Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T10:10:29.019Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does stress induce salt intake?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 April 2010

Susan J. Torres*
Affiliation:
School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood 3125, Vic, Australia
Anne I. Turner
Affiliation:
School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood 3125, Vic, Australia
Caryl A. Nowson
Affiliation:
School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood 3125, Vic, Australia
*
*Corresponding author: Susan J. Torres, fax +61 3 9244 6017, email susan.torres@deakin.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Psychological stress is a common feature of modern day societies, and contributes to the global burden of disease. It was proposed by Henry over 20 years ago that the salt intake of a society reflects the level of stress, and that stress, through its effect on increasing salt intake, is an important factor in the development of hypertension. This review evaluates the evidence from animal and human studies to determine if stress does induce a salt appetite and increase salt consumption in human subjects. Findings from animal studies suggest that stress may drive salt intake, with evidence for a potential mechanism via the sympatho-adrenal medullary system and/or the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis. In contrast, in the few laboratory studies conducted in human subjects, none has found that acute stress affects salt intake. However, one study demonstrated that life stress (chronic stress) was associated with increased consumption of snack foods, which included, but not specifically, highly salty snacks. Studies investigating the influence of chronic stress on eating behaviours are required, including consumption of salty foods. From the available evidence, we can conclude that in free-living, Na-replete individuals, consuming Na in excess of physiological requirements, stress is unlikely to be a major contributor to salt intake.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2010

Henry(Reference Henry1) suggested over 20 years ago that the ‘salt (NaCl) consumption of a society is a measure of the social stress to which it is exposed’, and that stress, through its effect on increasing salt intake, is an important factor in the development of hypertension. Henry's hypothesis was initially based on ecological studies in human subjects, which suggested that salt intake may be driven by exposure to stress(Reference Henry1, Reference Denton2). These ecological studies relied on observational analyses, rather than on direct measures, and included a comparison of stress exposure in two different Polynesian communities: one which reflected a modern day, Westernised society that was likely to have higher stress exposure, had higher salt intakes and exhibited a blood pressure (BP) rise with age; and the other was the traditional community, with potentially low exposure to stress, and was associated with low salt intakes and minimal increases of BP with age(Reference Henry1Reference Prior, Evans and Harvey3).

Hypertension is a well-established risk factor for CVD(Reference Bennett and Magnus4, 5), and is a contributing factor to the development of cerebrovascular disease, IHD, and cardiac and renal failure(Reference Whitworth6). There is overwhelming evidence that salt intake is linked to the development of hypertension(Reference Elliott, Stamler and Nichols7Reference Jurgens and Graudal11). It also appears that stress plays a role in increasing risk of CVD(Reference Bunker, Colquhoun and Esler12). This raises the question – could stress actually induce a salt appetite and increase salt consumption in human subjects?

In this review, we will address the question – does stress increase salt intake in human subjects? Firstly, we describe salt appetite, salt preferences and the physiological responses to stress. Secondly, we assess the effect of stress on salt intake, reviewing evidence from animal models and human studies, which has been the recent focus of the present research. Thirdly, we discuss the physiological mechanisms that may link stress to salt intake using data from animal studies.

Salt appetite and salt preference

Salt appetite is the strong motivation for ingesting salt in situations of salt wasting(Reference Stricker, Kare, Fregly and Bernard13), whereas salt preference is a liking for salt in a Na-replete state(Reference Mattes14); both salt appetite and salt preference can be induced or innate, the latter may be determined by epigenetic influences on the foetus's genotype(Reference Nicolaidis15). Herbivores characteristically exhibit a salt appetite, which is likely to be due to a low Na diet, but omnivores, such as human subjects, do not typically exhibit this behaviour due to a high salt diet(Reference Denton2). Of interest is the finding that taste cells expressing the epithelial Na channel, which mediates behavioural attraction to salt, have been identified in mice, and may be present in human subjects(Reference Chandrashekar, Kuhn and Oka16). Salt appetite in human subjects is rare, but it has been documented in Addison's disease, a condition caused by deficiency of adrenal cortex hormones(Reference Kumar and Clark17), although it only appears to occur in 15 % of the patients with this disease(Reference Thorn, Dorrance and Day18). Gitelman's syndrome is a genetic disorder that affects the renal system, and is characterised by hypomagnesaemia, metabolic alkalosis, hypocalciuria and renal losses of Na(Reference Knoers and Levtchenko19). Cruz et al. (Reference Cruz, Shaer and Bia20) evaluated symptoms in fifty patients with Gitelman's syndrome, and found that 64 % of the patients reported salt cravings, which included drinking pickle brine and consuming salted cucumbers. In a clinical study which induced Na depletion, subjects reported a greater appetite for salty foods, although the authors duly acknowledge that this degree of Na depletion is unlikely to occur in free-living individuals(Reference Beauchamp, Bertino and Burke21). Overall, Na appetite in human subjects is rare, but it has been documented in clinical conditions and can potentially be induced in situations of Na depletion.

