Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T05:02:36.617Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Brain correlates of impaired goal management in bipolar mania

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2021

Mercé Madre
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Addictive Behaviours Unit, Psychiatry Department, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Barcelona, Spain
Paola Fuentes-Claramonte*
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain
Pol Palau
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Benito Menni CASM, Barcelona, Spain Fundació Privada Hospital Asil de Granollers, Granollers, Spain Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Naia Sáez
Affiliation:
Hospital de Sant Rafael, Barcelona, Spain
Noemí Moro
Affiliation:
Benito Menni CASM, Barcelona, Spain
Clara Blanch
Affiliation:
Hospital Sagrat Cor, Martorell, Spain
Norma Verdolini
Affiliation:
Bipolar Disorders and Depressive Unit, Institute of Neuroscience, Hospital Clínic, University of Barcelona, Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain
Maria Angeles Garcia-Leon
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain
Isabel Feria
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Benito Menni CASM, Barcelona, Spain
Josep Munuera
Affiliation:
Unitat de Diagnòstic per la Imatge, Fundació de Recerca, Hospital de Sant Joan de Déu, Barcelona, Spain
Salvador Sarró
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain
Joaquim Raduà
Affiliation:
Imaging of Mood- and Anxiety-Related Disorders (IMARD) Group, Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain Early Psychosis: Interventions and Clinical-detection (EPIC) Lab, Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Centre for Psychiatric Research and Education, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Peter McKenna
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain
Raymond Salvador
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain
Edith Pomarol-Clotet
Affiliation:
FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain
*
Authors for correspondence: Mercé Madre, E-mail: mmadrer@gmail.com; Paola Fuentes-Claramonte, E-mail: pfuentes@fidmag.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Although executive impairment has been reported in mania, its brain functional correlates have been relatively little studied. This study examined goal management, believed to be more closely related to executive impairment in daily life than other executive tasks, using a novel functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) paradigm in patients in this illness phase.

Methods

Twenty-one currently manic patients with bipolar disorder and 30 matched healthy controls were scanned while performing the Computerized Multiple Elements Test (CMET). This requires participants to sequentially play four simple games, with transition between games being made either voluntarily (executive condition) or automatically (control condition).

Results

CMET performance was impaired in the manic patients compared to the healthy controls. Manic patients failed to increase activation in the lateral frontal, cingulate and inferior parietal cortex when the executive demands of the task increased, while this increase was observed in the healthy controls. Activity in these regions was associated with task performance.

Conclusions

Manic patients show evidence of impaired goal management, which is associated with a pattern of reduced medial and lateral frontal and parietal activity.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Besides its core clinical feature of episodes of mania and depression, bipolar disorder is now recognized to be accompanied by cognitive impairment. In some cases, this persists into euthymia (Anderson, Haddad, & Scott, Reference Anderson, Haddad and Scott2012), where it has been found to be associated with impaired daily life functioning (Wingo, Harvey, & Baldessarini, Reference Wingo, Harvey and Baldessarini2009). Neurocognitive impairment has been documented in bipolar disorder across different cognitive domains, but executive function and working memory seem to be particularly affected (Bourne et al., Reference Bourne, Aydemir, Balanzá-Martínez, Bora, Brissos, Cavanagh and Goodwin2013), and executive impairment may even persist during clinical remission (Dickinson, Becerra, & Coombes, Reference Dickinson, Becerra and Coombes2017).

Mania appears to be characterized by more marked cognitive deficits than euthymia (Bora, Reference Bora2018), and may be a risk factor for subsequent development of cognitive impairment in the euthymic phase (Robinson & Ferrier, Reference Robinson and Ferrier2006). Regarding executive impairment specifically, Volkert et al. (Reference Volkert, Schiele, Kazmaier, Glaser, Zierhut, Kopf and Reif2015) examined 35 depressed bipolar patients, 20 hypomanic or manic patients and 55 healthy controls on a neuropsychological test battery covering attention, working memory, verbal memory and executive functioning and found that depression was characterized particularly by psychomotor slowing, whereas manic patients showed severe deficits in executive functioning. Murphy et al. (Reference Murphy, Rubinsztein, Michael, Rogers, Robbins, Paykel and Sahakian2001) also found that manic patients showed more wide-ranging impairment than depressed patients (characterized by a milder impairment) and healthy controls on a gambling task thought to reflect ventrolateral and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) functions.

