Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4rdrl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-19T23:28:45.011Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

An Examination of Differences in Moral Disengagement and Empathy Among Bullying Participant Groups

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 August 2017

Aaron D. Haddock*
Affiliation:
Department of Counseling, Clinical, and School Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, California, USA
Shane R. Jimerson
Affiliation:
Department of Counseling, Clinical, and School Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, California, USA
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE: Aaron D. Haddock, Department of Counseling, Clinical, and School Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106–9490, USA. Email: aaronhaddock@gmail.com

Abstract

This study examines how different roles in school bullying (e.g., bullies, victims, defenders) vary in cognitive and affective empathy and moral disengagement. Findings from this study revealed that levels of empathy and moral disengagement differed significantly among bullying groups for 702 6th, 7th, and 8th grade students in the United States. An analysis of variance showed differential patterns between bullying groups and outcome variables (i.e., cognitive and affective empathy and moral disengagement). In addition, the correlation between moral disengagement and empathy was statistically significant and negative. Affective empathy and cognitive empathy both significantly predicted moral disengagement; with every one unit increase in moral disengagement, affective empathy decreased by .38 and cognitive empathy decreased by .39. Students who scored higher in moral disengagement tended to score lower in empathy. The current findings confirm and extend the literature on the relation between moral disengagement, empathy, prosociality, and victimising behaviour. This information can inform school-wide and targeted intervention efforts.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2017 

Long before the advent of experimental science, wisdom traditions around the world cautioned against instrumental aggression due to its deleterious effects for the victim, the aggressor, and the social order. Recognising blatantly aggressive and exploitative behaviour as critical impediments to the systems of reciprocity necessary for communal living, a key function of society and the state has been to institute and enforce social norms and laws aimed at curbing violence and promoting self-restraint (Elias & Jephcott, Reference Elias and Jephcott1982; Pinker, Reference Pinker2011). Indeed, the regulation of such behaviour resides at the core of social contracts and constitutional law. Harmful, aggressive, and violent behaviour have been topics of interest for the human sciences since their inception, and investigating the psychosocial factors at play has motivated some of the field's most well-known studies (e.g., Haney, Banks, & Zimbardo, Reference Haney, Banks and Zimbardo1973; Milgram, Reference Milgram1963; Sherif, Reference Sherif1958; Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood, & Sherif, Reference Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood and Sherif1961). In recent decades, the persistent prevalence of school bullying has prompted modern social science to focus on these issues (e.g., Dodge, Coie, & Lynam, Reference Dodge, Coie, Lynam, Eisenberg, William and Lerner2006) in an effort to stem this pernicious problem and create safer, more supportive schools (Bryn, Reference Bryn2011; Osher, Dwyer, Jimerson, & Brown, Reference Osher, Dwyer, Jimerson, Brown, Jimerson, Nickerson, Mayer and Furlong2012).

Bullying is a subcategory of interpersonal aggression traditionally defined as ‘unwanted, intentional, aggressive behavior that involves a real or perceived power imbalance that is often repeated over time’ (American Educational Research Association [AERA], 2013, p. 5; Olweus Reference Olweus1993, Reference Olweus1994, Reference Olweus, Jimerson, Swearer and Espelage2010; Smith & Morita, Reference Smith, Morita, Smith, Morita, JungerTas, Olweus, Catalano and Slee1999; Vaillancourt, Hymel, & McDougall, Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, McDougall, Hymel, Krygsman, Miller, Stiver and Davis2008). Bullying takes a variety of forms, including physical aggression, verbal aggression, and social or relational aggression, such as exclusion and humiliation, with the ‘systematic use and abuse of power’ distinguishing bullying from other forms of aggressive behaviour (AERA, 2013, p. 5; Espelage, Reference Espelage2012; Hymel & Swearer, Reference Hymel and Swearer2015; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, McDougall, Hymel, Krygsman, Miller, Stiver and Davis2008). As such, bullying is best understood as a component of the broader phenomenon of violence in schools and communities (AERA, 2013). Bullying is a widespread problem that negatively affects youth around the globe, with some research indicating the prevalence of bullying behaviour peaking in the middle school years (Neiman, Devoe, & Chandler, Reference Neiman, DeVoe and Chandler2009; Olweus, Reference Olweus1991, Reference Olweus, Jimerson, Swearer and Espelage2010). The aim of this study was to examine and further clarify the relation between differences in levels of empathy (cognitive and affective) and levels of moral disengagement, and different roles in school bullying (e.g., bullies, defenders, victims), as well as to examine the relation between levels of empathy and levels of moral disengagement.

Theoretical Perspective

In recent years, the social-ecological conceptualisation of bullying as occurring within the broader social context has gained purchase in the fields of psychology and education (Espelage & Swearer, Reference Espelage and Swearer2004, Reference Espelage, Swearer, Jimerson, Swearer and Espelage2010). It is a theoretical perspective that understands bullying behaviour as influenced by both individual characteristics and the multilayered, proximal, and distal systems of families, schools, key caregivers, peer groups, neighbourhoods, cultural expectations, society, and the reciprocal interactions among and across these systems (Benbenishty & Astor, Reference Benbenishty and Astor2005; Bronfenbrenner, Reference Bronfenbrenner1979; Espelage & Swearer, Reference Espelage, Swearer, Jimerson, Swearer and Espelage2010). Individuals’ participation in bullying situations can include victimising others, being victimised, and cooperating (i.e., defending the victim, staying uninvolved, or helping the bully; Rigby & Slee, Reference Rigby and Slee1993). In response to research examining bullying as a group phenomenon, Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman, and Kaukiainen (Reference Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman and Kaukiainen1996) developed the participant role approach, which organises individuals involved in the bullying process into six distinct groups: victim, bully, reinforcer, assistant, defender, and outsider.

Research has suggested a variety of individual and group-level risk factors related to bullying participation (Barboza et al., Reference Barboza, Schiamberg, Oehmke, Korzeniewski, Post and Heraux2009). Risk for involvement in bullying is associated with prior victimisation, poor school climate, parents and teachers with low expectations for academic achievement, and a lack of academic and emotional support from parents, teachers, and peers (Barboza et al., Reference Barboza, Schiamberg, Oehmke, Korzeniewski, Post and Heraux2009). While some studies indicate that bullies have poor social problem-solving skills (Slee, Reference Slee1993; Warden & Mackinnon, Reference Warden and Mackinnon2003), other studies indicate that bullies are perceived as having high social intelligence (Kaukiainen et al., Reference Kaukiainen, Bjorkqvist, Lagerspetz, Osterman, Salmivalli, Rothberg and Ahlbom1999) and as being popular (Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Hymel and McDougall2003). Bullying behaviour is consistently associated with anger, aggression, and externalising problems (Olweus, Reference Olweus1993). As Gini, Pozzoli, and Hymel (Reference Gini, Pozzoli and Hymel2014) explain, youth with a positive attitude toward aggressive behaviour (Carney & Merrell, Reference Carney and Merrell2001; Rigby & Slee, Reference Rigby and Slee1993), an enhanced sense of self-efficacy in employing aggressive behaviours (Andreou & Metallidou, Reference Andreou and Metallidou2004), and underdeveloped feelings of empathy for others (Gini, Albiero, Benelli, & Altoè, Reference Gini, Albiero, Benelli and Altoe2007; Jolliffe & Farrington, Reference Jolliffe and Farrington2011) are particularly predisposed to utilising aggressive behaviour instrumentally to achieve their goals. Rather than being reactive aggressors with social skills deficits (Crick & Dodge, Reference Crick and Dodge1994), some children who frequently bully appear to possess a well-developed understanding of social cues, moral norms, and the factors that guide moral judgments and adherence to moral norms (e.g., beliefs, values, impact on others; Dolan & Fullam, Reference Dolan and Fullam2010; Gini, Reference Gini2006; Gini, Pozzoli, & Hauser, Reference Gini, Pozzoli and Hauser2011; Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham, Reference Sutton, Smith and Swettenham1999). However, these social skills and moral reasoning abilities appear to be possessed in conjunction with low levels of empathy for victims’ suffering (Gini et al., Reference Gini, Pozzoli and Hauser2011) and a lack of motivation to act on that knowledge (Arsenio & Lemerise, Reference Arsenio and Lemerise2001). From this perspective, bullies are conceptualised as aggressive children who value instrumental goals more highly than relational goals and utilise their social knowledge and skills in conjunction with aggressive behaviour for personal gain (Arsenio & Lemerise, Reference Arsenio and Lemerise2001; Polman et al., Reference Polman, de Castro, Koops, van Boxtel and Merk2007). It is believed that such traits may facilitate bullying behaviour due to the lack of salience of others’ distress cues and a concomitant lack of empathy and guilt (Beauchaine & Hinshaw, Reference Beauchaine and Hinshaw2016; Frick, Reference Frick2006).

