Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T09:25:16.914Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sex-biased sound symbolism in French first names

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 July 2019

Alexandre Suire*
Affiliation:
ISEM, University of Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, Montpellier, France
Alba Bossoms Mesa
Affiliation:
ISEM, University of Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, Montpellier, France
Michel Raymond
Affiliation:
ISEM, University of Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, Montpellier, France
Melissa Barkat-Defradas
Affiliation:
ISEM, University of Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, Montpellier, France
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: alexandre.suire@umontpellier.fr

Summary

Low- and high-frequency vowels in the stressed syllable of French first names may respectively project impressions of largeness/masculinity and smallness/femininity.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

Introduction

Arbitrariness, the notion that the sound and the meaning of a word are independent, has long been considered one of the most widely shared principles in linguistics. However, a growing body of evidence challenges this view, stating that there is a natural link between the sound units of a word – known as phonemes – and the mental image they evoke (see Svantesson Reference Svantesson2017 for an overview). This principle, referred to as sound symbolism, is well illustrated by the ‘kiki-bouba’ and ‘maluma-takete’ experiments, in which participants are asked to associate such non-words to two figures of different shapes: results show above-chance matchings of ‘bouba’ and ‘maluma’ with a round silhouette, and ‘kiki’ and ‘takete’ with a sharp one (Ramachandran and Hubbard Reference Ramachandran and Hubbard2001; Werner Reference Werner1957; Köhler Reference Köhler1947). Although it is uncertain to generalize the ‘kiki–bouba’ effect across cultures (see Bremner et al. Reference Bremner, Caparos, Davidoff, de Fockert, Linnell and Spence2013 and Cuskley et al. Reference Cuskley, Simner and Kirby2017), other similar sound-meaning mappings have been recorded in thousands of the world's languages, suggesting an underlying universal cognitive association mechanism (Blasi et al. Reference Blasi, Wichmann, Hammarström, Stadler and Christiansen2016). Sexual selection for body size offers one possible explanation for why sound symbolism might be so ubiquitously distributed.

The first clue was provided by the ‘motivation-structural rules’ theory (Morton Reference Morton1977), after observing that many animals modulate their vocalizations during competitive encounters: they use low-pitched vocalizations when their intention is to be threatening and dominant, and high-pitched vocalizations if they wish to appear conciliatory or submissive. The hypothesized reason is that the frequency of vocalizations reflects a projection of the individual's body size, a key determinant in the outcome of physical contests but also courtship interactions (Bradbury and Vehrencamp Reference Bradbury and Vehrencamp2011). This notion was then extended to humans in the ‘frequency-code’ theory (Ohala Reference Ohala1984), which provides a plausible explanation for the observed vocal dimorphism in human voices. Before puberty, boys and girls exhibit similar vocal frequencies, until males experience a significant enlargement of their larynx and vocal folds under the influence of androgens, which lowers their vocal pitch and resonant frequencies to the point that they practically do not overlap with those of adult females (Titze Reference Titze1989). Such findings hint towards the action of sexual selection and can be interpreted as a result of different selective pressures acting on each sex (Puts Reference Puts2010). In males, lower-frequency voices could have been favoured within intra-sexual contests because they are perceptually associated with largeness (Pisanski et al. Reference Pisanski, Fraccaro, Tigue, O'Connor and Feinberg2014; Xu et al. Reference Xu, Lee, Wu, Liu and Birkholz2013; Pisanski and Rendall Reference Pisanski and Rendall2011; Rendall et al. Reference Rendall, Vokey and Nemeth2007; van Dommelen and Moxness Reference van Dommelen and Moxness1995), more masculine and more socially and physically dominant men (Hodges-Simeon et al. Reference Hodges-Simeon, Gurven, Puts and Gaulin2014; Puts et al. Reference Puts, Gaulin and Verdolini2006, Reference Puts, Hodges, Cárdenas and Gaulin2007; Xu et al. Reference Xu, Lee, Wu, Liu and Birkholz2013; although see Armstrong et al. Reference Armstrong, Lee and Feinberg2019 for why voice pitch may not be an honest signal of dominance). In contrast, higher frequencies in female voices could have been selected in mate-choice dynamics as such frequencies were shown to be associated with perceived smallness, femininity and more attractive women (Xu et al. Reference Xu, Lee, Wu, Liu and Birkholz2013; Fraccaro et al. Reference Fraccaro, Jones, Vukovic, Smith, Watkins, Feinberg, Little and Debruine2011; Puts et al. Reference Puts, Barndt, Welling, Dawood and Burriss2011; Jones et al. Reference Jones, Feinberg, DeBruine, Little and Vukovic2010; Feinberg et al. Reference Feinberg, DeBruine, Jones and Perrett2008; Collins and Missing Reference Collins and Missing2003).

Although naming practices are assumed to be highly driven by sociocultural factors, few studies have underpinned the ultimate causes that have driven most male and female names to not overlap phonetically (Pitcher et al. Reference Pitcher, Mesoudi and McElligott2013). As observed for other dimorphic traits in humans such as the body size and stature (Geary Reference Geary1998; Puts Reference Puts2010), one can reasonably assume that these two different sexual selective pressures on human voices could have driven the attested sexual phonetic dimorphism. Preliminary evidence has shown that across languages as diverse as English, Japanese, Chinese, Korean and several Native American and Australian languages, high- and low-frequency vowels are respectively associated with perceived smallness and largeness (Haynie et al. Reference Haynie, Bowern and LaPalombara2014; Shinohara and Kawahara Reference Shinohara and Kawahara2010; Ultan Reference Ultan1978; Newman Reference Newman1933; Sapir Reference Sapir1929), as well as perceived femininity and masculinity (Wu et al. Reference Wu, Klink and Guo2013; Klink Reference Klink2000). Thus, indexical cues that are known to be relevant to human mating (e.g. body size, masculinity and femininity) may be conveyed or projected in first names through sound symbolism, using an array of different phonemes that can differ in their intrinsic fundamental frequency (i.e. the perceptual correlate of pitch), formant frequencies (i.e. resonances of the vocal tract) and their dispersion (i.e. a proxy of the vocal tract length) (Knoeferle et al. Reference Knoeferle, Li, Maggioni and Spence2017; Ohala Reference Ohala1994; Ultan Reference Ultan1978).