An important open question is how far executive dysfunction contributes to the pattern of functional brain changes associated with bipolar disorder, and particularly manic episodes, which take the form of underactivity in prefrontal and some other cortical regions at rest and during task performance, coupled with overactivity in subcortical structures such as the amygdala and hippocampus (Green, Cahill, & Malhi, Reference Green, Cahill and Malhi2007; Kupferschmidt & Zakzanis, Reference Kupferschmidt and Zakzanis2011; Strakowski et al., Reference Strakowski, Adler, Almeida, Altshuler, Blumberg, Chang and Townsend2012). Chen, Suckling, Lennox, Ooi, and Bullmore (Reference Chen, Suckling, Lennox, Ooi and Bullmore2011) meta-analyzed voxel-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies with different cognitive and emotional components. In a subanalysis of 29 studies using purely cognitive tasks they found that, across all mood states, patients showed decreased activation in the inferior frontal gyrus, the lingual gyrus, and the putamen, while abnormal subcortical activation was much less prominent than in the subanalysis of 22 studies using emotional tasks. Functional imaging studies of manic patients using executive tasks specifically have supported the above finding of reduced activation in the inferior frontal gyrus during the performance of tasks requiring inhibition of prepotent responses, such as the go/no-go task (Altshuler et al., Reference Altshuler, Bookheimer, Townsend, Proenza, Eisenberger, Sabb and Cohen2005; Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Ogilvie, Rubinsztein, Calderon, Dolan and Sahakian2004; Hummer et al., Reference Hummer, Hulvershorn, Karne, Gunn, Wang and Anand2013; Mazzola-Pomietto, Kaladjian, Azorin, Anton, & Jeanningros, Reference Mazzola-Pomietto, Kaladjian, Azorin, Anton and Jeanningros2009; Strakowski et al., Reference Strakowski, Adler, Cerullo, Eliassen, Lamy, Fleck and Delbello2008), the Stroop task (Blumberg et al., Reference Blumberg, Leung, Skudlarski, Lacadie, Fredericks, Harris and Peterson2003), or the Stop Signal task (Cerullo et al., Reference Cerullo, Adler, Lamy, Eliassen, Fleck, Strakowski and Delbello2009; Pavuluri, Passarotti, Harral, & Sweeney, Reference Pavuluri, Passarotti, Harral and Sweeney2010). Other regions involved in response inhibition, like the ACC, have also shown hypoactivation, but less consistently. On the contrary, two studies that used a working memory task, such as the n-back task (Pomarol-Clotet et al., Reference Pomarol-Clotet, Moro, Sarró, Goikolea, Vieta, Amann and Salvador2012; Townsend, Bookheimer, Foland-Ross, Sugar, & Altshuler, Reference Townsend, Bookheimer, Foland-Ross, Sugar and Altshuler2010), found reduced activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the parietal cortex, across different mood states in one case (Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Bookheimer, Foland-Ross, Sugar and Altshuler2010) and specifically linked to mania in the other (Pomarol-Clotet et al., Reference Pomarol-Clotet, Moro, Sarró, Goikolea, Vieta, Amann and Salvador2012).

A further, relatively recently recognized aspect of executive function is goal management. This refers to the ability to carry out tasks while simultaneously keeping in mind and dealing with ongoing competing demands (Cullen, Brennan, Manly, & Evans, Reference Cullen, Brennan, Manly and Evans2016; Duncan, Emslie, Williams, Johnson, & Freer, Reference Duncan, Emslie, Williams, Johnson and Freer1996; Shallice & Burgess, Reference Shallice and Burgess1991). Impairment leads to so-called goal neglect, defined by Duncan et al. (Reference Duncan, Emslie, Williams, Johnson and Freer1996) as an active search for a path toward a behavioral goal being replaced by performance that seems passive, inert, stereotyped, or fragmented, and also prone to irrelevant or ill-judged intrusions. Goal neglect is commonly seen in patients with frontal lobe lesions (Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Emslie, Williams, Johnson and Freer1996), and has been argued to capture better than classical executive tests the kind of problems that such patients show in real life (Burgess et al., Reference Burgess, Alderman, Forbes, Costello, Coates, Dawson and Channon2006). It is typically measured by tasks such as the Six Elements Task (Wilson, Alderman, Burgess, Emslie, & Evans, Reference Wilson, Alderman, Burgess, Emslie and Evans1996), where subjects have to carry out parts of different tasks in a 10-min time period in circumstances where it would be impossible to complete all of them in the allotted time. The subjects accordingly must keep in mind the overall goal of the task by periodically switching from task to task. However, although neuropsychological studies often include goal management measures when examining executive function, there are, to our knowledge, no imaging studies on goal management in bipolar disorder and particularly in manic episodes. The present study took advantage of a recently developed fMRI-adapted version of the Six Elements Task to examine this aspect of executive function in bipolar disorder, specifically in patients with mania where the evidence of executive impairment is greatest.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-six patients with a manic episode were recruited from four hospitals in the metropolitan area of Barcelona, Spain (Benito Menni CASM, Sant Boi de Llobregat; Hospital General de Granollers; Hospital Sant Rafael, Barcelona; Hospital Sagrat Cor, Martorell). All patients fulfilled DSM-V criteria for bipolar disorder, confirmed by clinical interview and review of case notes. Patients were excluded if: (a) they were aged <18 or >65 years, (b) had a history of brain trauma or neurological disease, (c) had shown alcohol/substance abuse within 12 months prior to participation, or (d) had any contraindication for MRI scanning. Severity of manic and depressive symptoms was scored using the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (Young, Biggs, Ziegler, & Meyer, Reference Young, Biggs, Ziegler and Meyer1978) and Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS-21) (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1960). To be included in the study, patients were required to score ⩾17 in the YMRS and ⩽8 in the HDRS-21. Clinical evaluation also included the Positive Scale from the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (Kay, Flszbein, & Opfer, Reference Kay, Flszbein and Opfer1987). Overall severity of illness was rated using the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale (GAF) (Endicott, Spitzer, Fleiss, & Cohen, Reference Endicott, Spitzer, Fleiss and Cohen1976) and the Clinical Global Impression Scale (CGI) (Guy, Reference Guy1976). Clinical assessment took place within 72 h before the scanning session. Pre-morbid intelligence quotient (IQ) was estimated using the Word Accentuation Test (Test de Acentuación de Palabras, TAP (Del Ser, Gonzalez-Montalvo, Martinez-Espinosa, Delgado-Villapalos, & Bermejo, Reference Del Ser, Gonzalez-Montalvo, Martinez-Espinosa, Delgado-Villapalos and Bermejo1997; Gomar et al., Reference Gomar, Ortiz-Gil, McKenna, Salvador, Sans-Sansa, Sarró and Pomarol-Clotet2011), a word reading test requiring pronunciation of Spanish words whose accents have been removed).

Thirty healthy controls, selected to be matched to the patients for age, sex and estimated IQ, as estimated by the TAP, were recruited from non-medical staff working in the hospital, their relatives and acquaintances, plus independent sources in the community. They were questioned and excluded if they reported a personal or family history of mental illness and/or treatment with psychotropic medication. All participants were right-handed.