Empathy, like other moral emotions (e.g., shame and guilt), has been seen as a mediator of moral standards and behaviour (Tangney, Stuewig, & Mashek, Reference Tangney, Stuewig and Mashek2007). Moral emotions appear to enable individuals to anticipate how moral transgressions will lead to deleterious outcomes, which assists in altering behaviour for a more positive result (Malti, Gasser, & Buchmann, Reference Malti, Gasser and Buchmann2009). Empathy in particular has been shown to be a factor that can inhibit antisocial behaviour (Cohen & Strayer, Reference Cohen and Strayer1996; Hoffman, Reference Hoffman2000; Miller & Eisenberg, Reference Miller and Eisenberg1988). The construct of empathy has been defined in a variety of ways, but an inclusive definition conceptualises the construct as ‘the ability to understand and share in another's emotional state or context’ (Cohen & Strayer, Reference Cohen and Strayer1996, p. 988). Defined in this way, empathy is regarded as both a cognitive process and an affective capacity. The cognitive dimension connotes the ability to understand another's emotional state or perspective, whereas the affective dimension of empathy is characterised by the ability to share another's emotional state or experience feelings of concern or sympathy toward others (Davis, Reference Davis1994). At present, this multidimensional model of empathy is generally employed (e.g., Davis, Reference Davis1994; Hoffman, Reference Hoffman, Bohart and Stipek2001) with the understanding that while the cognitive and affective components of empathy can be studied in isolation, an accurate and comprehensive consideration of the construct integrates both dimensions (Hoffman, Reference Hoffman, Bohart and Stipek2001).

The relation between bullying behaviour and empathy in childhood and adolescence is often asserted (Bernstein & Watson, Reference Bernstein and Watson1997; Merrell, Gueldner, Ross, & Isava, Reference Merrell, Gueldner, Ross and Isava2008; Miller & Eisenberg, Reference Miller and Eisenberg1988; Olweus, Reference Olweus1993). Findings from a study by Jolliffe and Farrington (Reference Jolliffe and Farrington2006) indicated that low affective empathy was significantly related to frequently engaging in bullying for both male and female students and occasional bullying among females, but not males; however, students’ levels of cognitive empathy demonstrated no significant association with bullying of any kind. Male students reporting low levels of overall empathy were more likely to engage in violent bullying, whereas female students reporting low levels of overall empathy were more likely to engage in indirect bullying. In another study, Jolliffe and Farrington (Reference Jolliffe and Farrington2011) found that, independent of a variety of individual social and background variables (e.g., socioeconomic status, parental supervision, family status), low affective empathy, but not cognitive empathy, was related to bullying by males. Similarly, Endresen and Olweus (Reference Endresen, Olweus, Bohart and Stipek2001) found a significant negative correlation between bullying behaviour and affective empathy for both males and females. A study by Espelage, Mebane, and Adams (Reference Espelage, Mebane, Adams, Espelage and Swearer2004) found significant negative correlations between bullying and affective empathy and cognitive empathy for young adolescent males and bullying and cognitive empathy for young adolescent females. Results from a 2009 study by Caravita, Di Blasio, and Salmivalli suggested that high levels of affective empathy inhibited bullying only among adolescent males; moreover, both males and females with high levels of cognitive empathy were less likely to bully. Gini et al. (Reference Gini, Albiero, Benelli and Altoe2007) found that, among a sample of adolescents, low empathy was associated with bullying, whereas high empathy was positively associated with coming to the aid of bullied students.

It is important to note, however, that the relationship between low empathy and bullying has been called into question. A few studies that examined the relationship while controlling for other factors, such as socioeconomic status and intelligence (Endresen & Olweus, Reference Endresen, Olweus, Bohart and Stipek2001; Warden & Mackinnon, Reference Warden and Mackinnon2003), did not find that low empathy was independently related to bullying. Similarly, a systematic review and meta-analysis by Jolliffe and Farrington (Reference Jolliffe and Farrington2004) confirmed a positive relation between criminal offending and low levels of empathy, but found that controlling for socioeconomic status and intelligence caused the relationship to vanish. In response, the authors argued that further research employing better measures of empathy is needed.

There is evidence that defending behaviour is also related to levels of empathy. Warden and Mackinnon (Reference Warden and Mackinnon2003) found that, in a sample of 9- and 10-year-olds, prosocial children had significantly higher levels of affective empathy compared to bullies. They further found that this association was moderated by gender: females had higher levels of empathy and were less likely to bully, whereas males had lower levels of empathy and were more likely to bully. There also appears to be a developmental component to the relation between prosocial helping behaviour and empathy, with research findings suggesting that as children's prosocial competencies develop, their ability to interpret their own reactions to social situations improves and associations between indices of empathy and helping behaviour are strengthened (Eisenberg & Miller, Reference Eisenberg and Miller1987). Gini et al. (Reference Gini, Albiero, Benelli and Altoe2007) found that empathy was positively associated with actively helping victimised students; however, in a follow-up study, Gini, Albiero, Benelli, and Altoè (Reference Gini, Albiero, Benelli and Altoè2008) concluded that empathy alone was not powerful enough to predict defending behaviour, despite the fact that they again found that high levels of empathic responsiveness increased the likelihood of prosocial support in bullying situations. Like the results discussed above that found bullying was related to low levels of affective empathy but not cognitive empathy, similar results were found for defending behaviour. Pöyhönen, Juvonen, and Salmivalli (Reference Pöyhönen, Juvonen and Salmivalli2010) found a positive association between affective empathy and defending behaviour, but the same was not true for the relationship between cognitive empathy and defending behaviour.

Negative relations between empathy and bullying behaviour are documented in the literature. Stavrinides, Georgious, and Theofanous (Reference Stavrinides, Georgiou and Theofanous2010) found a negative relation between affective empathy and bullying though, like Pöyhönen et al. (Reference Pöyhönen, Juvonen and Salmivalli2010), no significant impact of cognitive empathy and bullying. In a similar study, Gini et al. (Reference Gini, Pozzoli and Hauser2011) investigated differences between bullying participant roles (i.e., bully, victim, defender) on levels of knowledge of right and wrong (‘moral competence’) and levels of emotional awareness and sensitivity concerning moral transgressions (‘moral compassion’). The study found that both bullies and defenders demonstrated advanced moral competence, but that bullies exhibited less moral compassion compared to victims and defenders. Consequently, the authors concluded that bullies appear to understand social norms and moral expectations, but lack the moral compassion or empathy that would curb their aggressive urges. These findings are consistent with research indicating that empathy orients individuals toward others’ needs and feelings, which in turn facilitates higher level moral reasoning and prosocial behaviour (e.g., Eisenberg, Reference Eisenberg1986; Hoffman, Reference Hoffman2000).

