, D, Firth, J, Rosenbaum, S, Ward, PB, Silva, ES, et al. Physical activity and incident depression: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175: 631–48.10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17111194CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2Cooney, GM, Dwan, K, Greig, CA, Lawlor, DA, Rimer, J, Waugh, FR, et al. Exercise for depression. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2013; 9: CD004366.Google Scholar
3Vancampfort, D, Firth, J, Schuch, FB, Rosenbaum, S, Mugisha, J, Hallgren, M, et al. Sedentary behavior and physical activity levels in people with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder: a global systematic review and meta-analysis. World Psychiatry 2017; 16: 308–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4Tremblay, MS, Aubert, S, Barnes, JD, Saunders, TJ, Carson, V, Latimer-Cheung, AE, et al. Sedentary Behavior Research Network (SBRN) – Terminology Consensus Project process and outcome. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2017; 14: 75.10.1186/s12966-017-0525-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5Young, DR, Hivert, MF, Alhassan, S, Camhi, SM, Ferguson, JF, Katzmarzyk, PT, et al. Sedentary behavior and cardiovascular morbidity and mortality a science advisory from the American Heart Association. Circulation 2016; 134: E26279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6Dempsey, PC, Owen, N, Yates, TE, Kingwell, BA, Dunstan, DW.Sitting less and moving more: improved glycaemic control for type 2 diabetes prevention and management. Curr Diab Rep 2016; 16: 114.10.1007/s11892-016-0797-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7Ekelund, U, Steene-Johannessen, J, Brown, WJ, Fagerland, MW, Owen, N, Powell, KE, et al. Does physical activity attenuate, or even eliminate, the detrimental association of sitting time with mortality? A harmonised meta-analysis of data from more than 1 million men and women. Lancet 2016; 388: 1302–10.10.1016/S0140-6736(16)30370-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Zhai, L, Zhang, Y, Zhang, D.Sedentary behaviour and the risk of depression: a meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med 2014; 49: 705–9.10.1136/bjsports-2014-093613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Edwards, MK, Loprinzi, PD.Effects of a sedentary behavior-inducing randomized controlled intervention on depression and mood profile in active young adults. Mayo Clin Proc 2016; 91: 984–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10Endrighi, R, Steptoe, A, Hamer, M.The effect of experimentally induced sedentariness on mood and psychobiological responses to mental stress. Br J Psychiatry 2016; 208: 245–51.10.1192/bjp.bp.114.150755CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Kikuchi, H, Inoue, S, Sugiyama, T, Owen, N, Oka, K, Nakaya, T, et al. Distinct associations of different sedentary behaviors with health-related attributes among older adults. Prev Med 2014; 67: 335–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12Schuch, F, Vancampfort, D, Firth, J, Rosenbaum, S, Ward, P, Reichert, T, et al. Physical activity and sedentary behavior in people with major depressive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord 2017; 210: 139–50.10.1016/j.jad.2016.10.050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13Hamer, M, Stamatakis, E.Prospective study of sedentary behavior, risk of depression, and cognitive impairment. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2014; 46: 718–23.10.1249/MSS.0000000000000156CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14Hallgren, M, Owen, N, Stubbs, B, Zeebari, Z, Vancampfort, D, Schuch, F, et al. Passive and mentally-active sedentary behaviors and incident major depressive disorder: a 13-year cohort study. J Affect Disord 2018; 241: 579–85.10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15Lagerros, YT, Hantikainen, E, Mariosa, D, Ye, WM, Adami, HO, Grotta, A, et al. Cohort profile: the Swedish National March cohort. Int J Epidemiol 2017; 46: 795.Google Scholar
16Lagerros, YT, Bellocco, R, Adami, HO, Nyren, O.Measures of physical activity and their correlates: the Swedish National March Cohort. Eur J Epidemiol 2009; 24: 161–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Yasunaga, A, Shibata, A, Ishii, K, Koohsari, J, Oka, K.Cross-sectional associations of sedentary behaviour and physical activity on depression in Japanese older adults: an isotemporal substitution approach. BMJ Open 8: e022282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18World Health Organization (WHO). Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health; World Health Organization Guidelines Approved by the Guidelines Review Committee. WHO, 2010.Google Scholar