All participants gave written informed consent prior to participation. All the study procedures had been previously approved by the Research Ethics Committee FIDMAG Sisters Hospitallers (Comité de Ética de la Investigación de FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias) and complied with its ethical standards on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Healthy controls received a gift-card as a compensation for their participation in the study.

CMET task

We used an adapted version of the CMET paradigm developed by Cullen et al. (Reference Cullen, Brennan, Manly and Evans2016) that we have validated in healthy individuals (Fuentes-Claramonte et al., Reference Fuentes-Claramonte, Santo-Angles, Argila-Plaza, Lechon, Guardiola-Ripoll, Almodovar-Payá and Salvador2021). In the task, subjects had to play four different simple games, which were displayed sequentially in pseudorandom order. The games were all similar and involved moving an interactive element on the screen to the left or to the right (with their left or right index fingers) to score points: in the first game (Car), the participant had to move a car to pick up fuel from the road; in the second (Catch), they had to move a tube to receive balloons that fell from the sky; in the third (Ball), they had to move a bar to keep a ball in movement and bouncing to the walls on the screen; in the last game (Brick), participants had to move a bar to use a ball to break bricks on the screen (Fig. 1). During each task block subjects were required to play these games and earn as many points as possible. The games were played in two conditions: in the control condition (automatic switching), the transition between games was automatically done by the computer (without intervention of the participant) and game duration was always the same, 12 s. All games were played once in each block, adding to a total block duration = 48 s. In the executive condition (voluntary switching), the transition between games was made voluntarily by button-press (right thumb) and subjects were instructed to divide their time equally between the four games, although no information about time played was available to them, or were they given instructions on which was the optimal strategy (i.e. playing 12 s per game). Thus, the executive condition required subjects to play the games to earn points but also to keep in mind that they needed to switch games regularly to be able to play all of them in each block. Four blocks of each condition were presented in alternating order (each one lasting 48 s), starting with the automatic condition to serve as a reference for switching time. Instructions on the condition of the subsequent block were presented for 3 s immediately before each block started. Between blocks, a fixation cross was presented for 9 s. Total task duration was 8 min and 10 s. Before scanning, participants underwent a practice session where they learned how to use the game controllers to play and switch games, but without any timing requirements. Although they were reminded that they should play each game for approximately the same time during the scanning session, they were free to practice for as long as they needed to get familiar with the games during the practice session. There were no automatic switches during the practice session.

Fig. 1. CMET task. Participants sequentially played four games requiring left or right index button presses during each 48s block. In the automatic switching condition, the game changed every 12s without intervention of the participant. In the voluntary switching condition, the participant had to actively switch games by button press (right thumb), with the instruction to spend approximately the same amount of time playing each game. No time information was shown during either condition. A fixation cross was shown during baseline periods between blocks (12s).

Stimuli were presented via MRI-compatible goggles (VisuaStim, Resonance Technology), and participants performed the task with an MRI-compatible response system (ResponseGrips, NordicNeuroLab).

Behavioral measures

During task performance, the total number of voluntary switches and the number of voluntary switches per block were registered. Additionally, we used the deviation from the optimal playing time as a measure of accuracy. The optimal playing time in each game was 12 s in each voluntary switching block. The deviation from this optimal time was calculated as the total time (in s) exceeding 12 s per game played for each block (time underplaying and overplaying a game were complementary, so only overplaying was penalized to avoid counting time twice). The deviation from optimal playing time was the sum of these deviations across the four blocks in the task, giving a range from 0 (perfect execution, played 12 s for all games in all blocks) to 144 (worse execution, no voluntary switches performed).

MRI data acquisition

All subjects were scanned using the same 3T Philips Ingenia scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) located at the Sant Joan de Déu Hospital in Barcelona (Spain). Functional data were acquired using a T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence with 245 volumes and the following acquisition parameters: TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 70°, in-plane resolution = 3.5 × 3.5 mm, FOV = 238 × 245 mm, slice thickness = 3.5 mm, inter-slice gap = 0.75 mm. Slices (32 per volume) were acquired with an interleaved order parallel to the AC–PC plane. A high-resolution anatomical volume was acquired using a Fast Field Echo (FFE) sequence for anatomical reference and inspection (TR = 9.90 ms; TE = 4.60 ms; flip angle = 8°; voxel size = 1 × 1 mm; slice thickness = 1 mm; slice number = 180; FOV = 240 mm).

Image preprocessing and analysis

Preprocessing and analysis were carried out with the FEAT module included in FSL 6.0 (FMRIB Software Library) software (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Woolrich, Beckmann, Behrens, Johansen-Berg and Matthews2004). The first 10 s (five volumes) of the sequence, corresponding to signal stabilization, were discarded. Preprocessing included motion correction (using the MCFLIRT algorithm), co-registration and normalization to a common stereotactic space (MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute template). For accurate registration, a two-step process was used. First, brain extraction was applied to the structural image, and the functional sequence was registered to it. Then the structural image was registered to the standard template. These two transformations were used to finally register the functional sequence to the standard space. Before group analyses, normalized images were spatially filtered with a Gaussian filter (FWHM = 5 mm). To minimize unwanted movement-related effects, individuals with an estimated maximum absolute movement >3.0 mm or an average absolute movement >0.3 mm were excluded from the study.