Albert Bandura's theory of moral disengagement may help explain the connection between empathy and bullying behaviour. Moral disengagement seeks to articulate the psychological mechanisms at play when individuals, whether acting alone or in groups, commit inhumane acts, with emphasis on the process by which affective self-regulatory mechanisms mediate the link between moral reasoning and behaviour (Bandura, Reference Bandura1990, Reference Bandura, Kurtines and Gewirtz1991, Reference Bandura1999, Reference Bandura2002). According to the theory of moral disengagement, as individuals are socialised, they construct and internalise an understanding of the moral standards of their community, which subsequently serves to guide their behaviour (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996). Once these moral norms are internalised, most people regulate their actions in accordance with the internalised standards because doing so is satisfying, enhances one's sense of self-worth, and enables one to avoid ‘self-censure’ (Bandura et al., Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996, p. 364). Utilising affective self-regulatory processes (e.g., empathy), such mechanisms or self-sanctions motivate and enable the cognitive regulation of behaviour that conforms to established moral norms.

However, these self-regulatory functions only have an impact on actual behaviour when they are activated and, according to the theory of moral disengagement, a variety of psychosocial processes exist that effectively disengage self-sanctions from inhumane conduct, in effect freeing the individual from self-censure and potential guilt (Bandura et al., Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996). As a result, individuals are able to selectively activate and disengage internal control to allow ‘different types of conduct with the same moral standards’ (Bandura et al., Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996, p. 364). In other words, one need not relinquish one's moral standards in order to transgress against them since through the process of moral disengagement, it is possible to maintain one's moral standards while at the same time justifying actions that violate those standards by convincing oneself that the standard does not apply to a particular situation or person.

The theory of moral disengagement posits that this can occur via eight distinct mechanisms clustered within four broad strategies. The four major strategies are: (1) cognitive restructuring of immoral behaviour, (2) obscuration of personal responsibility, (3) misrepresentation of injurious consequences, and (4) blaming the victim. Cognitive restructuring operates by framing the behaviour itself in a positive light through the mechanisms of (1) moral justification, (2) advantageous comparison, or (3) euphemistic labeling. Obscuration of personal responsibility involves the mechanisms (1) displacement of responsibility and (2) diffusion of responsibility. The third broad set of strategies, misrepresentation of injurious consequences, operates via the mechanisms (1) minimising, disregarding, or distorting the consequences of one's action, allowing individuals to distance themselves from the harm caused or to emphasise positive rather than negative outcomes. The fourth disengagement strategy, blaming the victim, employs the mechanisms (1) dehumanisation of the victim and (2) attribution of blame, or framing aggression as provoked by the victim (Gini et al., 2013). In sum, these strategies and mechanisms facilitate inhumane behaviour via a process of moral disengagement that insulates individuals from the negative feelings (e.g., guilt or shame) typically associated with acts that violate common moral norms.

The construct of moral disengagement may be a helpful framework for understanding school violence and victimisation, particularly as it relates to bullying (Hymel, Rocke-Henderson, & Bonanno, Reference Hymel, Rocke-Henderson and Bonanno2005; Hymel, Schonert-Reichl, Bonanno, Vaillancourt, & Rocke Henderson, Reference Hymel, Schonert-Reichl, Bonanno, Vaillancourt, Rocke Henderson, Jimerson, Swearer and Espelage2010). Since the mid-1990s, when Bandura first developed the theory of moral disengagement to understand historical and present-day atrocities, many studies have been conducted applying the theory of moral disengagement to understand bullying and aggressive behaviour among youth. Gini et al. (Reference Gini, Pozzoli and Hymel2014) conducted a meta-analysis of 27 studies examining the association between moral disengagement and aggressive behaviour, which reported data on 17,776 participants aged from 8 to 18. The findings revealed a small-to-medium mean effect size for the association between moral disengagement and aggressive behaviour. Let us now take a closer look at some of the most important findings from these studies.

Obermann (Reference Obermann2011b) examined the relation between moral disengagement and different self-reported and peer-nominated roles in school bullying among a group of 6th- and 7th-grade students in Denmark. The study found that both types of bullying were related to moral disengagement, and that bullies and bully-victims displayed higher moral disengagement than uninvolved students. Relatedly, results of a study by Gini (Reference Gini2006) demonstrated that bullies, reinforcers, and assistants all exhibited a significantly higher tendency to activate moral disengagement mechanisms, whereas defenders displayed higher levels of moral engagement. Unlike victims, bullies showed no difficulty on social cognition tasks. In a follow-up study, Gini et al. (Reference Gini, Pozzoli and Hauser2011) found that both defenders and bullies exhibited advanced moral competence relative to victims and that, according to their measures, victims showed delayed moral competence. Moreover, bullies displayed significant deficits in moral compassion (e.g., empathy) relative to victims and defenders. Similarly, Menesini et al. (Reference Menesini, Sanchez, Fonzi, Ortega, Costabile and Lo Feudo2003) examined the role of moral emotions (e.g., empathy) and reasoning in relation to children's behaviour in bullying situations. Results indicated that bullies, as compared to victims and uninvolved students, showed a higher level of disengaged emotions and motives when asked to put themselves in the role of bully. Closer analyses of the specific mechanisms of moral disengagement indicated that bullies reasoned more egocentrically. Finally, Hymel et al. (Reference Hymel, Rocke-Henderson and Bonanno2005) specifically investigated whether the construct of moral disengagement improves our understanding of bullying among adolescents. Results indicated that bullies were significantly more likely to report positive attitudes and beliefs about bullying, with 38% of the variance in reported bullying accounted for by the students’ endorsement of moral disengagement mechanisms. Thus, there appears to be growing evidence that the theory of moral disengagement may enhance our understanding of the relationship between students’ moral standards, affective self-regulatory mechanisms, and bullying behaviour.

Summary

Given the negative effects of and deleterious outcomes associated with bullying behaviour, continued research aimed at understanding the processes that undergird the development of bullying behaviour and aggressive children's social and emotional cognition (Crick & Dodge, Reference Crick and Dodge1994; Sutton et al., Reference Sutton, Smith and Swettenham1999) is needed to inform the creation of effective interventions. Research exploring the link between affective self-regulatory mechanisms, like empathy, and moral disengagement mechanisms is needed to improve our understanding of the various participant roles assumed in the bullying situation. The present study aims to contribute to this developing literature by examining differences in moral disengagement and empathy among bully participant groups.

Research Questions and Hypotheses

Based upon a review of the relevant literature on the interrelations between moral disengagement, empathy, and bullying, the study addressed the following questions.

Research question 1:

Do males and females demonstrate differences in levels of moral disengagement and empathy?

Hypothesis 1:

Males will demonstrate significantly higher levels of moral disengagement, and females will exhibit significantly higher levels of both cognitive and affective empathy.

Research question 2:

Do participants’ levels of moral disengagement differ significantly by bully participant role?

Hypothesis 2:

Levels of moral disengagement will significantly differ among the bullying participant groups, with mean levels of moral disengagement emerging as significantly higher for bullies than for defenders and a comparison group (i.e., students reporting no involvement in any bullying participant role). In contrast, defenders will exhibit significantly lower levels of moral disengagement in relation to all other groups.

Research question 3:

Do participants’ levels of empathy differ significantly by bully participant role?