Salt preference has also been documented in human subjects. One small study that was conducted in twelve 4–6-month-old exclusively breast-fed infants with apparently no exposure to added salt indicated a preference for salted v. unsalted cereals(Reference Birch22). This may suggest that salt preference may be due to a genetic predisposition, as there was no prior exposure to salt as breast milk is low in salt and the cereal was the first food offered to the infants; the sample size was small and the results should be interpreted cautiously. Studies conducted in twins have determined that salty taste perception and preference are mostly caused by learned experiences rather than by genetics(Reference Wise, Hansen and Reed23). There is also evidence in adults that exposure to highly salty foods results in an increased preference for these foods(Reference Bertino, Beauchamp and Engelman24). Similarly, consumption of a low salt diet increased the perceived saltiness of foods and resulted in a preference for foods with lower salt concentrations(Reference Bertino, Beauchamp and Engelman25). It appears that salt preference is more likely to be influenced by environmental factors (repeated exposure to salt) rather than by genetic factors.

Responses to stress

Stress can be defined as ‘a complex physiological state that embodies a range of integrative physiological and behavioural processes that occur when there is a real or perceived threat to homoeostasis’(Reference Tilbrook and Fink26). Stressors (the noxious agents that threaten homoeostasis) can be physical, psychological or physiological(Reference Dobson and Smith27). Stress can be of short term (acute stress; lasting for seconds, minutes or up to a few hours), can occur repeatedly or on a daily basis (repeated acute stress) or can be continuous and prolonged (chronic stress; lasting for days, weeks or months)(Reference Turner, Hemsworth and Tilbrook28Reference Turner, Tilbrook, Ashworth and Kraeling30).

Several physiological pathways are activated by stress(Reference Turner, Canny and Hobbs31). In brief, an immediate response to acute stress is the activation of the sympatho-adrenal medullary system, which results in the release of noradrenaline from sympathetic nerve terminals in peripheral tissues and the release of adrenaline and noradrenaline from the adrenal medulla into the systemic blood. This is a rapid response, with plasma concentrations of adrenaline and noradrenaline reaching their peak within about 10–15 min(Reference Tilbrook, Rivalland and Turner32). Activation of this system prepares the individual for a ‘fight or flight’ response to a stressor including increasing heart rate and a redistribution of blood flow to skeletal and cardiac muscle and away from gastrointestinal activities.

Another response to stress is the activation of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis. When acute stress is encountered, corticotrophin-releasing hormone and arginine vasopressin are released from the hypothalamus into the hypophyseal portal blood vessels. These neuropeptides stimulate corticotrophs of the anterior pituitary gland to release adrenocorticotrophic hormone into the peripheral blood. In turn, adrenocorticotrophic hormone stimulates the adrenal cortex to synthesise and release glucocorticoids. The principal glucocorticoid synthesised and released in human subjects is cortisol. This is a less rapid response than that of adrenaline and noradrenaline, with plasma concentrations of cortisol reaching a peak within 20–60 min (depending on the type of stress encountered)(Reference Turner, Canny and Hobbs31Reference Turner, Hosking and Parr33). Cortisol mobilises energy stores through processes such as glycogenolysis in the liver and lypolysis in adipose tissue, thus making substrates available for activities that may be required to respond to the stress. Physiological responses to stress are summarised in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1 Physiological systems activated in response to stress. * Any element of the physiological systems activated during stress may be involved in inducing a salt appetite, and there is evidence for some of these actions in animal studies (see text for details). Nevertheless, since human subjects are generally salt replete due to an excess of salt in the food supply, these mechanisms are not likely to be active in human populations. CRH, corticotrophin-releasing hormone; ACTH, adrenocorticotrophic hormone.