General linear models (GLM) were used to obtain activation maps in the two groups. At the first level, we defined a regressor for each condition: one for automatic switching blocks and one for voluntary switching blocks (fixation periods were not modeled and acted as implicit baseline). The contrast of interest was voluntary switching > automatic switching, to identify regions of increased sustained activation when playing the games with goal management demands. Additional contrasts were built to compare each condition against fixation. GLMs were fitted to generate individual activation maps for these contrasts and second level (group) analyses were performed within the FEAT module by means of mixed-effects GLMs (Beckmann, Jenkinson, & Smith, Reference Beckmann, Jenkinson and Smith2003). Statistical tests were carried out at p < 0.05, corrected at the cluster level using Gaussian random field methods. A threshold of z > 2.6 (p < 0.005) was used to define the initial set of clusters.

Given that a proportion of the patients achieved very few or no task switches during the voluntary switching blocks, and this could bias the comparison between conditions, we run a supplementary analysis with a different first-level design aimed to test brain activity in the voluntary blocks, relative to the rest of the task, controlling for the effect of playing the games and the transient signal changes associated with game switches (see online Supplementary information for details on this analysis and the results).

Results

From the 36 recruited manic bipolar patients, five did not complete the CMET task and eight were excluded because of excessive movement in the scanner, one due to an incidental MRI finding and one due to being left-handed. We compared 21 manic patients with 30 healthy controls. Demographic and clinical data for the patients and controls are shown in Table 1. The groups were not significantly different for age, sex and estimated premorbid IQ. Patients included in the study were on treatment with mood stabilizers (n = 17) (lithium alone or in combination n = 13, valproate alone or in combination n = 5, other mood stabilizers n = 1) and with antidepressants (n = 1); 18 were taking antipsychotics (second generation n = 16, combination n = 2) with a chlorpromazine equivalent dose of 387.44 mg/d (s.d. = 137.78).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical data for the sample included in the analysis

Values are means (s.d.).

Behavioral performance

The relevant behavioral variables (number of voluntary switches and deviation from optimal playing time) were not normally distributed, so analyses were performed with non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon rank-sum test for group comparison and Spearman's rho for correlation). The mean total number of voluntary switches was significantly lower in the manic patients than in the healthy subjects (mania mean = 8.19, s.d. = 6.62, range = 0–24; control mean = 12.97, s.d. = 5.66, range = 0–32; W = 449, p = 0.01). A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with one within-subjects factor (Game, with four levels, that coded the total time spent on each game) and one between-subjects factor (Group, with two levels, bipolar and control) revealed that no game was preferred over the others, since there were no differences in time spent on each game (no significant main effect of Game, F = 1.293, p = 0.280), nor a significant Game × Group interaction (F = 1.014, p = 0.378).

Deviation from optimal playing time (which reflects the difference between actual time spent playing each game and the gold standard of 12 s per block) was significantly larger in the manic patients compared to the healthy subjects (mania mean = 88.16s, s.d. = 39.29, range = 26.51–144; control mean = 37.14s, s.d. = 29.97, range = 8.38–144; W = 81, p < 0.001). Behavioral performance showed a learning effect, with deviation from optimal playing time becoming smaller as the task progressed in both groups (online Supplementary Fig. S1). This was confirmed by a linear mixed effects model with Block and Group as fixed effects and Subject as grouping factor (random effect), which showed a significant linear effect for Block and significant Group differences, but no Block×Group interaction (see online Supplementary Table S1 for the complete results).

In the patient group, scores in the YMRS showed a trend-level correlation with deviation from optimal playing time (ρ = 0.42, p = 0.058). We also explored the association between behavioral measures and scores in behavioral disorganization, with item P2 from the PANSS (conceptual disorganization) and item 7 from the YMRS (thought and language disorder), but no significant correlations were found.

Imaging results

Within-group activation maps

During the executive condition (voluntary switching) compared to the control condition (automatic switching), healthy subjects showed activation in the dorsolateral and inferior frontal cortex, the ACC extending to the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) and SMA, the frontal pole, and the inferior parietal cortex, all bilateral but more predominantly in the right hemisphere. Left-lateralized activity was observed in the pre/post-central gyrus. Significant signal increase was also observed in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and precuneus, bilateral middle temporal cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus and cerebellum (Fig. 2a and Table 2). Manic patients did not show any activation increase in the executive condition, relative to control blocks. However, they did show activation in similar regions during the executive condition relative to baseline (see online Supplementary Figure 2).

Fig. 2. Brain activation maps for the executive > control contrast for the control group (a). The mania group did not show any significant activation in this contrast. (b) Regions of significant differences between groups in this contrast, indicating that while controls increased fronto-parietal and cingulo-opercular activity in response to increased executive demands, patients failed to do so. Images are displayed in neurological convention (right is right). Color bars depict z values.

Table 2. Regions of significant activation increase in the executive > control contrast

ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; DLPFC, Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; SMA, Supplementary Motor Area.

Between-group comparison

The manic patients showed significantly less activation than the controls in the frontal cortex, including the bilateral DLPFC, inferior frontal cortex, ACC and SMA, and anterior frontal cortex, in the executive > control contrast. Significant differences were also observed in the bilateral inferior parietal cortex and PCC/precuneus (Fig. 2b and Table 2). No regions showed greater activation in the mania group relative to the control group.

To further characterize this hypoactivation in terms of its association with executive impairment, we extracted the beta values (parameter estimates) from the clusters of significant differences for each subject (eight regions: a cluster comprising the ACC and right DLPFC, the left DLPFC, right and left inferior frontal cortex, right and left inferior parietal cortex, the left orbitofrontal cortex and the PCC), and examined correlations between these and the deviation from optimal playing time as an index of behavioral performance (given that variables were not normally distributed, we used Spearman's rho). Results are shown in Table 3 and illustrated in online Supplementary Figure 3. Activity in these regions was associated with task performance in the whole sample (negative correlations indicate that increased activity is associated to reduced deviation times), although the correlations seem to be mainly driven by the mania group and were not significant in the control group. Clinical severity of manic symptoms, measured with the YMRS, was not associated with brain activity.