Hypothesis 3:

Mean levels of empathy will significantly differ among the bullying participant groups, with (1) mean levels of both cognitive and affective empathy emerging as significantly lower for bullies than for victims, defenders, and a comparison group and (2) mean levels of both cognitive and affective empathy emerging as significantly higher for defenders and victims than for bullies.

Research question 4:

How well are levels of moral disengagement predicted from levels of both affective and cognitive empathy?

Hypothesis 4:

Students scoring higher in moral disengagement will score lower in both cognitive and affective empathy.

Method

Participants

Participants in this study included 702 students across 6th (n = 44), 7th (n = 327), and 8th grade (n = 331) attending school in the United States. The sample was 45.8% male (n = 322) and 54.2% female (n = 380). The ethnicity breakdown was as follows: 37.8% Hispanic (n = 265), 42.3% White (n = 297), 6.6% Asian (n = 46), 2.1% African American (n = 15), 1% Native American (n = 5), and 10.5% multiracial (n = 74). Although socioeconomic status was not directly measured, the sample included students from a wide range of socioeconomic backgrounds. See Table 1.

TABLE 1 Summary of Participants

Procedure

Participants were recruited as part of a school-wide bullying prevention program, P3R Promoting Positive Peer Relationships (Faull, Swearer, Jimerson, Espelage, & Ng, Reference Faull, Swearer, Jimerson, Espelage and Ng2008). Data collection occurred before the program was implemented. With the approval of school administration, researchers employed passive parental consent adhering to state educational policy and in line with the research project's Institutional Review Board. All students in the sample were provided an opportunity to participate anonymously in order to assure them the confidentiality of their responses. Students completed the questionnaire anonymously using online survey software in a single administration. Classroom teachers oversaw survey administration at school during regular school hours. Teachers instructed students to keep their answers private and complete the survey in silence.

Aware of societal expectations for behaviour, youth tend to exaggerate positive behaviours on self-report surveys (Carifio, Reference Carifio1994; Cornell, Klein, Konold, & Huang, Reference Cornell, Klein, Konold and Huang2012; Furlong, Sharkey, Felix, Tanigawa, & Green, 2009). To attend to this issue, the survey included social desirability questions in order to reduce the chance that students provided more socially acceptable responses to questions about bullying than is in fact true for them. Twice, students responded to the statement ‘I am telling the truth on this survey’ on a 4-point scale ranging from totally false to totally true.

The cut-score methodology was employed to assign participant roles to students (Summers & Demaray, Reference Summers and Demaray2009). Groups of students were classified by participant role (bully, victim, bully/victim, defender, victim/defender, outsider, and comparison) in order to conduct comparative analyses. Students scoring in the top 15th percentile of all students on the Bully Participant Role Survey (BPRS) subscales compose the categories. In order to be classified into one of the combined groups (bully/victim or victim/defender), the student had to score in the top 15th percentile on both subscales. The 15th percentile cutoff provides a stringent criterion for group membership that ensures only students who report frequent engagement in these behaviours are included in the analysis. Based on the subscale correlations in the development of the BPRS (Summers & Demaray, Reference Summers and Demaray2009), evidence exists that the outsider group is mutually exclusive from other groups. Consequently, students in overlapping groups that included the outsider group were only placed in the outsider group. Students whose scores were below the top 15th percentile on all participant role subscales were placed in the comparison group. As such, a total of 320 (45.6%) students were in the comparison group, 40 (5.7%) were in the bully group, 34 (4.8%) were in the victim group, 19 (2.7%) were in the bully/victim group, 69 (9.8%) were in the defender group, 33 (4.7%) were in the victim/defender group, and 187 (26.6%) were in the outsider group (see Table 2).

TABLE 2 Bullying Groups Across Gender and Grade

Measures

Measures in this study included established self-report surveys to differentiate bully participant roles and to assess moral disengagement and empathy. Bully participant roles were designated using the BPRS (Summers & Demaray, Reference Summers and Demaray2009); the roles were differentiated with the BPRS because the measure was already in use as a component of the evaluation of the bullying prevention program. The BPRS was developed for use with children in 5th to 8th grades, based on the Participant Role Questionnaire (PRQ) developed by Salmivalli et al. (Reference Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman and Kaukiainen1996) and the Revised Olweus Bully-victim Questionnaire (Olweus, Reference Olweus1996), as well as the corresponding factor structures. The BPRS measures students’ perceptions of bullying in their school and, based on students’ responses on 48 items, assesses four different participant roles: bully, victim, defender, and outsider. Each subscale consists of 12 items. The bully and victim subscales can be combined to identify students in the bully-victim participation role. Students were asked to indicate how frequently they engaged in relevant activities in the past 30 days; responses are given according to a 5-point scale (never, 1–2 times, 3–4 times, 5–6 times, and 7 or more times). Question stems inquire about bullying behaviours (e.g., I have ignored another student) and victimisation behaviours (e.g., I have been pushed or shoved). There is evidence to support the psychometrics of the BPRS to accurately assess various participant roles in the bullying situation (Summers & Demaray, Reference Summers and Demaray2009). Summers & Demaray (Reference Summers and Demaray2009) conducted item level analysis and exploratory factor analysis to examine the instrument. Exploratory factor analysis resulted in a four-factor solution that accounted for 55% of the variance with factor loadings ranging from .428 to .863. As evidence of reliability, alpha coefficients ranged from .90 to .93.

The How I Feel in Different Situations (HIFDS) questionnaire was utilised to assess two different dimensions of empathy: cognitive and affective (Caravita et al., Reference Caravita, Di Blasio and Salmivalli2009; Feshbach et al., Reference Feshbach, Caprara, Lo Coco, Pastorelli, Manna and Menzres1991). Five items describing the understanding of others’ feelings measure cognitive empathy (e.g., ‘I'm able to recognise, before many other children, that other people's feelings have changed’), and seven items about sharing others’ feelings measure affective empathy (e.g., ‘When somebody tells me a nice story, I feel as if the story is happening to me’). Participants evaluate the extent to which each item is true for them using a 4-point scale (ranging from never true to always true). Scores for both scales are summed across items; higher scores indicate greater empathy. The internal consistency of the cognitive empathy subscale was .71, and the internal consistency of the affective empathy subscale was .80 (Bonino, Lo Coco, & Tani, Reference Bonino, Lo Coco and Tani1998; Caravita et al., Reference Caravita, Di Blasio and Salmivalli2009).

Bandura's Mechanisms of Moral Disengagement Scale (MDS) was employed to measure the individual's tendency to use cognitive mechanisms that can disengage self-sanctions that typically serve to regulate behaviour and justify the use of violent and aggressive behaviours (Bandura, Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996). This scale is the most commonly used measure of moral disengagement around the world (Gini et al., Reference Gini, Pozzoli and Hymel2014). The MDS is a 32-item questionnaire designed to assess a child's proneness to moral disengagement (Bandura, 1995). Items are rated on a 3-point scale (1 = disagree, 2 = not sure, and 3 = agree). Examples of the types of items included in this scale include ‘It is alright to fight to protect your friends’, ‘Teasing someone does not really hurt them’, and ‘Kids cannot be blamed for misbehaving if their friends pressured them to do it’. The MDS assesses the eight mechanisms of moral disengagement described above (Bandura et al., Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996). Research has shown that the MDS taps into each of these eight mechanisms. The scale has shown good reliability (α = .82 and .86; Bandura et al., Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996). Factor analysis (Bandura et al., Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996) has suggested that a one-factor structure can account for 16.2% of the variance.