Stress-induced salt appetite and potential physiological mechanism

The effect of stress on salt intake

Studies in rats, mice, rabbits and hamsters have investigated the effect of stress on daily salt intake, with most demonstrating an increase in salt intake in response to stress. Stressors of varying severity and duration have been imposed, and the intake of a test NaCl solution has been measured (Table 1). Eight studies reported an increase(Reference Bourjeili, Turner and Stinner34Reference Ely, Herman and Ely41), one study reported no change(Reference Howell, Harris and Clarke35) and two studies reported a decrease in salt intake(Reference Bensi, Bertuzzi and Armario42, Reference Niebylski, Bertuzzi and Bensi43). It appears that under these conditions, stress can increase salt intake.

Table 1 Summary of the effects of stress on salt intake in animals*

SHR, spontaneously hypertensive rat; WKYR, Wistar-Kyoto rats; NR, not reported.

* All differences are at P < 0·05 level.

Own control.

Intake measured after conclusion of stress.

§ Na depleted.

Physiological mechanism for a stress-induced salt appetite

Any element of the physiological systems activated during stress may mediate the effects of stress on salt intake. When rats that exhibited a stress-induced increase in salt intake were given a drug that blocked sympatho-adrenal medullary system activity and were then subjected to stress, salt intake from both food and fluids decreased(Reference Bourjeili, Turner and Stinner34, Reference Ely, Herman and Ely41). One possible explanation is that sympatho-adrenal medullary system activity may increase urinary Na excretion, resulting in a Na-depleted state which may increase Na appetite(Reference Light and Turner44, Reference Harshfield, Pulliam and Alpert45).

Corticotrophin-releasing hormone decreased salt intake when administered subcutaneously to sheep(Reference Weisinger, Blair-West and Burns46) and baboons(Reference Shade, Blair-West and Carey47) and when administered directly into the lateral parabrachial nucleus of the brain in rats(Reference De Castro e Silva, Fregoneze and Johnson48). In contrast, corticotrophin-releasing hormone increased salt intake after intracerebroventricular infusion in mice(Reference Denton, Blair-West and McBurnie37) and rabbits(Reference Tarjan and Denton49), and it had no effect after subcutaneous infusion in mice(Reference Denton, Blair-West and McBurnie37). Subcutaneous infusion of adrenocorticotrophic hormone stimulated salt intake in mice(Reference Denton, Blair-West and McBurnie37), sheep(Reference Weisinger, Blair-West and Burns46), rabbits(Reference Tarjan and Denton49) and rats(Reference Weisinger, Denton and McKinley50), although studies in baboons(Reference Shade, Blair-West and Carey47) and pigs(Reference Jankevicius and Widowski51) reported no effect of intramuscular injections of adrenocorticotrophic hormone. Glucocorticoids may also influence salt appetite, but possibly only when co-administered with mineralocorticoids. Shelat et al. (Reference Shelat, King and Flanagan-Cato52) found that glucocorticoids in isolation did not influence salt intake in rats, but when glucocorticoids were administered in combination with a mineralocorticoid, a salt appetite was induced, and this may have been mediated by the direct effects of angiotensin II in the brain. In another study conducted in rats, glucocorticoid co-administered with a mineralocorticoid was found to increase salt intake, and this may have been due to increased urinary excretion of water and Na(Reference Thunhorst, Beltz and Johnson53); a Na-induced appetite may have resulted from Na depletion(Reference Rowland, Farnbauch and Crews54). It is not clear why administration of elements of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis resulted in different outcomes in different studies. The route of administration may be important since salt appetite-regulatory pathways(Reference Geerling and Loewy55) may be influenced in a stimulatory or inhibitory manner through different routes of administration of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis hormones. There may also be species-specific differences related to the function of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis hormones in salt appetite. Further studies would be needed to elucidate the precise mechanisms involved. A systematic approach in a single species would be best, with caution being exercised in the extrapolation of the findings to human subjects.