Table 3. Spearman correlations between activation increase in the executive > control contrast and behavioral performance in the whole sample and in healthy subjects and mania patients separately

R, Right; L, Left; B, Both.

Since the behavioral performance indices showed two outliers in deviation from optimal playing time in the control group (their performance was more than 2 s.d.s distal from the group mean), we repeated the analyses excluding these two subjects. Results remained essentially identical when these two subjects were excluded.

Discussion

The CMET task showed that mania is characterized by hypo-activation in well-known executive networks, including the fronto-parietal and cingulo-opercular regions that have been previously associated with task control and goal management (Berkman, Falk, & Lieberman, Reference Berkman, Falk and Lieberman2012; Dosenbach et al., Reference Dosenbach, Visscher, Palmer, Miezin, Wenger, Kang and Petersen2006; Lopez-Garcia et al., Reference Lopez-Garcia, Lesh, Salo, Barch, MacDonald, Gold and Carter2016), accompanied by impairment in behavioral performance. Although manic patients displayed some activation of the executive networks, they failed to increase the activity of these regions in response to the increases in the executive demands of the task.

Reduced prefrontal activation has been a regular finding in inhibition and working memory fMRI studies in mania. The meta-analysis of Hajek, Alda, Hajek, and Ivanoff (Reference Hajek, Alda, Hajek and Ivanoff2013) included 10 studies of response inhibition (147 bipolar patients in manic state and 151 healthy subjects) and reported, similar to our results, diminished activity within the frontal lobes including the right inferior frontal cortex, and in the left medial frontal cortex extending into the ACC. Both the inferior frontal cortex and the ACC are central nodes of the cingulo-opercular network, that also includes portions of the anterior prefrontal cortex (frontal poles) and is involved in task control and the maintenance of task sets (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Dosenbach, Cohen, Wheeler, Schlaggar and Petersen2010). The right inferior frontal cortex has been strongly associated with response inhibition (Aron, Robbins, & Poldrack, Reference Aron, Robbins and Poldrack2004, Reference Aron, Robbins and Poldrack2014), although its role seems to extend to other executive tasks (Chatham et al., Reference Chatham, Claus, Kim, Curran, Banich and Munakata2012; Erika-Florence, Leech, & Hampshire, Reference Erika-Florence, Leech and Hampshire2014; Swick & Chatham, Reference Swick and Chatham2014) and current views ascribe it to a more general task-control function (Menon & Uddin, Reference Menon and Uddin2010). In this aspect our results are consistent with previous reports of reduced inferior frontal and ACC activation in manic patients during executive control (Altshuler et al., Reference Altshuler, Bookheimer, Townsend, Proenza, Eisenberger, Sabb and Cohen2005; Blumberg et al., Reference Blumberg, Leung, Skudlarski, Lacadie, Fredericks, Harris and Peterson2003; Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Ogilvie, Rubinsztein, Calderon, Dolan and Sahakian2004), even in first episodes (Strakowski et al., Reference Strakowski, Adler, Cerullo, Eliassen, Lamy, Fleck and Delbello2008). Moreover, hypo-activation in the mania group was also evident in fronto-parietal regions (DLPFC and inferior parietal cortex), also involved in cognitive control (Lopez-Garcia et al., Reference Lopez-Garcia, Lesh, Salo, Barch, MacDonald, Gold and Carter2016) and with a proposed role in the dynamic adjustments of behavior in response to environmental cues (Dosenbach et al., Reference Dosenbach, Visscher, Palmer, Miezin, Wenger, Kang and Petersen2006).

The coordinated activity of fronto-parietal and cingulo-opercular networks is proposed to regulate the implementation of top-down control (Dosenbach, Fair, Cohen, Schlaggar, & Petersen, Reference Dosenbach, Fair, Cohen, Schlaggar and Petersen2008), which is also supported by our current results that link regions from these two networks to goal management in healthy individuals (see also Fuentes-Claramonte et al., Reference Fuentes-Claramonte, Santo-Angles, Argila-Plaza, Lechon, Guardiola-Ripoll, Almodovar-Payá and Salvador2021). Moreover, the DLPFC and the ACC have also been involved in voluntary task selection in task-switching contexts in healthy individuals (Orr & Banich, Reference Orr and Banich2014; Wisniewski, Reverberi, Tusche, & Haynes, Reference Wisniewski, Reverberi, Tusche and Haynes2015). Following on this, the brain functional alterations that we have observed in manic patients have been found in the context of impaired performance in the CMET task. Moreover, the regions showing reduced activation in the manic patients were linked to task performance. Thus, the findings of hypoactivation of executive control areas during a goal management task provide a plausible mechanism for poor regulation in daily life goal-directed behavior in manic patients.

An open question remains as to whether the reported alterations in task performance and brain activity during executive function are specific to manic episodes or a trait marker of bipolar disorder. Although some studies have found fronto-parietal hypoactivation across all three phases of bipolar disorder (see Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Bookheimer, Foland-Ross, Sugar and Altshuler2010), others have found reduced fronto-parietal activity during mania and depression, but not in euthymia (Pomarol-Clotet et al., Reference Pomarol-Clotet, Alonso-Lana, Moro, Sarró, Bonnin, Goikolea and Salvador2015). Consistent with the latter, Alonso-Lana et al. (Reference Alonso-Lana, Moro, McKenna, Sarró, Romaguera, Monté and Pomarol-Clotet2019) recently showed that DLPFC and parietal hypoactivation during a working memory task normalized after recovery from mania in a longitudinal study, and inferior frontal gyrus function during response inhibition has also shown improvement after treatment with lamotrigine (Pavuluri et al., Reference Pavuluri, Passarotti, Harral and Sweeney2010). Neuropsychological studies have also shown severe executive impairment in manic patients that seemed to recover after remission (Volkert et al., Reference Volkert, Schiele, Kazmaier, Glaser, Zierhut, Kopf and Reif2015). An important next step of this research will be to reassess the patients after remission of the manic episode, which will help to understand if the observed behavioral and brain functional alterations are state- or trait-related. Furthermore, examination of drug-free patients would also be desirable, since all participants in our study were under pharmacological treatment, a common limitation of many studies of affective disorders.