Data Analyses

Power

This study employed strategies to increase its power and sensitivity (Rossi, Reference Rossi1990). To determine the necessary sample size for this study, power analyses for the design were calculated a priori using G*Power 3.1 (Buchner, Erdfelder, & Faul, Reference Buchner, Erdfelder and Faul1997). Based on Myors & Wolach's (Reference Myors and Wolach2014) recommended power statistic of 0.80, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) with fixed effects, omnibus, one-way was conducted. Given an effect size of 0.14 (low effect size), alpha level of 0.05, power statistic of .80, seven groups, and three response variables, the study's sample size of 702 possesses sufficient power to determine whether there is a relationship between the independent variable and outcome measures if such a relationship exists and is robust enough to detect.

Multivariate data analysis

An multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was utilised to evaluate differences in patterns of mean scores of the quantitative outcome variables across the bully participant groups in a non-experimental research situation (Warner, Reference Warner2008). The independent variables consisted of the bully participants’ roles (Bully, Victim, Outsider, Defender, Bully/Victim, Victim/Defender, Comparison) as measured by the BPRS. The outcome variables consisted of moral disengagement, cognitive empathy, and affective empathy, as measured by the MDS and HIFDS respectively. These outcome measures embody the theoretical dependent variable (Hancock & Mueller, Reference Hancock and Mueller2010). The goal was to see whether the set of means for the bullying groups differed significantly across either of the two outcome variables.

Bivariate linear regressions

Bivariate linear regressions were conducted to evaluate how well moral disengagement could be predicted from both affective and cognitive empathy.

Results

Descriptive Analyses

Gender

See Table 2 for the BPRS groups disaggregated by gender. An independent samples t test was performed to assess whether mean moral disengagement, cognitive empathy, and affective empathy scores differed significantly between males and females. The assumption of homogeneity of variance was assessed by the Levene test for each outcome variable, and indicated a significant violation of the equal variance assumption for moral disengagement and no significant violation of the equal variance assumption for the empathy variables. Consequently, equal variances were not assumed for moral disengagement when examining differences in mean scores between males and females. The mean moral disengagement score differed significantly between males (M = 48.27, SD = 9.57) and females (M = 45.19, SD = 8.42), t(647.143) = 4.49, p < .001, two-tailed. The effect size, as indexed by Cohen's d, was .34, which suggests a small-to-medium effect of gender on moral disengagement when compared to the effects of all other uncontrolled variables on moral disengagement. The mean cognitive empathy score differed significantly between males (M = 12.7, SD = 3.63) and females (M = 14.18, SD = 3.38), t(701) = −1.49, p < .001, two-tailed. The effect size, as indexed by Cohen's d, was .42, which suggests a medium effect of gender on cognitive empathy when compared to the effects of all other uncontrolled variables on cognitive empathy. The mean affective empathy score differed significantly between males (M = 12.46, SD = 3.99) and females (M = 16.91, SD = 4.04), t(701) = −4.44, p < .001, two-tailed. The effect size, as indexed by Cohen's d, was 1.11, which suggests a large effect of gender on affective empathy when compared to the effects of all other uncontrolled variables on affective empathy.

Correlations

A Pearson correlation was performed to examine the relationship among moral disengagement, cognitive empathy, and affective empathy. The correlations between moral disengagement and both cognitive and affective empathy were statistically significant and negative, with a small effect size. The correlation between cognitive empathy and affective empathy was statistically significant and positive, with a large positive effect. See Table 3.

TABLE 3 Correlations Between Moral Disengagement, Cognitive Empathy, and Affective Empathy

Note: *p < .001.

Multivariate Analyses

A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted. The assumptions of a MANOVA include independence of cases, normality, and homogeneity of variances. Examination of histograms for each of the outcome variables indicated that scores were approximately normally distributed. Prior to the analysis, the Levene test for homogeneity of variance was used to examine whether there were serious violations of the assumption of homogeneity of variance across groups. No violations were detected for the outcome variables. Descriptive statistics can be found in Table 4. Results are displayed in Table 5. In addition, all possible pairwise comparisons were made using the Tukey HSD test (using α = .05).

TABLE 4 Means and Standard Deviations of Outcome Variables Across Participant Roles

Note: Range for moral disengagement = 32–96; Range for affective empathy = 6–24; Range for cognitive empathy = 5–20.

TABLE 5 Analysis of Variance for Outcome Variables Across Participant Roles

Note: *p < .001.

Based on Tukey HSD post-hoc test, significant differences were found among the bullying participant groups on the outcome variables (see Table 4 for means and standard deviations of outcome variables across participant roles). On moral disengagement, bullies were significantly more morally disengaged than defenders and the comparison group, and outsiders were significantly more morally disengaged than defenders, victim/defenders, and the comparison group. Outsiders’ elevated level of moral disengagement approached significance in relation to victims. Defenders and victim/defenders had significantly higher levels of cognitive empathy than the comparison group. Defenders and victim/defenders had significantly higher levels of affective empathy than outsiders and the comparison.

Using a Bonferonni correction, an appropriate alpha value was obtained by dividing the error rate (α = .05) by the number of significance tests performed (in this case, three). Thus, a p value less than .0166 was required for the F test to be statistically significant. By this more conservative criterion, most of the individual variables noted above still showed statistically significant differences across the bullying participant groups, with the exception of the difference between victim/defenders and the comparison group on affective empathy (p = .19), the difference between bullies and defenders on moral disengagement (p = .023), and the difference between outsiders and victim/defenders on moral disengagement (p = .049).

Bivariate Linear Regressions

Bivariate regressions were performed to evaluate how well moral disengagement could be predicted from both affective and cognitive empathy (see Table 3). Correlations between moral disengagement and both affective and cognitive empathy were statistically significant and negative. Affective empathy significantly predicted moral disengagement, b = −.381, t(701) = −5.191, p < .001. Affective empathy also explained a statistically significant proportion of variance in moral disengagement, r 2 = .04, F(1, 701) = 26.94, p < .001, suggesting a small effect (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). The results indicate that a negative relationship exists between affective empathy and moral disengagement; with every one unit increase in moral disengagement, affective empathy will decrease by .38. The correlation between cognitive empathy and moral disengagement was statistically significant. Cognitive empathy significantly predicted moral disengagement, b = −.392, t(701) = −4.126, p < .001. Cognitive empathy also explained a statistically significant proportion of variance in moral disengagement, r 2 = .02, F(1, 701) = 17.022, p < .001, suggesting a small effect (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). The results indicated that a negative relationship exists between cognitive empathy and moral disengagement; with every one unit increase in moral disengagement, cognitive empathy will decrease by .39. In sum, students scoring higher in moral disengagement tended to score lower in both types of empathy.

Discussion

This study examined the relation between participant roles in bullying and moral disengagement, cognitive empathy, and affective empathy. Based on self-report measures, participant roles in bullying involvement were identified and levels of outcome variables were assessed. Participation in bullying was categorised according to seven different groups: bully, victim, defender, outsider, bully/victim, victim/defender, and comparison. Findings revealed twice as many females than males in the defender group and the victim/defender group, suggesting that females may be more likely to defend than males, which is consistent with the previous literature (O'Connell, Pepler, & Craig, Reference O'Connell, Pepler and Craig1999; Oh & Hazler, Reference Oh and Hazler2009; Salmivalli et al., Reference Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman and Kaukiainen1996). Initial analyses further revealed that females had significantly higher levels of affective empathy and cognitive empathy. Gender exhibited a large effect on affective empathy and medium effect on cognitive empathy. In general, the empirical evidence is mixed concerning sex differences in empathy. As Eisenberg and Lennon (Reference Eisenberg and Lennon1983) and Eisenberg and Fabes (Reference Eisenberg, Fabes, Damon and Eisenberg1998) have pointed out, meta-analyses examining the empirical data found effect sizes were inconsistent across studies, ranging from small to large. Self-report measures consistently find the largest effect sizes for sex differences in empathy, which Eisenberg and Lennon (Reference Eisenberg and Lennon1983) have suggested may result from demand characteristics that prompt gender-stereotyped responses.