Although parents may not volitionally seek a large or small, dominant and attractive sounding name for their offspring, they might display an unconscious preference for either a more masculine or feminine name to suit their child's sex. This behaviour can be explained by the fact that gendered naming is an important tool of categorization in humans. Indeed, sex is one the most pervasive characteristic individuals first infer when interacting with others: distinguishing it by using different phonetic material for first names may find benefits in that it increases cognitive efficiency by allowing individuals to rapidly infer properties of sex category, even with little or no first-hand experience with that person. In turn, it enables individuals to tailor their expectations about the behaviours and capacities linked to the biological composition of that individual. Additionally, masculine and feminine names take on great importance in the reinforcement of an individual's sexual identity and gender role (Pilcher Reference Pilcher2016). Although first names are not inherited and no studies have yet tackled the issue of their influence on reproductive success, it has been reported that first names can impact their bearers on several aspects: their physical perception (Zwebner et al. Reference Zwebner, Sellier, Rosenfeld, Goldenberg and Mayo2017; Hartung Reference Hartung2018; Perfors Reference Perfors2004; Erwin Reference Erwin1993; Hassebrauck Reference Hassebrauck1988; Hensley and Spencer Reference Hensley and Spencer1985), inferences on personality (Mehrabian Reference Mehrabian2001; Mehrabian and Piercy Reference Mehrabian and Piercy1993; Leirer et al. Reference Leirer, Hamilton and Carpenter1982), attitudes and behaviours (Figlio Reference Figlio2007; Pelham et al. Reference Pelham, Mirenberg and Jones2002), social desirability (Gebauer et al. Reference Gebauer, Leary and Neberich2012; Busse and Seraydarian Reference Busse and Seraydarian1978) and social outcomes (Cotton et al. Reference Cotton, O'Neill and Griffin2008; Figlio Reference Figlio2005; Hodson and Olson Reference Hodson and Olson2005; Harari and McDavid Reference Harari and McDavid1973). Thus, it can be suggested that this cognitive bias could interfere during the naming process, since the phonetic peculiarities of forenames may underline and reinforce the perceptual associations of the biological and social characteristics linked to each sex through sound symbolism, which ultimately might be relatively important towards competitors and potential mates. Furthermore, to our knowledge, no societies (industrialized or not) currently use, or have been using, the same set of names for males and females. Lastly, it is worth noting that even though cultural evolution drives popularity and the emergence of novel names (e.g. Berger et al. Reference Berger, Bradlow, Braunstein and Zhang2012), it merely explains why individuals primarily perceive them as either male or female.

Sound symbolism has already been observed in the phonetic composition of English first names (Sidhu and Pexman Reference Sidhu and Pexman2015; Pitcher et al. Reference Pitcher, Mesoudi and McElligott2013; Cassidy et al. Reference Cassidy, Kelly and Sharoni1999; Cutler et al. Reference Cutler, McQueen and Robinson1990). So far, only one study has formally tested these evolutionary hypotheses through the lens of sexual selection using a database of the thousand most popular English, American and Australian first names between 2001 and 2010 (Pitcher et al. Reference Pitcher, Mesoudi and McElligott2013). In accordance with the evolutionary predictions, high-frequency vowels such as /i/ or /e/ were mostly attested in female names and low-frequency ones such as /u/ or /o/ in male names. Such differences were found on the first syllable, where stress is generally located and which is consequently perceptually prominent in English. However, the authors did not investigate consonant patterns nor take a look on the last syllable to ensure that no phonetic dimorphism was also present there.

The goal of the present study is to quantify the hypothesized phonetic dimorphism of male and female names, using a large sample size of popular first names in France that extends over the last century. In this context, this study extends the results that have already been observed in English first names. However, two major differences exist between French and English. First the lexical stress falls on the last syllable in French and most of the time on the first syllable in English. Secondly, all phonological units are not equally represented in French and English. For example, nasal vowels are attested in the former but absent in the latter. Moreover, analyses can be expanded by including consonants, for which patterns of sound symbolism have been previously reported (Nielsen and Rendall Reference Nielsen and Rendall2013; Maurer et al. Reference Maurer, Pathman and Mondloch2006). Consequently, we expect to find sex-bias sound symbolic patterns in the phonemes of the stressed syllable in French names, namely back and nasal vowels and voiced consonants in male names, as they are produced at lower frequencies, as opposed to front vowels as well as voiceless consonants in female names, since their articulation produces noise in relatively higher frequencies (Knoeferle et al. Reference Knoeferle, Li, Maggioni and Spence2017; Ohala Reference Ohala1994; Ultan Reference Ultan1978). Lastly, we will conduct exploratory analyses of the temporal variations of these sound symbolic patterns from 1900 to 2009 in order to examine whether they have remained constant or have evolved over time for each sex.

Material and methods

Data pre-treatment

Data was retrieved on September 2014 from the Institut National de la Statistique et des Études Économiques. We selected the most popular 100 female and 100 male names for each decade, ranging from 1900–1909 to 2000–2009. In order to control for population size, popularity was estimated by calculating the annual ranking position of each name and adding these up per decade. Although this approach excludes rare names, it captures naming practices properly for a given decade (Pitcher et al. Reference Pitcher, Mesoudi and McElligott2013).

All retrieved names were subsequently transcribed independently by two native French-speaking phoneticians, following the International Phonetic Alphabet principles. When no agreement arose for certain transcriptions or when pronunciation was unknown, different web sources were used (e.g. https://fr.wiktionary.org/wiki). For each syllable of a name, we recorded the following articulatory features:

  • The vowel place of articulation, which corresponds to the position of the tongue in the oral cavity during its articulation. As the tongue is closer to the lips, the sounds produced have an overall higher frequency spectrum (i.e. front vowels such as /i/). Conversely, sounds that are produced with the tongue retracted at the back of the mouth (i.e. back vowels such as /u/) have an overall lower spectral distribution. Central vowels (i.e. /a/) correspond to a position where the tongue is placed in the middle of the mouth. Acoustically, vocalic frontness and/or backness correspond to the frequencies of the second formant (i.e. the spectral peaks of the sound spectrum). The vowel height, which corresponds to the degree of aperture of the mandible (i.e. the open/close dimension, corresponding acoustically to the first formant), was not retained here, as it would produce redundant information with vowel articulation (i.e. multicollinearity in the statistical analyses).