The mechanism of salt appetite involves two processes: central regulation and regulation by the renal system. Mechanisms of central regulation of salt appetite are not fully resolved, but they are thought to include input signals from aldosterone and angiotensin II, from sensory inputs via baroreceptors and from detection of intracerebroventricular Na concentrations(Reference Geerling and Loewy55). Many different brainstem and forebrain regions (such as lamina terminalis and amygdala) are involved in the integration of these signals. These brain regions are involved in inducing motor responses such as salt-ingestive behaviours in the case of Na deficiency(Reference Geerling and Loewy55). Glucocorticoids are thought to enhance the salt appetite-promoting actions of aldosterone by increasing the concentration of mineralocorticoid receptors in the brain(Reference Ma, McEwen and Sakai56, Reference Zhang, Epstein and Schulkin57). The renal system is also involved in the regulation of Na levels via the renin–angiotensin–aldosterone system(Reference Sherwood58). When Na levels fall, renin is secreted by the kidney. Renin catalyses the conversion of angiotensinogen to angiotensin I and then to angiotensin II via angiotensin-converting enzyme. Angiotensin II results in the secretion of aldosterone from the adrenal cortex, which in turn increases Na reabsorption by the distal and collecting tubules of the kidney. Angiotensin II and aldosterone act directly on the lamina terminalis and amygdala to stimulate Na appetite.

Stress-induced salt intake: evidence from human studies

There are five laboratory studies conducted in human subjects, which allow close monitoring of food intake, that have examined the effect of stress on intake of salt and high salt foods (Table 2). One of these was a study that we conducted in men and women (n 20) with a mean age of 38·6 (sd 11·5) years and a mean BMI of 23·8 (sd 3·3) kg/m2, which investigated the effect of acute mental arithmetic stress induced in a laboratory setting on salt preference (Torres SJ & Nowson CA, unpublished results, 2008). Subjects were asked to indicate their preference for tomato juice with a range of salt concentrations (0, 109, 173, 240, 304, 370, 435 and 565 mmol/l) before and after acute mental stress. The mean perceived level of stress (range: 1 (no stress) to 10 (severe stress)) was 5·9 (sem 0·5). The mental stress test caused a significant increase in systolic BP (+13·8 (sem 2·2) mmHg), diastolic BP (+8·7 (sem 1·5) mmHg) and pulse rate (+11·2 (sem 7·9) beats per minute) (P < 0·05 for all). There was no significant difference in mean salt preferences in the non-stressed and post-stress states, 82 (sem 16) v. 96 (sem 13) mmol/l (P>0·05). All five laboratory studies reported significant increases in stress by either subjective or objective measures: three studies reported significant increases in BP and heart rate (Torres SJ & Nowson CA, unpublished results, 2008)(Reference Oliver, Wardle and Gibson59, Reference Miller, Friese and Dolgoy60); one study reported an increase in cortisol(Reference Epel, Lapidus and McEwen61) and three studies reported increases in self-reported stress (Torres SJ & Nowson CA, unpublished results, 2008)(Reference Oliver, Wardle and Gibson59, Reference Zellner, Loaiza and Gonzalez62). In summary, laboratory-based studies have found no effect of stress on salt intake in human subjects. Even though all the studies reported significant increases in stress, we may not see an effect on salt intake in laboratory studies as the response to acute stress induced may differ from chronic exposure to a stressful environment.

Table 2 Summary of the effects of stress on salt intake in human subjects*

TSST, Trier Social Stress Test.

* All differences are at P < 0·05 level.

Torres SJ & Nowson CA, unpublished results, 2008.

Own control.

§ Based on Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory scores.

Naturalistic studies provide the opportunity to measure the effect of life stress on salt intake. The effect of self-reported stress on eating behaviour was examined in 212 undergraduate students (Table 2)(Reference Oliver and Wardle63). Snacking increased during periods of stress, and foods eaten in greater quantity included sweets and chocolate, cakes and biscuits, and savoury snacks (high salt foods). However, the observed increase in the consumption of savoury snacks in response to stress may be due to a drive for fat rather than for salt, or the perception that snacks are treats/rewards. In a previous review, we concluded that chronic life stress seems to be associated with a greater preference for energy- and nutrient-dense foods, namely those that are high in sugar and fat(Reference Torres and Nowson64).

It has been suggested that cortisol, a key hormone secreted during stress, may be a critical factor in the drive for hedonic, highly palatable foods(Reference Dallman, la Fleur and Pecoraro65) such as foods containing a high content of salt(Reference Yeomans, Blundell and Leshem66). Cortisol may increase appetite by affecting leptin and neuropeptide Y, key hormones that reduce(Reference Blundell, Goodson and Halford67) and stimulate food intake(Reference Levine and Billington68), respectively. A study with fifty-nine premenopausal women subjected to 45 min of stress (visuospatial puzzles, serial subtraction of a high number from a low number and delivery of a videotaped speech) found that women with high cortisol reactions (defined as the increase from baseline to stress levels of salivary cortisol) consumed significantly more high fat sweet foods but the same amount of salty foods (potato chips) compared with women with low cortisol reactions (Table 2)(Reference Epel, Lapidus and McEwen61). In a study with six men, administration of cortisol over 5 d, which significantly increased BP, did not alter salt preference(Reference Wong, Williamson and Brown69). Currently, there is limited evidence to support the suggestion that cortisol can increase salt intake in human subjects.