We might also consider hypoactivation in task-control regions to be characteristic not only of manic episodes but, more generally, of mood episodes or other conditions characterized by executive impairment, since it has also been found in schizophrenia (Alústiza, Radua, Pla, Martin, & Ortuño, Reference Alústiza, Radua, Pla, Martin and Ortuño2017; Landin-Romero et al., Reference Landin-Romero, McKenna, Salgado-Pineda, Sarró, Aguirre, Sarri and Pomarol-Clotet2015; Minzenberg, Laird, Thelen, Carter, & Glahn, Reference Minzenberg, Laird, Thelen, Carter and Glahn2009; Poppe et al., Reference Poppe, Barch, Carter, Gold, Ragland, Silverstein and MacDonald2016), bipolar depression (Penfold, Vizueta, Townsend, Bookheimer, & Altshuler, Reference Penfold, Vizueta, Townsend, Bookheimer and Altshuler2015; Pomarol-Clotet et al., Reference Pomarol-Clotet, Alonso-Lana, Moro, Sarró, Bonnin, Goikolea and Salvador2015), and to a lesser extent also unipolar depression (Kikuchi et al., Reference Kikuchi, Miller, Schneck, Oquendo, John Mann, Parsey and Keilp2012; Miller, Hamilton, Sacchet, & Gotlib, Reference Miller, Hamilton, Sacchet and Gotlib2015). Moreover, even within bipolar patients in the euthymic phase, those with executive impairment (as measured by neuropsychological tests) have shown hypoactivation in the lateral prefrontal cortex during a working memory task, compared with cognitively preserved patients with similar clinical status (Alonso-Lana et al., Reference Alonso-Lana, Goikolea, Bonnin, Sarro, Segura, Amann and McKenna2016). This is also supported by the fact that, although patients with greater clinical severity tended to show greater executive impairment at the behavioral level, no association with symptom severity was apparent with brain activity in our data. Although this should be taken with caution given that these correlations were run in a relatively small sample, it is not implausible to consider that the observed hypoactivation reflects a disturbance that is shared across different disorders and that may ultimately be associated with impaired regulation of thought and behavior in mania and other conditions (McTeague, Goodkind, & Etkin, Reference McTeague, Goodkind and Etkin2016).

Conclusions

To our knowledge, this is the first study to address goal management in bipolar disorder during a manic episode. Our results replicate, using a novel and more ecologically valid experimental paradigm, previous findings of hypoactivation in task-control brain areas during executive function. Moreover, we can link this hypoactivation to impaired behavioral performance during the executive task, thus providing a possible mechanism that may (probably in conjunction with others) disturb the ability of the individual to regulate their own behavior.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721002452

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the CIBERSAM and the Catalonian Government, Generalitat de Catalunya (2017-SGR-1271 to EP-C from AGAUR and PERIS SLT006/17/232 to MM and SLT006/17/249 to EP-C from Departament de Salut). Also by a grant from the Plan Nacional de I + D + i 2013–2016: Juan de la Cierva-formación contract (FJCI-2015-25278 to PF-C). And by several grants funded by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III, co-funded by European Union (ERDF/ESF, ‘Investing in your future’): Miguel Servet Research contracts (CPII13/00018 to RS, MS10/00596 to EP-C and to CP14/00041 to JR), Sara Borrell Research contract (CD19/00149 to PF-C), and Research Project Grants (PI14/00810 and PI18/00810 to EP-C, PI14/01151 to RS, PI14/700292 to JR and PI1801535 to MM).

Financial support

None of the authors declare any financial disclosures related to this study.

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

*

Joint first and corresponding authors.

Joint last authors.