  • The vowel's nasality, which is determined by the low position of the velum during articulation, leads the air to flow through the nose as well as the mouth. This extra resonance, which results from the intervention of the nasal cavity during phonation, lowers the frequency of the sound in comparison to its non-nasal counterpart. Note that only one type of vowel (oral or nasal) can be found in each syllable.

  • The consonant's manner of articulation, which is determined by the way the airflow escapes from the vocal tract during articulation. Here, we focused on plosives, which are produced by a complete closing of the airflow that causes its blocking before the air is suddenly released. This type of sound produces a burst noise that is typical of consonantal stops. We also focused on fricatives, which are produced with a major constriction of the airflow, which acoustically causes a turbulent noise. Owing to their manner of articulation, plosives generally produce lower frequencies than fricatives.

  • The consonant's voicing, which is determined by whether the vocal folds vibrate or not during articulation. This new source of laryngeal noise explains why voiced consonants are lower in frequencies than voiceless ones.

All phonemes coded with examples of first names are given in Table 1.

Table 1. Examples of first names for each phoneme investigated (underlined)

Statistical analyses

Analysis on sound symbolism

The aim of this analysis is to study and quantify sex differences in first names’ phonetic composition. According to our predictions, we expect to find in the stressed syllable of male names either back or nasal vowels and voiced consonants, as opposed to front, non-nasal vowels and unvoiced consonants in female names. In order to test these predictions, we aggregated all of the first names spanning over the century, giving only one list of first names (e.g. ‘Marie’ was found in several decades). Only one version of phonetically equivalent names in each sex was collated (e.g. ‘Danielle’ and ‘Daniele’, homophones non-homographs, i.e. names pronounced alike but not written alike). Compound names (e.g. ‘Jean-Marie’, ‘Marie-Pierre’) were discarded as they represent a particular set of names mostly composed of a masculine name joint to a feminine name. Monosyllabic names were also discarded from the analysis because it would preclude comparing the first and last syllable. This resulted in a sample size of 275 female and 197 male popular unique names distributed across the century. A generalized linear model was then used to investigate the existence of sex-biased sound symbolic patterns in French male vs. female names. Because the response variable ‘sex’ was binary, a binomial distribution with a logit link function was specified. The explanatory variables were the articulatory features aforementioned, each repeated for the first and the stressed last syllable:

  • The vowel's place of articulation – a fixed factor with three modalities (i.e. front, central or back vowel).

  • The vowel's nasality – a fixed factor with two modalities (i.e. nasal and non-nasal vowel).

  • Counts of voiced and unvoiced consonants (plosives and fricatives) – covariates that were standardized.

Finally, post-hoc comparisons (Tukey's range test) with a Bonferroni correction were performed for the vowel's place of articulation in order to assess comparisons between the sexes in each syllable. The general size effect was computed using Cohen's f 2. A symbolic representation of the regression formula is given in the Supplementary Material (Figure S1).

Temporal analyses

We assessed if the potential significant sound symbolic patterns found in the previous analysis have evolved or remained constant over time between male and female French first names. Pseudo-replication was allowed but phonetically equivalent, compound and monosyllabic names were still excluded, as the aim was to study temporal variations in both the first and last syllable. This resulted in a sample size of 897 female and 790 male names distributed across all decades. To address the time series nature of the data, we first calculated all autocorrelations and partial correlations between each time lag in order to assess if the frequency of a given phonetic variable is dependent on its previous frequency. Vowel articulation was counted as the number of each type of vowel in each syllable and was centred around 0; with 0 corresponding to central vowels, 1 to front vowels and −1 to back vowels. For vowel nasality, it was counted as the proportion of each vowel type: if values are close to 0, first names contain overall fewer nasal vowels, and conversely, if values are close to 1, they contain more nasal vowels. For voiced and voiceless consonants, the mean number in each syllable was studied. Linear models were then used to describe all of the temporal trends. To study possible non-linear effects of time, we modelled a cubic and quadratic effect of decade. Sex was included as another explanatory variable and was put in interaction with time. Model comparisons using the Akaike Information Criterion were then used to assess the best describing model of the temporal variations. All statistical analyses were performed using the R software (version 3.4.4).

Results

Sex-biased sound symbolism

We found a sex-biased sound structure in first names as a function of the syllable under study (Table 2). In the last stressed syllable, significant clues of masculinity were given by the vowel place of articulation ($\chi _2^2$ = 11.82, p < 0.01), nasality ($\chi _1^2$ = 65.41, p < 0.001) and voiceless fricatives ($\chi _1^2$ = 13.23, p < 0.001). Namely, male names were significantly more prone to contain back vowels like /o/ or /ɔ/ (e.g. ‘Enzo’, ‘Renaud’), instead of front or central ones such as /i/, /y/ or /a/ (respectively t = 1.17, p < 0.01; t = 1.35, p < 0.01; e.g. ‘Jackie’, ‘Luc’, ‘Bernard’). Although back vowels can be found in female names (e.g. ‘Simone’, ‘Laure’), front and central vowels are more common (e.g. ‘Emilie’, ‘Julie’, ‘Léa’, ‘Maria’) along with mid-front vowels such as /ε/ (e.g. ‘Claire’, ‘Hélène’). Male names were also significantly more likely to contain nasal vowels such as /ã/ or /ɔ̃/ (e.g. ‘Roland’, ‘Raymond’; female counter-examples, ‘Fernande’, ‘Marion’) and voiceless fricatives such as /s/ or /ʃ/ (e.g. ‘Fabrice’, ‘Michel’; female counter-examples, ‘Clemence’, ‘Blanche’). Probabilities of being a male name as a function of the type of vowel (oral and nasal) are given in Figure 1.