Thus, evidence from laboratory studies in human subjects, who are consuming salt in amounts in excess of physiological requirement, indicate that stress is not likely to influence salt intake.

Does stress induce salt intake in human subjects?

While animal models provide some evidence for a relationship between stress and salt intake, this has not been demonstrated in laboratory studies conducted in human subjects. We may not see an effect in laboratory studies in this acute stress situation, and this may differ from chronic stress that is experienced in the real world. This is supported by the findings of one study which found that during periods of life stress, consumption of highly salty snack foods increased(Reference Oliver and Wardle63); this finding will need to be confirmed in future studies. Importantly, an alternative explanation will need to be considered for why salty foods are consumed during periods of life stress, such as insufficient time to purchase and prepare foods and increased use of convenience foods which are typically high in salt, or as a learned response to a stressful situation.

The physiological requirement for Na in human subjects is 8·0–10·0 mmol/d (8·5–10·3 mmol salt/d)(70), yet many Westernised populations are consuming in excess of these requirements, up to twenty-four times of what is needed(71). In a study conducted in Australia, Na intake determined from 24 h urinary Na excretion was 118·0 mmol/d in women and 170·0 mmol/d in men, which is eighteen and seventeen times of what is needed in women and men, respectively(Reference Beard, Woodward and Ball72). These current high intakes of salt are due to the abundance of salt in the food supply, particularly in processed food products(Reference James, Ralph and Sanchez-Castillo73). Examples of population groups that have very low salt intakes are rare and limited to traditional communities(Reference Denton2). Therefore, it seems that in situations of Na depletion, human subjects could exhibit an increase in salt appetite, but this is generally not seen as salt is mostly consumed in amounts which exceed requirements. This was confirmed by two animal studies which found that rats on high intakes of salt, in excess of physiological requirements, exhibited no change in salt intake in response to stress(Reference Howell, Harris and Clarke35), whereas rats on lower intakes did increase their salt intake in response to stress(Reference Bourjeili, Turner and Stinner34). We may also not see a shift in salt preference in human subjects at these current high intakes of salt.

Conclusions

Data from animal studies suggest that stress might be a driver for salt intake, with evidence for a potential mechanism by the sympatho-adrenal medullary system and/or hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis. In contrast, laboratory studies conducted in human subjects have found no effect of acute stress on salt intake. However, one study which measured the effect of life stress found that intake of snack foods including highly salty foods did increase, although there are likely to be a range of drivers for this behaviour other than a craving for salt taste. Stress could induce a learned response to consume comfort foods during stress which could include high salt foods. The majority of studies in human subjects have investigated the effect of acute stress. Studies investigating the influence of chronic stress on eating behaviours are required, including consumption of salty foods. In the current environment where most human subjects are consuming Na well in excess of physiological requirements, acute stress is unlikely to increase salt intake.

Acknowledgements

S. J. T. performed the literature review and wrote the manuscript except for the sections on ‘Responses to stress’ and ‘Physiological mechanism for a stress-induced salt appetite’, which were written by A. I. T.; C. A. N. provided expert input and guidance. All authors have read and approved all sections of the manuscript, and participated in the decision to submit for publication. The authors have no conflict of interest. The present research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