19McDowell, CP, Dishman, RK, Vancampfort, D, Hallgren, M, Stubbs, B, MacDonncha, C, et al. Physical activity and generalized anxiety disorder: results from The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA). Int J Epidemiol 2018; 47: 1443–53.10.1093/ije/dyy141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20Lagerros, YT, Mucci, LA, Bellocco, R, Nyren, O, Balter, O, Balter, KA.Validity and reliability of self-reported total energy expenditure using a novel instrument. Eur J Epidemiol 2006; 21: 227–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Bellocco, R, Jia, C, Ye, W, Lagerros, YT.Effects of physical activity, body mass index, waist-to-hip ratio and waist circumference on total mortality risk in the Swedish National March Cohort. Eur J Epidemiol 2010; 25: 777–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22Mekary, RA, Willett, WC, Hu, FB, Ding, EL.Isotemporal substitution paradigm for physical activity epidemiology and weight change. Am J Epidemiol 2009; 170: 519–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23Healy, GN, Winkler, EAH, Owen, N, Anuradha, S, Dunstan, DW.Replacing sitting time with standing or stepping: associations with cardio-metabolic risk biomarkers. Eur Heart J 2015; 36: 2643–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24Ryan, CG, Wellburn, S, McDonough, S, Martin, DJ, Batterham, AM.The association between displacement of sedentary time and chronic musculoskeletal pain: an isotemporal substitution analysis. Physiotherapy 2017; 103: 471–7.10.1016/j.physio.2017.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25Hamer, M, Stamatakis, E.Sedentary behavior and risk of future depression and cognitive decline in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Psychol Health 2013; 28: 32.Google Scholar
26Teychenne, M, Ball, K, Salmon, J.Sedentary behavior and depression among adults: a review. Int J Behav Med 2010; 17: 246–54.10.1007/s12529-010-9075-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Mekary, RA, Lucas, M, Pan, A, Okereke, OI, Willett, WC, Hu, FB, et al. Isotemporal substitution analysis for physical activity, television watching, and risk of depression. Am J Epidemiol 2013; 178: 474–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28Horwath, E, Johnson, J, Klerman, GL, Weissman, MM.Depressive symptoms as relative and attributable risk-factors for 1st-onset major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49: 817–23.10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820100061011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29Schuch, FB, Vancampfort, D, Firth, J, Rosenberg, S, Ward, PB, Silva, ES, et al. Physical activity and incident depression: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175: 631–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30Loprinzi, PD.Objectively measured light and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity is associated with lower depression levels among older US adults. Aging Ment Health 2013; 17: 801–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31Wheeler, MJ, Dempsey, PC, Grace, MS, Ellis, KA, Gardiner, PA, Green, DJ, et al. Sedentary behavior as a risk factor for cognitive decline? A focus on the influence of glycemic control in brain health. Alzheimers Dement (N Y) 2017; 3: 291300.Google ScholarPubMed
32Dishman, RK.Brain monoamines, exercise, and behavioral stress: animal models. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1997; 29: 6374.10.1097/00005768-199701000-00010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33Portugal, EMM, Cevada, T, Monteiro, RS, Guimaraes, TT, Rubini, ED, Lattari, E, et al. Neuroscience of exercise: from neurobiology mechanisms to mental health. Neuropsychobiology 2013; 68: 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
34Moylan, S, Eyre, HA, Maes, M, Baune, BT, Jacka, FN, Berk, M.Exercising the worry away: how inflammation, oxidative and nitrogen stress mediates the beneficial effect of physical activity on anxiety disorder symptoms and behaviours. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37: 573–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35Hallgren, M, Helgadottir, B, Herring, MP, Zeebari, Z, Lindefors, N, Kaldo, V, et al. Exercise and internet-based cognitive-behavioural therapy for depression: multicentre randomised controlled trial with 12-month follow-up. Br J Psychiatry 2016; 209: 416–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36Helgadottir, B, Hallgren, M, Ekblom, O, Forsell, Y.Training fast or slow? Exercise for depression: a randomized controlled trial. Prev Med 2016; 91: 123–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37World Health Organization (WHO). Sweden Physical Activity Factsheet. World Health organization, 2014.Google Scholar
38Hallgren, M, Stubbs, B, Vancampfort, D, Lundin, A, Jaakallio, P, Forsell, Y.Treatment guidelines for depression: greater emphasis on physical activity is needed. Eur Psychiatry 2016; 40: 13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39Richardson, CR, Faulkner, G, McDevitt, J, Skrinar, GS, Hutchinson, DS, Piette, JD.Integrating physical activity into mental health services for persons with serious mental illness. Psychiatr Serv 2005; 56: 324–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40Vancampfort, D, Stubbs, B, Ward, PB, Teasdale, S, Rosenbaum, S.Integrating physical activity as medicine in the care of people with severe mental illness. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2015; 49: 681–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Characteristics of participants

Figure 1

Table 2 Odds ratios for self-reported depression when substituting 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with other types of activity (n = 24 060; cases of depression 1526)

Figure 2

Table 3 Hazard ratios for incident major depressive disorder when substituting 30 min of passive sedentary behaviour with other types of activity (n = 22 534; cases of major depressive disorder 320)

Supplementary material: File

Hallgren et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Hallgren et al. supplementary material(File)
File 17.9 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.