Table 2. Results of the generalized linear model. For each predictor, the estimate, standard error of the mean, the χ 2, the degrees of freedom and the p-values associated from the likelihood ratio test of the comparison between the full model and the model without the predictor are given. For the categorical variables ‘Vowel place of articulation’ and ‘Nasality’, the estimates are given compared to the reference category (front and non-nasal vowels, respectively) for both syllables. Pseudo-R 2 is the variance explained by the model (adjusted by the number of predictors) and Cohen's f2 the overall size effect. Significant p-values are in bold

Figure 1. Estimates of the generalized linear model, log back-transformed to provide the probabilities of a name belonging to a male in function of the presence of a particular (a) oral vowel and (b) nasal vowel. Bars represent the mean probability associated with 95% confidence intervals. Significance code from the post-hoc comparisons: *** p < 0.001; ** p < 0.01; NS, non-significant.

Unexpectedly, in the first syllable, the probability of being a male name significantly increased as a function of the number of voiced plosives ($\chi _1^2$ = 12.59, p < 0.001) such as /b/, /d/ or /g/ (e.g. ‘Bernard’, ‘Dimitri’, ‘Gustave’; female counter-examples, ‘Brigitte’, ‘Deborah’, ‘Gwenaëlle’). Within the first syllable, vowel articulation and nasality did not differ between sexes, nor did the number of voiceless fricatives (all p > 0.05). Eventually, articulatory features explained 14% of the variation in sex differences and the Cohen's f 2 (0.17) suggests a moderate size effect (Cohen Reference Cohen2013).

Temporal analyses from 1900 to 2009

Trends investigated were the vowel's place of articulation, vowel's nasality, the number of voiced plosives and voiceless fricatives in both the first and last syllable. All trends are shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Barplots (mean ± standard-error) of the temporal variations for each decade from 1900 to 2009 of each articulatory feature that revealed significant in the sound symbolic patterns analysis. Female first names are represented in light blue and male first names in dark blue. The vowel's place of articulation is represented in (a) last syllable and (b) first syllable. Vowel's nasality in the (c) last syllable and (d) first syllable. Mean number of voiced plosives are represented in the (e) last syllable and (f) first syllable. Lastly, mean number of voiceless fricatives are represented in the (g) last syllable and (h) first syllable. Vowel articulation accounts for the number of each type of vowel in each syllable and were centred around 0; with 0 more central vowels, 1 more front vowels and −1 more back vowels. For vowel nasality, it accounts for the number of each vowel type: if values are close to 0, first names contain fewer nasal vowels, and conversely, if values are close to 1, they contain more nasal vowels.

Analyses of the autocorrelations and partial correlations revealed that the frequency of each articulatory feature at a given timepoint is mostly independent of its previous frequency (most p > 0.05, all autocorrelations and partial correlations are given in the Supplementary Material, Table S1).

The proportion of oral vowels across time in the last syllable of both male and female names showed a cubic change (F 1,1686 = 14.01, p < 0.01, Figure 2a) and the overall difference in proportion between the sexes was significant (F 1,1686 = 33.41, p < 0.001). Interestingly, female names tended to be ‘masculinized’ (i.e. contained more central and back vowels, especially the former) over time starting from the 1960s with convergent values between male and female names towards 2009. In the first syllable, no overall difference in proportion was observed between the sexes (F 1,1686 = 1.62, p = 0.22), but both followed a quadratic temporal change (F 1,1686 = 38.71, p < 0.001, Figure 2b). In the last syllable, the difference in proportion of names with nasal vowels was different between male and female names (F 1,1686 = 117.25, p < 0.001) and both remained more or less constant over time (F 1,1686 = 1.46, p = 0.24, Figure 2c). In the first syllable, a slight difference in proportion was observed (F 1,1686 = 6.34, p < 0.05), and both sexes followed a quadratic change over time (F 1,1686 = 51.59, p < 0.001, Figure 2d).

In the last syllable, no sex difference and no temporal change in the mean number of voiced plosives were observed (respectively F 1,1686 = 1.11, p = 0.30; F 1,1686 = 4.24, p = 0.054, Figure 2e). In the first syllable, overall difference in voiced plosives between the sexes was significant (F 1,789 = 87.81, p < 0.001), but no change was observed over time (Figure 2f), although the interaction between sex and a quadratic effect of time was significant (F 1,1686 = 8.48, p < 0.01). Overall differences in the mean number of voiceless fricatives between the sexes was found in the last syllable (F 1,1686 = 60.09, p < 0.001). In both sexes, the mean number of voiceless fricatives followed a cubic evolution through time (F 1,1686 = 12.46, p = 0.023, Figure 2g), and an interaction between sex and time revealed to be significant (F 1,1686 = 30.66, p < 0.001). Lastly, the mean number of voiceless fricatives in the first syllable for both sexes linearly varied over time (F 1,1686 = 31.50, p < 0.001, Figure 2h) and an overall difference between the sexes was observed (respectively F 1,1686 = 103.32, p < 0.001). The interaction between sex and time was also significant (F 1,1686 = 55.59, p < 0.001).

Discussion

French first names exhibited sex differences in the distribution of vocalic sounds: low-frequency vowels (i.e. back and nasal) were more likely to be found in masculine names while higher frequency vowels (i.e. front and non-nasal) as well as central vowels (i.e. /a/) were more frequent in female names.