References

1Henry, JP (1988) Stress, salt and hypertension. Soc Sci Med 26, 293302.Google Scholar
2Denton, D (1982) The Hunger for Salt: An Anthropological, Physiological and Medical Analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
3Prior, IA, Evans, JG, Harvey, HP, et al. (1968) Sodium intake and blood pressure in two Polynesian populations. N Engl J Med 279, 515520.Google Scholar
4Bennett, SA & Magnus, P (1994) Trends in cardiovascular risk factors in Australia. Med J Aust 161, 519527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5World Health Organisation (2002) Reducing the Risks, Promoting Healthy Life, Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
6Whitworth, JA; World Health Organization, International Society of Hypertension Writing Group (2003) 2003 World Health Organization (WHO)/International Society of Hypertension (ISH) statement on management of hypertension. J Hypertens 21, 19831992.Google ScholarPubMed
7Elliott, P, Stamler, J, Nichols, R, et al. (1996) Intersalt revisited: further analyses of 24 hour sodium excretion and blood pressure within and across populations. Intersalt Cooperative Research Group. BMJ 312, 12491253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Law, MR (1997) Epidemiologic evidence on salt and blood pressure. Am J Hypertens 10, 42S45S.Google Scholar
9He, FJ & MacGregor, GA (2007) Salt, blood pressure and cardiovascular disease. Curr Opin Cardiol 22, 298305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10Suter, PM, Sierro, C & Vetter, W (2002) Nutritional factors in the control of blood pressure and hypertension. Nutr Clin Care 5, 919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Jurgens, G & Graudal, NA (2004) Effects of low sodium diet versus high sodium diet on blood pressure, renin, aldosterone, catecholamines, cholesterols, and triglyceride. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, issue 1, CD004022.http://www.cochrane.org/reviews/en/ab004022.html.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12Bunker, SJ, Colquhoun, DM, Esler, MD, et al. (2003) ‘Stress’ and coronary heart disease: psychosocial risk factors. National Heart Foundation of Australia position statement update. MJA 178, 272276.Google Scholar
13Stricker, EM (1980) The physiological basis of sodium appetite: a new look at the ‘depletion–repletion’ model. In Biological and Behavioral Aspects of Salt Intake, pp. 185204 [Kare, M, Fregly, M and Bernard, R, editors]. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14Mattes, RD (1997) The taste for salt in humans. Am J Clin Nutr 65, 692S697S.Google Scholar
15Nicolaidis, S (2008) Prenatal imprinting of postnatal specific appetites and feeding behavior. Metabolism 57, Suppl. 2, S22S26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16Chandrashekar, J, Kuhn, C, Oka, Y, et al. (2010) The cells and peripheral representation of sodium taste in mice. Nature (Epublication ahead of print version 27 January 2010).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Kumar, P & Clark, M (1994) Clinical Medicine, 3rd ed.London: Bailliere Tindall.Google Scholar
18Thorn, G, Dorrance, S & Day, E (1942) Addison's disease: evaluation of synthetic desoxycorticosterone acetate therapy in 158 patients. Ann Intern Med 16, 10531096.Google Scholar
19Knoers, N & Levtchenko, EN (2008) Gitelman syndrome. Orphanet J Rare Dis 3, 22.Google Scholar
20Cruz, DN, Shaer, AJ, Bia, MJ, et al. (2001) Gitelman's syndrome revisited: an evaluation of symptoms and health-related quality of life. Kidney Int 59, 710717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Beauchamp, GK, Bertino, M, Burke, D, et al. (1990) Experimental sodium depletion and salt taste in normal human volunteers. Am J Clin Nutr 51, 881889.Google Scholar
22Birch, LL (1999) Development of food preferences. Annu Rev Nutr 19, 4162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23Wise, PM, Hansen, JL, Reed, DR, et al. (2007) Twin study of the heritability of recognition thresholds for sour and salty taste. Chem Senses 32, 749754.Google Scholar
24Bertino, M, Beauchamp, GK & Engelman, K (1986) Increasing dietary salt alters salt taste preference. Physiol Behav 38, 203213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25Bertino, M, Beauchamp, GK & Engelman, K (1982) Long-term reduction in dietary sodium alters the taste of salt. Am J Clin Nutr 36, 11341144.Google Scholar
26Tilbrook, AJ (2007) Neuropeptides, stress-related. In Encylopedia of Stress, 2nd ed., pp. 903908 [Fink, G, editor]. Oxford: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
27Dobson, H & Smith, RF (1995) Stress and reproduction in farm-animals. J Reprod Fertil Suppl 49, 451461.Google ScholarPubMed
28Turner, AI, Hemsworth, PH & Tilbrook, AJ (2002) Susceptibility of reproduction in female pigs to impairment by stress and the role of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis. Reprod Fertil Dev 14, 377391.Google Scholar