In addition, analyses revealed that males had significantly higher levels of moral disengagement than females and a small-to-medium effect size of gender on moral disengagement. Higher levels of moral disengagement have been consistently found in male as compared to female samples and after controlling for other demographic variables (e.g., ethnicity, socioeconomic status; Bandura et al., Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996; Čermák & Blatný, Reference Čermák and Blatný1995; Obermann, Reference Obermann2011b; Yadava, Sharma, & Gandhi, Reference Yadav, Sharma and Gandhi2001). Studies have further suggested stronger links between moral disengagement and aggression in males than females (Bussman, Reference Bussman2007; Paciello, Fida, Tramontano, Lupinetti, & Caprara, Reference Paciello, Fida, Tramontano, Lupinetti and Caprara2008). However, the results from a recent meta-analysis challenge this link, as it found no significant effect for sex, with identical effect sizes for both males and females (Gini et al., 2013).

Consistent with the hypotheses, several significant differences among the bullying participant groups were detected on moral disengagement, affective empathy, and cognitive empathy. Given that a student who disagreed with all items on the MDS would earn a score of 32 and a student who agreed with all items would earn a score of 96, bullies’ mean score of 49.35 on the MDS suggests that they were significantly more unsure about the rightness of the actions, beliefs, and attitudes described in the items on the MDS than defenders and the comparison group, as hypothesised. This is consistent with research that has shown that children and youth with elevated levels of moral disengagement are more likely to engage in both general aggression (e.g., Bandura et al., Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996; Caprara, Pastorelli, & Bandura, Reference Caprara, Pastorelli and Bandura1995) and peer bullying (e.g., Gini, Reference Gini2006; Hymel et al., Reference Hymel, Rocke-Henderson and Bonanno2005). Similarly, outsiders were significantly more unsure about the rightness of the actions, beliefs, and attitudes described by the items in the MDS than defenders, victim/defenders, and the comparison group. Put another way, defenders and victim/defenders were significantly more certain that the actions, beliefs, and attitudes described in the MDS were wrong than bullies and outsiders, as predicted. Defenders and victim/defenders had significantly higher levels of cognitive empathy than the comparison group, as hypothesised, but not the other participant groups. Defenders and victim/defenders had significantly higher levels of affective empathy than outsiders and the comparison group. Contrary to the hypotheses, mean levels of both types of empathy were not significantly lower for bullies in relation to the other groups.

The results of this study are similar to findings by Obermann (Reference Obermann2011a) that indicated that outsiders, like passive bystanders, may activate and employ more of the moral disengagement mechanisms. While this study found that bullies’ mean level of moral disengagement was significantly higher than the comparison group and defenders, the outsiders indicated the highest level of moral disengagement, significantly higher than nearly all other groups.

In contrast to studies that have found lower levels of empathy among bullies (e.g., Gini et al., Reference Gini, Pozzoli and Hauser2011), including emotional empathy (Arsenio & Lemerise, Reference Arsenio and Lemerise2001) and cognitive empathy (see Crick & Dodge, Reference Crick and Dodge1999 and Espelage et al., Reference Espelage, Mebane, Adams, Espelage and Swearer2004 for discussion), this study did not find significant differences in bullies’ levels of empathy — only in levels of moral disengagement. Bullies’ higher levels of moral disengagement relative to their peers suggests that they view the use of aggressive behaviour instrumentally to achieve social goals as more permissible than other groups. These results provide support for Bandura's social cognitive theory of moral behaviour, which posits that moral reasoning is linked to moral behaviour through a series of self-regulatory mechanisms through which moral agency is exercised (Bandura, Reference Bandura2002). In this study, bullies appeared to possess normal to slightly higher levels of cognitive and affective empathy relative to their peers in conjunction with significantly higher levels of moral disengagement. As such, it does not appear that bullies necessarily lack empathy as some research suggests (e.g., Gini et al., Reference Gini, Albiero, Benelli and Altoe2007; Gini et al., Reference Gini, Pozzoli and Hauser2011), but that, consistent with Bandura's theory, they may more readily employ moral disengagement mechanisms to override their empathic feelings and justify their use of instrumental aggression with some of their peers (Bandura et al., Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996). It is not that they fail to emotionally recognise the pain and suffering they are inflicting on others, but that they disengage from self-regulatory mechanisms that would normally cause them to feel guilty about such actions.

According to Bandura, this is precisely how ‘good’ people behave ‘badly’. It is not that they are incapable of an ‘emotional response that emanates from the emotional state of another individual’ (Feshback, Reference Feshbach, Bohart and Greenberg1997, p. 35), but that they are able to employ psychological manoeuvres that disengage moral self-sanctions and short-circuit emotional responses like empathy under certain circumstances (Bandura, Reference Bandura1999). As Bandura clarifies, empathetic responsiveness to the plight of others is most salient and arousing for those with whom we identify and has a ‘powerful self-restraining effect’ (Bandura, Reference Bandura1999, p. 200). The dehumanisation of the other decreases victimisers’ sense of personal distress and self-condemnation when committing harmful acts (Bandura, Underwood, & Fromson, Reference Bandura, Underwood and Fromson1975). In this way, students engaging in bullying may hold moral standards that they deem applicable to some but not all of their fellow students.

This study did find that defenders and defenders who had been bullied (i.e., victim/defenders) possessed significantly higher levels of cognitive empathy than the comparison group and significantly higher levels of affective empathy than outsiders and the comparison group. By contrast, Pöyhönen and colleagues (Reference Pöyhönen, Juvonen and Salmivalli2010) found that defenders indicated significantly higher levels of affective, but not cognitive, empathy. Findings from this study suggest that individuals who intellectually understand or have knowledge about the victim's suffering and also emotionally identify with it will be more likely to defend victims in bullying situations. Along with empathy and moral engagement, other factors that contribute to the likelihood that a bystander will actively intervene in a bullying situation include prosocial attitudes, a sense of personal responsibility, a strong sense of self-efficacy for defending, and higher levels of social status among peers (Pöyhönen et al., Reference Pöyhönen, Juvonen and Salmivalli2010).

Limitations and Future Directions

This study suffered from many of the normal limitations inherent to quantitative descriptive research designs that use convenience samples and self-report surveys. While the large sample size is a strength, the convenience sample used raises issues regarding external validity, generalisability, and bias. The use of self-reports to ask children questions about bullying and empathy also introduces construct validity issues related to reactivity (e.g., social desirability), which could confound the true effects. For example, defending behaviour is likely perceived as expected and prosocial by students, which may have influenced their responses and inflated the size of the defender group. Moreover, since the results of this study are based on self-assessments, the results are dependent upon whether or not the participants answered honestly and were subsequently placed in participant role groups that accurately reflected their participation in bullying, which cannot be conclusively determined. Validating the results through independent assessment of participants’ roles (e.g., peer nominations, teacher nominations) would improve the validity of the data through triangulation.

Various methods are utilised to create groups and group membership in bullying and victimisation research. This study used cut-scores to classify students and define groups, which is a common method; however, this method may impose measurement inaccuracies and, as a result, introduce error into the assessment of outcome variables associated with those groups (Nylund, Bellmore, Nishina, & Graham, Reference Nylund, Bellmore, Nishina and Graham2007). Latent class analysis represents an alternative method that assesses the existence of underlying categorical latent variables. As a result, LCA is able to empirically define unique profiles for mutually exclusive latent classes, such as bully or victim groups, and clarify nuanced patterns among students (Giang & Graham, 2007; Williford, Brisson, Bender, Jenson, & Forrest-Bank, Reference Williford, Brisson, Bender, Jenson and Forrest-Bank2011). Since behaviour associated with bullying and victimisation is complex, future research on moral disengagement, empathy and participation in bullying that utilises latent class analysis to adequately attend to this complexity is warranted.