This sex-biased sound symbolism pattern was found in the last syllable, which is perceptually prominent in French, while in English, a similar sex-biased symbolism was reported for the first stressed syllable (Pitcher et al. Reference Pitcher, Mesoudi and McElligott2013). However, regarding consonants, our results were more unexpected. Indeed, the mean number of voiceless fricatives (i.e. /f/, /s/ and /ʃ/; e.g. ‘Joseph’, ‘Alexis’, ‘Michel’) was higher in male than female names within the final stressed syllable (e.g. female names: ‘Delphine’, ‘Clarisse’). This is surprising according to the ‘frequency-code’ theory since their higher domain of frequency, relatively to voiced consonants, would rather be associated with indexical cues of smallness. The second unexpected finding was the presence of voiced plosives in the first syllable (i.e. /b/, /d/ and /g/; e.g. ‘Bernard’, ‘David’, ‘Gabriel’; e.g. female names, ‘Brigitte’, ‘Geraldine’), which is theoretically perceptually non-prominent in French. A possible explanation is that these consonantal patterns may perceptually compensate for each other, by which the presence in masculine names of voiceless fricatives in the last stressed syllable is perceptually counterbalanced by the presence of voiced consonants in the unstressed one. Otherwise, in a more general manner, vowels and consonants in the first and last syllable may be perceptually associated with different physical qualities. In this sense, while oral and nasal vowels could refer to body size, consonants might evoke other qualities such as shape or speed (Berlin Reference Berlin2006). For instance, it has been shown that people perceive a form as rounder if its signifier contains voiced consonants (such as /b/, /m/, /l/ or /n/) and as sharper if it contains voiceless stops (such as /k/, /p/, /t/) (Sidhu and Pexman Reference Sidhu and Pexman2015; Nielsen and Rendall Reference Nielsen and Rendall2013; Maurer et al. Reference Maurer, Pathman and Mondloch2006). In the case of voiced plosives in the first syllable of male names, another explanation can be invoked as it is in accordance with results observed in American and Indian forenames (Slepian and Galinsky Reference Slepian and Galinsky2016). The authors showed a voiced gender naming effect, whereby the initial phonemes of masculine first names were voiced, as opposed to unvoiced in feminine names. They argued that voiced phonemes would sound ‘harder’ as a consequence of the vocal folds vibrating during pronunciation, whereas unvoiced phonemes will sound ‘softer’ to the ear as a consequence of unmodulated airflow, which in both cases would perceptually reinforce the stereotyped representations of males and females having respectively ‘tougher’ vs. ‘tender’ personalities and behaviours. Interestingly, the endorsement of these traditional gender stereotypes related to these ‘tougher/harder’ vs. ‘softer/tender’ dimensions moderated the influence of voiced and unvoiced phonemes on masculine vs. feminine judgments.

The name selected by parents for their offspring is, most of the time, linked to the assigned sex at birth, probably because such an information takes on great importance in both the perception of the bearer's sex properties by conspecifics in the social environment, and in the bearer's reinforcement of sexual identity and gender role (Pilcher Reference Pilcher2016). In human societies, males and females have distinct roles and different reproductive strategies (Schmitt Reference Schmitt2015). Owing to the associated sex-sound symbolism, giving a masculine or feminine name to conform to sex stereotypes could thus be seen as a form of parental investment with a lifelong-lasting effect. Although these effects have not been measured yet in reproductive value, it remains to be shown whether or not they influence fitness-related traits. However, the fact that most first names are sex-specific suggests that they are not fully socially neutral, and many studies have disclosed the influence of given names on some social trait, such as social desirability (Gebauer et al. Reference Gebauer, Leary and Neberich2012; Busse and Seraydarian Reference Busse and Seraydarian1978) and social outcomes (Cotton et al. Reference Cotton, O'Neill and Griffin2008; Figlio Reference Figlio2005; Hodson and Olson Reference Hodson and Olson2005; Harari and McDavid Reference Harari and McDavid1973). For instance, several studies have shown that having only the information of a masculine or feminine name already influences the bearer's job and career outcomes (Kasof Reference Kasof1993; Moss-Racusin et al. Reference Moss-Racusin, Dovidio, Brescoll, Graham and Handelsman2012; Steinpreis et al., Reference Steinpreis, Anders and Ritzke1999).

Yet while our results support the idea that humans possess a cognitive bias to assign different phonetic material to either sex, the relatively small amount of variance explained in sex differences (~14%) and the relatively modest size effect (Cohen's f 2 = 0.17) suggest that other factors than sexually sound symbolic patterns need to be considered when parents choose a particular name for their child. Evidence shows that the cultural environment is undeniably one of them (Acerbi and Bentley Reference Acerbi and Alexander Bentley2014; Barucca et al. Reference Barucca, Rocchi, Marinari, Parisi and Ricci-Tersenghi2015; Bentley et al. Reference Bentley, Hahn and Shennan2004; Berger et al. Reference Berger, Bradlow, Braunstein and Zhang2012; Xi et al. Reference Xi, Zhang, Zhang, Ge, She and Zhang2014). For instance, Bentley et al. (Reference Bentley, Hahn and Shennan2004) have shown that name distributions and changes over time followed power laws, which were predicted by a simple mechanism of cultural drift and random copying between individuals, assuming that names are value-neutral in regards to fitness. Other models have been used to describe their distributions across time and space, the rate of innovation and their diversity, such as activation–inhibition processes (Zanette Reference Zanette2012), individual preferences and social influence (Xi et al. Reference Xi, Zhang, Zhang, Ge, She and Zhang2014), and spatial–temporal homogeneity (Bentley and Ormerod Reference Bentley and Ormerod2012). Most interestingly, Berger et al. (Reference Berger, Bradlow, Braunstein and Zhang2012) have shown that names are more likely to be chosen when similar-sounding names in terms of phonetic similarity (i.e. sharing phonemes and their position within the name) have been popular the previous year, regardless of the names’ gender. For instance, their model predicted that the popularity of the name ‘Karen’ depended on popular names that possessed the same first phoneme (i.e. /k/), such as ‘Carl’ (a male name) and ‘Katie’ (a female name). Predicted popularity was also correlated with other cultural items such as hurricanes’ names (i.e. ‘Katrina’), suggesting a strong effect of other cultural items on naming processes.