29Turner, AI, Hemsworth, PH & Tilbrook, AJ (2005) Susceptibility of reproduction in female pigs to impairment by stress or elevation of cortisol. Domest Anim Endocrinol 29, 398410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30Turner, A & Tilbrook, A (2006) Stress, cortisol and reproduction in female pigs. In Control of Pig Reproduction VII, Reproduction Supplement, vol. 62, pp. 191203 [Ashworth, C and Kraeling, R, editors]. Nottingham: Nottingham University Press.Google Scholar
31Turner, AI, Canny, BJ, Hobbs, RJ, et al. (2002) Influence of sex and gonadal status of sheep on cortisol secretion in response to ACTH and on cortisol and LH secretion in response to stress: importance of different stressors. J Endocrinol 173, 113122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32Tilbrook, AJ, Rivalland, EA, Turner, AI, et al. (2008) Responses of the hypothalamopituitary adrenal axis and the sympathoadrenal system to isolation/restraint stress in sheep of different adiposity. Neuroendocrinology 87, 193205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33Turner, AI, Hosking, BJ, Parr, RA, et al. (2006) A sex difference in the cortisol response to tail docking and ACTH develops between 1 and 8 weeks of age in lambs. J Endocrinol 188, 443449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34Bourjeili, N, Turner, M, Stinner, J, et al. (1995) Sympathetic nervous system influences salt appetite in four strains of rats. Physiol Behav 58, 437443.Google Scholar
35Howell, LA, Harris, RBS, Clarke, C, et al. (1999) The effects of restraint stress on intake of preferred and nonpreferred solutions in rodents. Physiol Behav 65, 697704.Google Scholar
36Denton, DA, Coghlan, JP, Fei, DT, et al. (1984) Stress, ACTH, salt intake and high blood pressure. Clin Exp Hypertens A6, 403415.Google Scholar
37Denton, DA, Blair-West, JR, McBurnie, MI, et al. (1999) Effect of adrenocorticotrophic hormone on sodium appetite in mice. Am J Physiol 277, R1033R1040.Google Scholar
38Ely, DE, Thoren, P, Wiegand, J, et al. (1987) Sodium appetite as well as 24-h variations of fluid balance, mean arterial pressure and heart rate in spontaneously hypertensive (SHR) and normotensive (WKY) rats, when on various sodium diets. Acta Physiol Scand 129, 8192.Google Scholar
39Kuta, CC, Bryant, HU, Zabik, JE, et al. (1984) Stress, endogenous opiods and salt intake. Appetite 5, 5360.Google Scholar
40Leshem, M, Maroun, M & Del Canho, S (1996) Sodium depletion and maternal separation in the suckling rat increase its salt intake when adult. Physiol Behav 59, 199204.Google Scholar
41Ely, D, Herman, M, Ely, L, et al. (2000) Sodium intake is increased by social stress and the Y chromosome and reduced by clonidine. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 278, R407R412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
42Bensi, N, Bertuzzi, M, Armario, A, et al. (1997) Chronic immobilization stress reduces sodium intake and renal excretion in rats. Physiol Behav 62, 13911396.Google Scholar
43Niebylski, A, Bertuzzi, M, Bensi, N, et al. (2000) Renal excretion and saline intake during post-stress immobilization period in rats. Arch Physiol Biochem 108, 268274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44Light, K (1992) Differential responses to salt intake–stress interactions. Relevance to hypertension. In Individual Differences in Cardiovascular Responses to Stress, [Turner, J, editor]. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
45Harshfield, GA, Pulliam, DA & Alpert, BS (1991) Patterns of sodium-excretion during sympathetic nervous-system arousal. Hypertension 17, 11561160.Google Scholar
46Weisinger, RS, Blair-West, JR, Burns, P, et al. (2000) The inhibitory effect of hormones associated with stress on Na appetite of sheep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97, 29222927.Google Scholar
47Shade, RE, Blair-West, JR, Carey, KD, et al. (2002) Ingestive responses to administration of stress hormones in baboons. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 282, R10R18.Google Scholar
48De Castro e Silva, E, Fregoneze, JB & Johnson, AK (2006) Corticotropin-releasing hormone in the lateral parabrachial nucleus inhibits sodium appetite in rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 290, R1136R1141.Google Scholar
49Tarjan, E & Denton, D (1991) Sodium/water intake of rabbits following administration of hormones of stress. Brain Res Bull 26, 133136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50Weisinger, RS, Denton, DA, McKinley, MJ, et al. (1978) ACTH induced sodium appetite in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 8, 339342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51Jankevicius, ML & Widowski, TM (2003) Exogenous adrenocorticotrophic hormone does not elicit a salt appetite in growing pigs. Physiol Behav 78, 277284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