Finally, Bandura's theory assumes that moral disengagement occurs when individuals bypass previously accepted and internalised social and moral standards. What is unclear is the degree to which bullies have in fact internalised these social and moral norms. Conversely, it may be the case that bullies are knowledgeable of such standards but have not personally internalised them. In such a scenario, there would be no moral standards to disengage, nothing to prevent one from transgressing against them. Laible, Eye, and Carlo (Reference Laible, Eye and Carlo2008) investigated the relationship between the level of internalisation of moral values as one aspect of moral cognition and found that level of internalisation is negatively associated with bullying behaviour. Yet, the relationship between internalisation of moral values and bullying behaviour is still fairly unclear. Future research should endeavor to discern the degree to which bullies have in fact accepted and internalised social and moral norms as opposed to simply possessing knowledge about them.

Implications

The results of this study hold implications for the promotion of prosocial behaviour and the reduction of bullying and victimisation among youth at school. The study's findings concerning levels of empathy and moral disengagement may contribute to our theoretical understanding of the bullying process. The levels of affective and cognitive empathy found for bullies diverge from studies that have found significantly lower levels of cognitive empathy (Crick & Dodge, Reference Crick and Dodge1999; Sutton et al., Reference Sutton, Smith and Swettenham1999) and, especially, affective empathy (Arsenio & Lemerise, Reference Arsenio and Lemerise2001; Gini et al., Reference Gini, Albiero, Benelli and Altoe2007) among bullies. In contrast, this study found similar levels of both types of empathy for bullies and other groups. In addition, the negative relation found between both types of empathy and moral disengagement coupled with the finding of higher levels of empathy and moral engagement among defenders suggests prosocial behaviour, and moral engagement may involve a critical interrelation between the two factors. In sum, it appears that while empathy may be necessary for the promotion of moral engagement and prosocial behaviour, empathy alone may not be sufficient. The findings of the present study seem to indicate that, for some aggressive individuals, certain psychological mechanisms articulated by Bandura et al. (Reference Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara and Pastorelli1996) are capable of overriding normal levels of empathy and moral self-sanctions to justify the victimisation of others.

Given the study's focus on differences in moral disengagement and empathy among bullying participant groups, the implications for practice are critical. Defenders’ levels of empathy and moral engagement stood out as different from the various bullying roles and the comparison group. If we take the defenders as the optimal outcome, and if a true relation exists between empathy and moral disengagement, it appears that both higher levels of empathy and lower levels of moral disengagement are necessary to prevent bullying and promote defending behaviour. Consequently, all students, not just bullies, likely stand to benefit from programs aimed at fostering empathy and, especially, moral engagement. The results of this study can also inform intervention. For example, if outsiders are employing displacement or diffusion of responsibility to justify not intervening when they witness bullying, interventions must take steps to directly counteract this type of thinking and effectively persuade outsiders that they are responsible. If bullies are using in-group/out-group thinking and dehumanisation to justify victimising others, interventions must address this by fostering students’ perspective-taking abilities and a more inclusive sense of connection with the human community. Such efforts can be undertaken in a variety of ways, ranging from targeted social skills groups and counselling to school- and classroom-based programs to community-based youth sports, community, and religious groups.

Cognitive and affective empathy and moral engagement can be fostered gradually and systematically by weaving social-emotional learning programming (Bowles et al., Reference Bowles, Jimerson, Haddock, Nolan, Jablonski, Czub and Coelho2017) into school curriculums (e.g., Responsive Classroom; Rimm-Kaufman et al., Reference Rimm-Kaufman, Larsen, Baroody, Curby, Ko, Thomas and DeCoster2014) and through universal, classroom-based interventions (e.g., Second Step; Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor, & Schellinger, Reference Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor and Schellinger2011; Elias & Arnold, Reference Elias and Arnold2006). Importantly, it is recommended that such programs be grounded in a developmentalist approach to moral development, such as domain theory (Nucci, Reference Nucci2009; Turiel, Reference Turiel1983, Reference Turiel2002, Reference Turiel, Damon, Lerner and Eisenberg2006), rather than a traditionalist approach to ‘character education’ (e.g., Ryan, Reference Ryan and Nucci1989; Wynne, Reference Wynne and Nucci1989; on the debate between developmentalists and traditionalists, see Haddock, Reference Haddock, Fitzsimmons and Lanphar2010). A developmentalist approach to moral education emphasises moral reasoning and the development of empathy and teaches emotional regulation and conflict resolution skills (Arsenio, Reference Arsenio2002; Nucci, Reference Nucci2001, Reference Nucci2009; Turiel, Reference Turiel1983, Reference Turiel and Nucci1989, Reference Turiel2002). From the developmentalist perspective, authentic moral education means transcending the socialisation of conventionally prosocial children toward fostering the development of the moral conceptions of fairness, human welfare, and rights coupled with a critical moral orientation toward their own conduct and the norms and mores of society (Turiel, Reference Turiel2002). In the words of John Holt, ‘we do not want to mistake good behaviour for good character’ (Holt, 1966, as quoted in Kohn, Reference Kohn1997). Rather, we want to employ practices that cultivate students’ empathy for others, capacity to resist unjust conventions, and motivation to contribute to the ethicality of our social institutions. In sum, this study makes significant contributions to research on the relation between moral disengagement, affective and cognitive empathy, and bullying that hold important insights for effective prevention and intervention efforts in schools.