In this context, the temporal variations of the articulatory features suggest a strong effect of culture, given the somewhat stochastic variations of some phonetic variants, such as the frequency of occurrence of voiced plosives and voiceless fricatives. Nonetheless, we feel that particular attention should be given to the vowel's place of articulation. Its evolution in the stressed syllable of female first names suggests that high-frequency sounds were considered as most feminine in the 1960s, a period after which we notice an increase in phonetic masculinization that continues up to 2009. For instance, names with front vowels (e.g. ‘Marie’) in the early 1900s were more frequent than those with central and back vowels (e.g. ‘Léa’, ‘Manon’), which increased in frequency in the 1960s up to the 2000s. Interestingly, an earlier study dealing with the evolution of feminization across the last century has shown that the ‘ideal’ waist-to-hip ratio (WHR), an important component of men's mate preferences, seemed to have followed the same trend in Western society. This ‘ideal’ WHR, as assessed through Playboy models and Miss pageants from 1920 to 2014, is most feminine in the 1960s (lower WHR values) then becomes less and less feminine until the 2010s (higher WHR values) (Bovet and Raymond Reference Bovet and Raymond2015). Additionally, a meta-analysis on the self-perception of femininity and masculinity, as assessed through the Bem Sex–Role Inventory and the Personal Attributes Questionnaire, showed that American women perceived themselves as more masculine over time from the early 1970s to the mid 1990s (Twenge Reference Twenge1997), with additional findings demonstrating a decrease in endorsing feminine traits in women after the 2000s (Donnelly and Twenge Reference Donnelly and Twenge2017). Two other meta-analyses investigating women's own assertiveness from 1931 to 1993 showed that it decreased from 1946 to 1967, but increased from 1968 to 1993 (Twenge Reference Twenge2001). Such changes from the 1960s might be closely linked to historical political feminists’ movements particularly active in this era during which awareness of inequalities in civil rights and social positions has been increasing. We hypothesize that one possible strategy to compensate for such inequalities is to masculinize some traits in women in order to compete against men for the same rights and privileges, at least in industrialized and traditionally male-dominated societies.

Conclusions

Overall, the present study offers some promising opportunities for follow-up studies that would lead to a better understanding of naming processes. An interesting avenue for further research would be to model the relative importance of different selective pressures (sexual and cultural, or a joint effect) acting on the phonetic dimorphism, names’ frequency and the emergence of novel names. Most importantly, to fully acknowledge the action of sexual selection on the phonetic dimorphism, a study on names and their relationship to reproductive value is required. One limitation is that not all names from each decade were analysed and a particular attention should be given to rare names in order to strengthen the present results. Moreover, particular attention should also be given to syllables between the first and last ones, as they can potentially play a particular role. Further inquiries in sound symbolic patterns in first names in dead and modern languages should be performed, so as to find some universal components in vowel quality to convey perceived masculinity and femininity.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.7

Author ORCIDs

Alexandre Suire, 0000-0002-1852-3083

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Institut National de la Statistique et des Études Économiques for providing the data on French first names (‘Fichier des prénoms’ – Edition 2011 (electronic data), INSEE (producer) and Centre Maurice Halbwachs (distributor)). This is a contribution of the Institut des Sciences de l’Évolution 2019-101.

Author contributions

A.S. and A.B.M. wrote the paper. A.S. and M.B.D. transcribed the data. A.S., A.B.M and M.R. analysed the data. M.B.D. and M.R. supervised the study.

Financial Support

This study received a ‘Equipe Recherche Junior’ funding from the Centre Méditerranéen de l'Environnement et de la Biodiversité under the project name ‘Symphosex’.

Conflict of Interest

None.

Data access

The data and the R code are available at https://figshare.com/s/5618e367fae4c5774272

Footnotes

A.S. and A.B.M. are both first authors.