References

Alonso-Lana, S., Goikolea, J. M., Bonnin, C. M., Sarro, S., Segura, B., Amann, B. L., … McKenna, P. J. (2016). Structural and functional brain correlates of cognitive impairment in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. PLoS ONE, 11(7), 117. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alonso-Lana, S., Moro, N., McKenna, P. J., Sarró, S., Romaguera, A., Monté, G. C., … Pomarol-Clotet, E. (2019). Longitudinal brain functional changes between mania and euthymia in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 21(5), 449457. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altshuler, L. L., Bookheimer, S. Y., Townsend, J., Proenza, M. A., Eisenberger, N., Sabb, F., … Cohen, M. S. (2005). Blunted activation in orbitofrontal cortex during mania: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 58(10), 763769. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alústiza, I., Radua, J., Pla, M., Martin, R., & Ortuño, F. (2017). Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of timing and cognitive control in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: Evidence of a primary time deficit. Schizophrenia Research, 188, 2132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.01.039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, I. M., Haddad, P. M., & Scott, J. (2012). Bipolar disorder. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.), 345(December), 110. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.e8508.Google ScholarPubMed
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 170177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 177185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckmann, C. F., Jenkinson, M., & Smith, S. M. (2003). General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in fMRI. NeuroImage, 20(2), 10521063. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00435-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkman, E. T., Falk, E. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Interactive effects of three core goal pursuit processes on brain control systems: Goal maintenance, performance monitoring, and response inhibition. PLoS ONE, 7(6), e40334. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blumberg, H. P., Leung, H.-C., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C. M., Fredericks, C. A., Harris, B. C., … Peterson, B. S. (2003). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of bipolar disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(6), 601. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.6.601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E. (2018). Neurocognitive features in clinical subgroups of bipolar disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 229, 125134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.12.057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourne, C., Aydemir, O., Balanzá-Martínez, V., Bora, E., Brissos, S., Cavanagh, J. T. O., … Goodwin, G. M. (2013). Neuropsychological testing of cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: An individual patient data meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 128(3), 149162. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Forbes, C., Costello, A., Coates, L. M., Dawson, D. R., … Channon, S. (2006). The case for the development and use of ‘ecologically valid’ measures of executive function in experimental and clinical neuropsychology. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 12(2), 194209. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617706060310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cerullo, M. A., Adler, C. M., Lamy, M., Eliassen, J. C., Fleck, D. E., Strakowski, S. M., & Delbello, M. P. (2009). Differential brain activation during response inhibition in bipolar and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorders. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 3(3), 189197. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-7893.2009.00132.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chatham, C. H., Claus, E. D., Kim, A., Curran, T., Banich, M. T., & Munakata, Y. (2012). Cognitive control reflects context monitoring, not motoric stopping, in response inhibition. PLoS ONE, 7, e31546. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0031546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, C. H., Suckling, J., Lennox, B. R., Ooi, C., & Bullmore, E. T. (2011). A quantitative meta-analysis of fMRI studies in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 13(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00893.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cullen, B., Brennan, D., Manly, T., & Evans, J. J. (2016). Towards validation of a new computerised test of goal neglect: Preliminary evidence from clinical and neuroimaging pilot studies. PLoS ONE, 11(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Ser, T., Gonzalez-Montalvo, J., Martinez-Espinosa, S., Delgado-Villapalos, C., & Bermejo, F. (1997). Estimation of premorbid intelligence in Spanish people with the word accentuation test and its application to the diagnosis of dementia. Brain and Cognition, 33, 343356. https://doi.org/10.1086/518948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, T., Becerra, R., & Coombes, J. (2017). Executive functioning deficits among adults with bipolar disorder (types I and II): A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 218, 407427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2008). A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(3), 99105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dosenbach, N. U. F., Visscher, K. M., Palmer, E. D., Miezin, F. M., Wenger, K. K., Kang, H. C., … Petersen, S. E. (2006). A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron, 50(5), 799812. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R., & Freer, C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe: The organization of goal-directed behavior. Cognitive Psychology, 30(3), 257303. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1996.0008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, R., Ogilvie, A., Rubinsztein, J. S., Calderon, G., Dolan, R. J., & Sahakian, B. J. (2004). Abnormal ventral frontal response during performance of an affective go/no go task in patients with mania. Biological Psychiatry, 55(12), 11631170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.03.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endicott, J., Spitzer, R. L., Fleiss, J. L., & Cohen, J. (1976). The global assessment scale: A procedure for measuring overall severity of psychiatric disturbance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33(6), 766–771. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1976.01770060086012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erika-Florence, M., Leech, R., & Hampshire, A. (2014). A functional network perspective on response inhibition and attentional control. Nature Communications, 5, 4073. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuentes-Claramonte, P., Santo-Angles, A., Argila-Plaza, I., Lechon, M., Guardiola-Ripoll, M., Almodovar-Payá, C., … Salvador, R. (2021). Brain imaging of executive function with the Computerised Multiple Elements Test. Brain Imaging and Behavior, Online ahead of print.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomar, J. J., Ortiz-Gil, J., McKenna, P. J., Salvador, R., Sans-Sansa, B., Sarró, S., … Pomarol-Clotet, E. (2011). Validation of the Word Accentuation Test (TAP) as a means of estimating premorbid IQ in Spanish speakers. Schizophrenia Research, 128(1–3), 175176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. J., Cahill, C. M., & Malhi, G. S. (2007). The cognitive and neurophysiological basis of emotion dysregulation in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 103(1–3), 2942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2007.01.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guy, W. (1976). Clinical global impression. ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology, revised. Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health.Google Scholar
Hajek, T., Alda, M., Hajek, E., & Ivanoff, J. (2013). Functional neuroanatomy of response inhibition in bipolar disorders – Combined voxel based and cognitive performance meta-analysis. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(12), 19551966. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.08.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 23, 5662. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hummer, T. A., Hulvershorn, L. A., Karne, H. S., Gunn, A. D., Wang, Y., & Anand, A. (2013). Emotional response inhibition in bipolar disorder: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of trait- and state-related abnormalities. Biological Psychiatry, 73(2), 136143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.06.036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, S. R., Flszbein, A., & Opfer, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13(2), 261276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kikuchi, T., Miller, J. M., Schneck, N., Oquendo, M. A., John Mann, J., Parsey, R. V., & Keilp, J. G. (2012). Neural responses to incongruency in a blocked-trial Stroop fMRI task in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 143(1–3), 241247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kupferschmidt, D. A., & Zakzanis, K. K. (2011). Toward a functional neuroanatomical signature of bipolar disorder: Quantitative evidence from the neuroimaging literature. Psychiatry Research – Neuroimaging, 193(2), 7179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.02.