52Shelat, SG, King, JL, Flanagan-Cato, LM, et al. (1999) Mineralocorticoids and glucocorticoids cooperatively increase salt intake and angiotensin II receptor binding in rat brain. Neuroendocrinology 69, 339351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53Thunhorst, RL, Beltz, TG & Johnson, AK (2007) Glucocorticoids increase salt appetite by promoting water and sodium excretion. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 293, R1444R1451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54Rowland, NE, Farnbauch, LJ & Crews, EC (2004) Sodium deficiency and salt appetite in ICR: CD1 mice. Physiol Behav 80, 629635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55Geerling, JC & Loewy, AD (2008) Central regulation of sodium appetite. Exp Physiol 93, 177209.Google Scholar
56Ma, LY, McEwen, BS, Sakai, RR, et al. (1993) Glucocorticoids facilitate mineralocorticoid-induced sodium intake in the rat. Horm Behav 27, 240250.Google Scholar
57Zhang, DM, Epstein, AN & Schulkin, J (1993) Medial region of the amygdala: involvement in adrenal-steroid-induced salt appetite. Brain Res 600, 2026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58Sherwood, L (2001) Human Physiology from Cells to Systems, 4th ed. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
59Oliver, G, Wardle, J & Gibson, L (2000) Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. Psychosom Med 62, 853865.Google Scholar
60Miller, SB, Friese, M, Dolgoy, L, et al. (1998) Hostility, sodium consumption, and cardiovascular response to interpersonal stress. Psychosom Med 60, 7177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
61Epel, E, Lapidus, R, McEwen, B, et al. (2001) Stress may add bite to appetite in women: a laboratory study of stress-induced cortisol and eating behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 3749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62Zellner, DA, Loaiza, S, Gonzalez, Z, et al. (2006) Food selection changes under stress. Physiol Behav 87, 789793.Google Scholar
63Oliver, G & Wardle, J (1999) Perceived effects of stress on food choice. Physiol Behav 66, 511515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64Torres, SJ & Nowson, CA (2007) Relationship between stress, eating behavior, and obesity. Nutrition 23, 887894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65Dallman, MF, la Fleur, SE, Pecoraro, NC, et al. (2004) Minireview: glucocorticoids – food intake, abdominal obesity, and wealthy nations in 2004. Endocrinology 145, 26332638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66Yeomans, MR, Blundell, JE & Leshem, M (2004) Palatability: response to nutritional need or need-free stimulation of appetite? Br J Nutr 92, S3S14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
67Blundell, JE, Goodson, S & Halford, JCG (2001) Regulation of appetite: role of leptin in signalling systems for drive and satiety. Int J Obes 25, S29S34.Google Scholar
68Levine, AS & Billington, CJ (1997) Why do we eat? A neural systems approach. Ann Rev Nutr 17, 597619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69Wong, KS, Williamson, PM, Brown, MA, et al. (1993) Effects of cortisol on blood pressure and salt preference in normal humans. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 20, 121126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70World Health Organisation (2007) Reducing Salt Intake in Populations. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
71Intersalt Cooperative Research Group (1988) Intersalt: an international study of electrolyte excretion and blood pressure. Results for 24 hour urinary sodium and potassium excretion. Intersalt Cooperative Research Group. BMJ 297, 319328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
72Beard, TC, Woodward, DR, Ball, PJ, et al. (1997) The Hobart Salt Study 1995: few meet national sodium intake target. Med J Aust 166, 404407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
73James, WP, Ralph, A & Sanchez-Castillo, CP (1987) The dominance of salt in manufactured food in the sodium intake of affluent societies. Lancet 1, 426429.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Physiological systems activated in response to stress. * Any element of the physiological systems activated during stress may be involved in inducing a salt appetite, and there is evidence for some of these actions in animal studies (see text for details). Nevertheless, since human subjects are generally salt replete due to an excess of salt in the food supply, these mechanisms are not likely to be active in human populations. CRH, corticotrophin-releasing hormone; ACTH, adrenocorticotrophic hormone.

Figure 1

Table 1 Summary of the effects of stress on salt intake in animals*

Figure 2

Table 2 Summary of the effects of stress on salt intake in human subjects*