References

Acerbi, A and Alexander Bentley, R (2014) Biases in cultural transmission shape the turnover of popular traits. Evolution and Human Behavior 35(3), 228236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.02.003Google Scholar
Armstrong, MM, Lee, AJ and Feinberg, DR (2019) A house of cards: bias in perception of body size mediates the relationship between voice pitch and perceptions of dominance. Animal Behaviour 147, 4351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.11.005Google Scholar
Barucca, P, Rocchi, J, Marinari, E, Parisi, G and Ricci-Tersenghi, F (2015) Cross-correlations of American baby names. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112(26), 79437947. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1507143112Google Scholar
Bentley, RA and Ormerod, P (2012) Accelerated innovation and increased spatial diversity of US popular culture. Advances in Complex Systems 15(01n02), 1150011. https://doi.org/10.1142/S0219525911003232Google Scholar
Bentley, RA, Hahn, MW and Shennan, SJ (2004) Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 271(1547), 14431450. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2004.2746Google Scholar
Berger, J, Bradlow, ET, Braunstein, A and Zhang, Y (2012) From Karen to Katie: using baby names to understand cultural evolution. Psychological Science 23(10), 10671073. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612443371Google Scholar
Berlin, B (2006) The first congress of ethnozoological nomenclature. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 12(s1), S23S44. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9655.2006.00271.xGoogle Scholar
Blasi, DE, Wichmann, S, Hammarström, H, Stadler, PF and Christiansen, MH (2016) Sound–meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113(39), 1081810823. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1605782113Google Scholar
Bovet, J and Raymond, M (2015) Preferred women's waist-to-hip ratio variation over the last 2,500 years. PLoS One 10(4), e0123284. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0123284Google Scholar
Bradbury, JW and Vehrencamp, SL (2011) Chapter 4: Conflict resolution and Chapter 12: Mate attraction and courtship. In Principles of Animal Communication (2nd edn) Sunderland: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Bremner, AJ, Caparos, S, Davidoff, J, de Fockert, J, Linnell, KJ and Spence, C (2013) ‘Bouba’ and ‘kiki’ in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape–sound matches, but different shape–taste matches to Westerners. Cognition 126(2), 165172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.007Google Scholar
Busse, TV and Seraydarian, L (1978) The relationships between first name desirability and school readiness, IQ, and school achievement. Psychology in the Schools 15(2), 297302. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(197804)15:2<297::AID-PITS2310150229>3.0.CO;2-93.0.CO;2-9>Google Scholar
Cassidy, KW, Kelly, MH and Sharoni, LAJ (1999) Inferring gender from name phonology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128(3), 362.Google Scholar
Cohen, J (2013) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hoboken, NJ: Taylor and Francis.Google Scholar
Collins, SA and Missing, C (2003) Vocal and visual attractiveness are related in women. Animal Behaviour 65(5), 9971004. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2123Google Scholar
Cotton, JL, O'Neill, BS and Griffin, A (2008) The ‘name game’: affective and hiring reactions to first names. Journal of Managerial Psychology 23(1), 1839. https://doi.org/10.1108/02683940810849648Google Scholar
Cuskley, C, Simner, J and Kirby, S (2017) Phonological and orthographic influences in the bouba–kiki effect. Psychological Research 81(1), 119130. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0709-2Google Scholar
Cutler, A, McQueen, J and Robinson, K (1990) Elizabeth and John: sound patterns of men's and women's names. Journal of Linguistics 26(02), 471. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700014754Google Scholar
Donnelly, K and Twenge, JM (2017) Masculine and feminine traits on the Bem Sex–Role Inventory, 1993–2012: a cross-temporal meta-analysis. Sex Roles 76(9–10), 556565. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0625-yGoogle Scholar
Erwin, PG (1993) First names and perceptions of physical attractiveness. The Journal of Psychology 127(6), 625631. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1993.9914901Google Scholar
Feinberg, DR, DeBruine, LM, Jones, BC and Perrett, DI (2008) The role of femininity and averageness of voice pitch in aesthetic judgments of women's voices. Perception 37(4), 615623. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5514Google Scholar
Figlio, D (2005) Names, Expectations and the Black–White Test Score Gap (no. w11195). Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research, Available at: https://www.nber.org/papers/w11195.Google Scholar
Figlio, DN (2007) Boys named Sue: Disruptive children and their peers. Education Finance and Policy 2(4), 376394.Google Scholar
Fraccaro, PJ, Jones, BC, Vukovic, J, Smith, FG, Watkins, CD, Feinberg, DR, Little, AC and Debruine, LM (2011) Experimental evidence that women speak in a higher voice pitch to men they find attractive. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology 9(1), 5767. https://doi.org/10.1556/JEP.9.2011.33.1.Google Scholar
Geary, DC (1998) Male, Female: The Evolution of Human Sex Differences. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Gebauer, JE, Leary, MR and Neberich, W (2012) Unfortunate first names: Effects of name-based relational devaluation and interpersonal neglect. Social Psychological and Personality Science 3(5), 590596. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611431644Google Scholar
Harari, H and McDavid, JW (1973) Name stereotypes and teachers’ expectations. Journal of Educational Psychology 65(2), 222225. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034978Google Scholar
Hartung, F (2018) Are Tims hot and Toms not? https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9ctq5Google Scholar
Hassebrauck, M (1988) Beauty is more than ‘name’ deep: the effect of women's first names on ratings of physical attractiveness and personality attributes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 18(9), 721726. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1988.tb02350.xGoogle Scholar
Haynie, H, Bowern, C and LaPalombara, H (2014) Sound symbolism in the languages of Australia. PLoS One 9(4), e92852. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092852Google Scholar
Hensley, WE and Spencer, BA (1985) The effect of first names on perceptions of female attractiveness. Sex Roles 12(7–8), 723729. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287866Google Scholar
Hodges-Simeon, CR, Gurven, M, Puts, DA and Gaulin, SJC (2014) Vocal fundamental and formant frequencies are honest signals of threat potential in peripubertal males. Behavioral Ecology 25(4), 984988. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru081Google Scholar
Hodson, G and Olson, JM (2005) Testing the generality of the name letter effect: name initials and everyday attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31(8), 10991111. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205274895Google Scholar
Jones, BC, Feinberg, DR, DeBruine, LM, Little, AC and Vukovic, J (2010) A domain-specific opposite-sex bias in human preferences for manipulated voice pitch. Animal Behaviour 79(1), 5762. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.10.003Google Scholar
Kasof, J (1993) Sex bias in the naming of stimulus persons. Psychological Bulletin 113(1), 140163. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.113.1.140Google Scholar
Klink, RR (2000) Creating brand names with meaning: the use of sound symbolism. Marketing Letters 11(1), 520.Google Scholar
Knoeferle, K, Li, J, Maggioni, E and Spence, C (2017) What drives sound symbolism? Different acoustic cues underlie sound-size and sound-shape mappings. Scientific Reports 7(1) https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-05965-yGoogle Scholar
Köhler, W (1947) Gestalt Psychology (2nd edn) New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
Leirer, VO, Hamilton, DL and Carpenter, S (1982) Common first names as cues for inferences about personality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 8(4), 712718.Google Scholar
Maurer, D, Pathman, T and Mondloch, CJ (2006) The shape of boubas: sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Developmental Science 9(3), 316322. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00495.xGoogle Scholar
Mehrabian, A (2001) Characteristics attributed to individuals on the basis of their first names. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs 127(1), 59.Google Scholar
Mehrabian, A and Piercy, M (1993) Affective and personality characteristics inferred from length of first names. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 19(6), 755758.Google Scholar
Morton, ES (1977) On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. The America Naturalist 111(981), 855869.Google Scholar
Moss-Racusin, CA, Dovidio, JF, Brescoll, VL, Graham, MJ and Handelsman, J (2012) Science faculty's subtle gender biases favor male students. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109(41), 1647416479. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1211286109Google Scholar
Newman, SS (1933) Further experiments in phonetic symbolism. The American Journal of Psychology 45(1), 53. https://doi.org/10.2307/1414186Google Scholar
Nielsen, AKS and Rendall, D (2013) Parsing the role of consonants versus vowels in the classic Takete–Maluma phenomenon. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale 67(2), 153163. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030553Google Scholar
Ohala, JJ (1984) An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of F0 of voice. Phonetica 41(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1159/000261706Google Scholar
Ohala, J (1994) The frequency codes underlies the sound symbolic use of voice pitch. In Sound Symbolism (pp. 325347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pelham, BW, Mirenberg, MC and Jones, JT (2002) Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: implicit egotism and major life decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82(4), 469487. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.4.469Google Scholar
Perfors, A (2004) What's in a Name? The effect of sound symbolism on perception of facial attractiveness. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, vol. 26.Google Scholar
Pilcher, J (2016) Names, bodies and identities. Sociology 50(4), 764779. https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038515582157Google Scholar
Pisanski, K and Rendall, D (2011) The prioritization of voice fundamental frequency or formants in listeners’ assessments of speaker size, masculinity, and attractiveness. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 129(4), 22012212. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3552866Google Scholar
Pisanski, K, Fraccaro, PJ, Tigue, CC, O'Connor, JJM and Feinberg, DR (2014) Return to Oz: voice pitch facilitates assessments of men's body size. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 40(4), 13161331. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036956Google Scholar
Pitcher, BJ, Mesoudi, A and McElligott, AG (2013) Sex-biased sound symbolism in English-language first names. PLoS One 8( 6), e64825. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0064825Google Scholar
Puts, DA (2010) Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior 31(3), 157175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.005Google Scholar
Puts, DA, Gaulin, SJC and Verdolini, K (2006) Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior 27(4), 283296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.11.003Google Scholar
Puts, DA, Hodges, CR, Cárdenas, RA and Gaulin, SJC (2007) Men's voices as dominance signals: vocal fundamental and formant frequencies influence dominance attributions among men. Evolution and Human Behavior 28(5), 340344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.05.002Google Scholar
Puts, DA, Barndt, JL, Welling, LLM, Dawood, K and Burriss, RP (2011) Intrasexual competition among women: vocal femininity affects perceptions of attractiveness and flirtatiousness. Personality and Individual Differences 50(1), 111115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.011Google Scholar
Ramachandran, VS and Hubbard, EM (2001) Synaesthesia–A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of consciousness studies 8(12), 334.Google Scholar
Rendall, D, Vokey, JR and Nemeth, C (2007) Lifting the curtain on the Wizard of Oz: biased voice-based impressions of speaker size. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 33(5), 12081219. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1208Google Scholar
Sapir, E (1929) A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology 12(3), 225239. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0070931Google Scholar
Schmitt, DP (2015) Fundamentals of human mating strategies. In The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 123.Google Scholar
Shinohara, K and Kawahara, S (2010) A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: the images of size. Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 36(1), 396. https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v36i1.3926Google Scholar
Sidhu, DM and Pexman, PM (2015) What's in a name? Sound symbolism and gender in first names. PLoS One 10(5), e0126809. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126809Google Scholar
Slepian, ML and Galinsky, AD (2016) The voiced pronunciation of initial phonemes predicts the gender of names. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 110(4), 509527. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000041Google Scholar
Steinpreis, RE, Anders, KA and Ritzke, D (1999) The impact of gender on the review of the curricula vitae of job applicants and tenure candidates: a national empirical study. Sex Roles 41(7–8), 509528.Google Scholar
Svantesson, J.-O (2017) Sound symbolism: the role of word sound in meaning: sound symbolism. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 8(5), e1441. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1441Google Scholar
Titze, IR (1989) Physiologic and acoustic differences between male and female voices. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 85(4), 16991707. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.397959Google Scholar
Twenge, JM (1997) Changes in masculine and feminine traits over time: a meta-analysis. Sex Roles 36(5–6), 305325. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02766650Google Scholar
Twenge, JM (2001) Changes in women's assertiveness in response to status and roles: a cross-temporal meta-analysis, 1931–1993. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81(1), 133145. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.1.133Google Scholar
Ultan, R (1978) Size-sound symbolism. In Universals of Human Language (Vol. 2, p. 525568) Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
van Dommelen, WA and Moxness, BH (1995) Acoustic parameters in speaker height and weight identification: sex-specific behaviour. Language and Speech 38(3), 267287. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383099503800304Google Scholar
Werner, H (1957) Comparative Psychology of Mental Development (revised edn). New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Wu, L, Klink, RR and Guo, J (2013) Creating gender brand personality with brand names: the effects of phonetic symbolism. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice 21(3), 319330. https://doi.org/10.2753/MTP1069-6679210306Google Scholar
Xi, N, Zhang, Z.-K, Zhang, Y.-C, Ge, Z, She, L and Zhang, K (2014) Cultural evolution: the case of babies’ first names. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications 406, 139144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2014.03.042Google Scholar
Xu, Y, Lee, A, Wu, W.-L, Liu, X and Birkholz, P (2013) Human vocal attractiveness as signaled by body size projection. PLoS One 8(4), e62397. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062397Google Scholar
Zanette, DH (2012) Dynamics of fashion: the case of given names. ArXiv Preprint ArXiv:1208.0576, 7.Google Scholar
Zwebner, Y, Sellier, A.-L, Rosenfeld, N, Goldenberg, J and Mayo, R (2017) We look like our names: the manifestation of name stereotypes in facial appearance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 112(4), 527554. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000076Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Examples of first names for each phoneme investigated (underlined)