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landin-Romero, R., McKenna, P. J., Salgado-Pineda, P., Sarró, S., Aguirre, C., Sarri, C., … Pomarol-Clotet, E. (2015). Failure of deactivation in the default mode network: A trait marker for schizophrenia? Psychological Medicine, 45(06), 13151325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez-Garcia, P., Lesh, T. A., Salo, T., Barch, D. M., MacDonald, A. W., Gold, J. M., … Carter, C. S. (2016). The neural circuitry supporting goal maintenance during cognitive control: A comparison of expectancy AX-CPT and dot probe expectancy paradigms. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(1), 164175. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0384-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzola-Pomietto, P., Kaladjian, A., Azorin, J. M., Anton, J. L., & Jeanningros, R. (2009). Bilateral decrease in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation during motor response inhibition in mania. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(4), 432441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McTeague, L. M., Goodkind, M. S., & Etkin, A. (2016). Transdiagnostic impairment of cognitive control in mental illness. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 83, 3746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function, 214, 655667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, C. H., Hamilton, J. P., Sacchet, M. D., & Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging of major depressive disorder in youth. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(10), 10451053. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minzenberg, M. J., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S., Carter, C. S., & Glahn, D. C. (2009). Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(8), 811822. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, F. C., Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Rogers, R. D., Robbins, T. W., Paykel, E. S., & Sahakian, B. J. (2001). Decision-making cognition in mania and depression. Psychological Medicine, 31(4), 679693. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, S. M., Dosenbach, N. U. F., Cohen, A. L., Wheeler, M. E., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2010). Role of the anterior insula in task-level control and focal attention. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5-6), 669680. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0260-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orr, J. M., & Banich, M. T. (2014). The neural mechanisms underlying internally and externally guided task selection. NeuroImage, 84, 191205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavuluri, M. N., Passarotti, A. M., Harral, E. M., & Sweeney, J. A. (2010). Enhanced prefrontal function with pharmacotherapy on a response inhibition task in adolescent bipolar disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 71(11), 15261534. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.09m05504yel.Enhanced.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penfold, C., Vizueta, N., Townsend, J. D., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Altshuler, L. L. (2015). Frontal lobe hypoactivation in medication-free adults with bipolar II depression during response inhibition. Psychiatry Research – Neuroimaging, 231(3), 202209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.11.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomarol-Clotet, E., Alonso-Lana, S., Moro, N., Sarró, S., Bonnin, M. C., Goikolea, J. M., … Salvador, R. (2015). Brain functional changes across the different phases of bipolar disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 206(2), 136144. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.114.152033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomarol-Clotet, E., Moro, N., Sarró, S., Goikolea, J. M., Vieta, E., Amann, B., … Salvador, R. (2012). Failure of de-activation in the medial frontal cortex in mania: Evidence for default mode network dysfunction in the disorder. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 13(8), 616626. https://doi.org/10.3109/15622975.2011.573808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poppe, A. B., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., Gold, J. M., Ragland, J. D., Silverstein, S. M., & MacDonald, A. W. (2016). Reduced frontoparietal activity in schizophrenia is linked to a specific deficit in goal maintenance: A multisite functional imaging study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(5), 11491157. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbw036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, L. J., & Ferrier, I. N. (2006). Evolution of cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: A systematic review of cross-sectional evidence. Bipolar Disorders, 8(2), 103116. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00277.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application. Brain, 114, 727741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., … Matthews, P. M. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage, 23(Suppl. 1), 208219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strakowski, S. M., Adler, C. M., Almeida, J., Altshuler, L. L., Blumberg, H. P., Chang, K. D., … Townsend, J. D. (2012). The functional neuroanatomy of bipolar disorder: A consensus model. Bipolar Disorders, 14(4), 313325. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01022.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strakowski, S. M., Adler, C. M., Cerullo, M. A., Eliassen, J. C., Lamy, M., Fleck, D. E., … Delbello, M. P. (2008). Magnetic resonance imaging brain activation in first-episode bipolar mania during a response inhibition task. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 2(4), 225233. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-7893.2008.00082.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swick, D., & Chatham, C. H. (2014). Ten years of inhibition revisited. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 329. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, J., Bookheimer, S. Y., Foland-Ross, L. C., Sugar, C. A., & Altshuler, L. L. (2010). fMRI abnormalities in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a working memory task in manic, euthymic and depressed bipolar subjects. Psychiatry Research – Neuroimaging, 182(1), 2229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.11.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkert, J., Schiele, M., Kazmaier, J., Glaser, F., Zierhut, K., Kopf, J., … Reif, A. (2015). Cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder: From acute episode to remission. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 266, 225237. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-015-0657-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioral assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome. Oxford: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Wingo, A. P., Harvey, P. D., & Baldessarini, R. J. (2009). Neurocognitive impairment in bipolar disorder patients: Functional implications. Bipolar Disorders, 11(2), 113125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00665.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wisniewski, D., Reverberi, C., Tusche, A., & Haynes, J. D. (2015). The neural representation of voluntary task-set selection in dynamic environments. Cerebral Cortex, 25(12), 47154726. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, R. C., Biggs, J. T., Ziegler, V. E., & Meyer, D. A. (1978). A rating scale for mania: Reliability, validity and sensitivity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 133(11), 429435. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.133.5.429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. CMET task. Participants sequentially played four games requiring left or right index button presses during each 48s block. In the automatic switching condition, the game changed every 12s without intervention of the participant. In the voluntary switching condition, the participant had to actively switch games by button press (right thumb), with the instruction to spend approximately the same amount of time playing each game. No time information was shown during either condition. A fixation cross was shown during baseline periods between blocks (12s).

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic and clinical data for the sample included in the analysis

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Brain activation maps for the executive > control contrast for the control group (a). The mania group did not show any significant activation in this contrast. (b) Regions of significant differences between groups in this contrast, indicating that while controls increased fronto-parietal and cingulo-opercular activity in response to increased executive demands, patients failed to do so. Images are displayed in neurological convention (right is right). Color bars depict z values.

Figure 3

Table 2. Regions of significant activation increase in the executive > control contrast

Figure 4

Table 3. Spearman correlations between activation increase in the executive > control contrast and behavioral performance in the whole sample and in healthy subjects and mania patients separately

Supplementary material: File

Madre et al. supplementary material

Madre et al. supplementary material

Download Madre et al. supplementary material(File)
File 5.6 MB