Figure 1

Table 2. Results of the generalized linear model. For each predictor, the estimate, standard error of the mean, the χ2, the degrees of freedom and the p-values associated from the likelihood ratio test of the comparison between the full model and the model without the predictor are given. For the categorical variables ‘Vowel place of articulation’ and ‘Nasality’, the estimates are given compared to the reference category (front and non-nasal vowels, respectively) for both syllables. Pseudo-R2 is the variance explained by the model (adjusted by the number of predictors) and Cohen's f2 the overall size effect. Significant p-values are in bold

Figure 2

Figure 1. Estimates of the generalized linear model, log back-transformed to provide the probabilities of a name belonging to a male in function of the presence of a particular (a) oral vowel and (b) nasal vowel. Bars represent the mean probability associated with 95% confidence intervals. Significance code from the post-hoc comparisons: *** p < 0.001; ** p < 0.01; NS, non-significant.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Barplots (mean ± standard-error) of the temporal variations for each decade from 1900 to 2009 of each articulatory feature that revealed significant in the sound symbolic patterns analysis. Female first names are represented in light blue and male first names in dark blue. The vowel's place of articulation is represented in (a) last syllable and (b) first syllable. Vowel's nasality in the (c) last syllable and (d) first syllable. Mean number of voiced plosives are represented in the (e) last syllable and (f) first syllable. Lastly, mean number of voiceless fricatives are represented in the (g) last syllable and (h) first syllable. Vowel articulation accounts for the number of each type of vowel in each syllable and were centred around 0; with 0 more central vowels, 1 more front vowels and −1 more back vowels. For vowel nasality, it accounts for the number of each vowel type: if values are close to 0, first names contain fewer nasal vowels, and conversely, if values are close to 1, they contain more nasal vowels.

Supplementary material: File

Suire et al. supplementary material

Suire et al. supplementary material 1

Download Suire et al. supplementary material(File)
File 20.8 KB