References

American Educational Research Association (AERA). (2013). Prevention of bullying in schools, colleges, and universities: Research report and recommendations. Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Andreou, E., & Metallidou, P. (2004). The relationship of academic and social cognition to behaviour in bullying situations among Greek primary school children. Educational Psychology, 24, 2741. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144341032000146421 Google Scholar
Arsenio, W.F. (2002). Moral education and domains in the classroom: Is nothing as practical as a good theory? [Book review]. School Psychology Quarterly, 17, 100107. https://doi.org/10.1521/scpq.17.1.100.19905 Google Scholar
Arsenio, W.F., & Lemerise, E.A. (2001). Varieties of childhood bullying: Values, emotion processes, and social competence. Social Development, 10, 5973. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00148 Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1990). Selective activation and disengagement of moral control. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 2746. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb00270.x Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action. In Kurtines, W.M. & Gewirtz, G.L. (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development: Theory, research and applications (vol. 1, pp. 71129). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 193209. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_3 Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Moral Education, 31, 101119. https://doi.org/10.1080/0305724022014322 Google Scholar
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G.V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996) Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 125135. doi: 0022–3514/96/$3.00 https://doi.org/10.1037/t00941-000 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A., Underwood, B., & Fromson, M.E. (1975). Disinhibition of aggression through diffusion of responsibility and dehumanization of victims. Journal of Research in Personality, 9, 253269. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(75)90001-x Google Scholar
Barboza, G.E., Schiamberg, L.B., Oehmke, J., Korzeniewski, S.J., Post, L.A., & Heraux, C.G. (2009). Individual characteristics and the multiple contexts of adolescent bullying: An ecological perspective. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 101121. doi:10.1007/s10964-008-9271-1 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9271-1 Google Scholar
Beauchaine, T.P., & Hinshaw, S.P. (Eds.). (2016). Child and adolescent psychopathology (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Benbenishty, R., & Astor, R.A. (2005). School violence in context: Culture, neighborhood, family, school, and gender. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, J.Y., & Watson, M.W. (1997). Children who are targets of bullying: A victim pattern. Journal of interpersonal violence, 12, 483498. https://doi.org/10.1177/088626097012004001 Google Scholar
Bonino, S., Lo Coco, A., & Tani, F. (1998). Empatia. Processi di condivisione delle emozioni.Google Scholar
Bowles, T., Jimerson, S., Haddock, A., Nolan, J., Jablonski, S., Czub, M., & Coelho, V. (2017). A review of the provision of social and emotional learning in Australia, the United States, Poland and Portugal. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bryn, S. (2011). Stop bullying now! A federal campaign for bullying prevention and intervention. Journal of School Violence, 10, 213219. https://doi.org/10.1080/15388220.2011.557313 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchner, A., Erdfelder, E., & Faul, F. (1997). How to use G*Power. Retrieved from http://www.psycho.uni-duesseldorf.de/aap/projects/gpower/how_to_use_gpower.html Google Scholar
Bussman, J.R. (2007). Moral disengagement in children's overt and relational aggression. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 68 (7-B), 4813.Google Scholar
Caprara, G.V., Pastorelli, C., & Bandura, A. (1995). La misura del disimpegno morale in età evolutiva. Età Evolutiva, 51, 1829.Google Scholar
Caravita, S.C.S., Di Blasio, P., & Salmivalli, C. (2009). Unique and interactive effects of empathy and social status on involvement in bullying. Social Development, 18 (1), 140163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00465.x Google Scholar
Carifio, J. (1994). Sensitive data and students’ tendencies to give socially desirable responses. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 39 (2), 7484.Google Scholar
Carney, A.G., & Merrell, K.W. (2001). Bullying in schools: Perspectives on understanding and preventing an international problem. School Psychology International, 22, 364382. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034301223011 Google Scholar
Čermák, I., & Blatný, M. (1995). Personality indicators of aggression and moral disengagement. Studia Psychologica, 37, 199201.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587 Google Scholar
Cohen, D., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy in conduct-disordered and comparison youth. Developmental Psychology, 32, 988998. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.32.6.988 Google Scholar
Cornell, D., Klein, J., Konold, T., & Huang, F. (2012). Effects of validity screening items on adolescent survey data. Psychological Assessment, 24, 2135. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024824 Google Scholar
Crick, N.R., & Dodge, K.A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social-information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustments. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74101. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.115.1.74 Google Scholar
Crick, N.R., & Dodge, K.A. (1999). ‘Superiority’ is in the eye of the beholder: A comment on Sutton, Smith, and Swettenham. Social Development, 8, 128131. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00084 Google Scholar
Davis, M.H. (1994). Empathy: A social psychological approach. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Dodge, K.A., Coie, J.D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth. In Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), William, D. & Lerner, R.M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 719788). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0312 Google Scholar
Dolan, M.C. & Fullam, R.S. (2010). Moral/conventional transgression distinction and psychopathy in conduct disordered adolescent offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 9951000. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.011 Google Scholar
Durlak, J.A., Weissberg, R.P., Dymnicki, A.B., Taylor, R.D., & Schellinger, K.B. (2011). The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta‐analysis of school‐based universal interventions. Child Development, 82, 405432. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R.A. (1998). Prosocial development. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 701778). New York: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0311 Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100131. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.94.1.100 Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P.A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 91119. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.101.1.91 Google Scholar
Elias, N., & Jephcott, E. (1982). The civilizing process (vol. 2). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Elias, M.J., & Arnold, H. (2006). The educator's guide to emotional intelligence and academic achievement: Social-emotional learning in the classroom. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.Google Scholar
Endresen, I.M., & Olweus, D. (2001). Self-reported empathy in Norwegian adolescents: Sex differences, age trends, and relationship to bullying. In Bohart, A.C. and Stipek, D.J. (Eds.), (2001), Constructive & destructive behavior: Implications for family, school, & society (pp. 147165). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10433-007 Google Scholar
Espelage, D.L. (2012). Bullying prevention: A research dialogue with Dorothy Espelage. Prevention Researcher, 19, 1719. https://doi.org/10.1037/e534992013-006 Google Scholar
Espelage, D.L., Mebane, S.E., & Adams, R.S. (2004). Empathy, caring and bullying: Toward an understanding of complex associations. In Espelage, D.L. & Swearer, S.M. (Eds.), Bullying in American schools (pp. 3761). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Espelage, D.L., & Swearer, S.M. (Eds.). (2004). Bullying in American schools: A social-ecological perspective on prevention and intervention. New York, NY: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410609700 Google Scholar
Espelage, D.L., & Swearer, S.M. (2010). A social-ecological model for bullying prevention and intervention: Understanding the impact of adults in the social ecology of youngsters. In Jimerson, S.R., Swearer, S.M., & Espelage, D.L. (Eds.), Handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective (pp. 6172). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Faull, C., Swearer, S.M., Jimerson, S.R., Espelage, D.L., & Ng, R. (2008). Promoting positive peer relationships: Middle school bullying prevention program — Classroom resource. Adelaide, Australia: Readymade Productions.Google Scholar
Feshbach, N.D. (1997). Empathy: The formative years—Implications for clinical practice. In Bohart, A.C. & Greenberg, L.S. (Eds.), Empathy reconsidered: New directions in psychotherapy (pp. 3359). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10226-001 Google Scholar
Feshbach, N.D., Caprara, G.V., Lo Coco, A., Pastorelli, C., Manna, G., & Menzres, J. (1991, July). Empathy and its correlates: Cross cultural data from Italy. Paper presented at the Eleventh Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Minneapolis, MN.Google Scholar
Frick, P.J. (2006). Developmental pathways to conduct disorder. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 15, 311331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2005.11.003 Google Scholar
Furlong, M.J., Sharkey, J.D., Felix, E., Tanigawa, D., & Greif-Green, J. (2010). Bullying assessment: A call for increased precision of self-reporting procedures. In Jimerson, S.R., Swearer, S.M., & Espelage, D.L. (Eds.), The international handbook of school bullying (pp. 329346). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Giang, M.T., & Graham, S. (2008). Using latent class analysis to identify aggressors and victims of peer harassment. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 203213. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20233 Google Scholar
Gini, G. (2006). Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: What's wrong? Aggressive Behavior, 32, 528539. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20153 Google Scholar
Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoe, G. (2007). Does empathy predict adolescents’ bullying and defending behavior? Aggressive Behavior, 33, 467476. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20204 Google Scholar
Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoè, G. (2008). Determinants of adolescents’ active defending and passive bystanding behavior in bullying. Journal of Adolescence, 31, 93105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.05.002 Google Scholar
Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., & Hauser, M. (2011). Bullies have enhanced moral competence to judge relative to victims, but lack moral compassion. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 603608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.12.002 Google Scholar
Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., & Hymel, S. (2014). Moral disengagement among children and youth: A meta‐analytic review of links to aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 40, 5668. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21502 Google Scholar
Haddock, A. (2010). The debate over moral education: Traditionalist, developmentalist, modernist. In Fitzsimmons, P., & Lanphar, E. (Eds.), Cross examination of the core: An interdisciplinary focus on authentic learning (pp. 2042). Santa Barbara, CA: Antonian Publishing.Google Scholar
Hancock, G.R. & Mueller, R.O. (2010). The reviewer's guide to quantitative methods in the social sciences. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haney, C., Banks, W.C., & Zimbardo, P.G. (1973). A study of prisoners and guards in a simulated prison. Naval Research Review, 30, 417.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M.L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M.L. (2001). Toward a comprehensive empathy-based theory of prosocial moral development. In Bohart, A. & Stipek, D. (Eds), Constructive and destructive behavior: Implications for family, school, and society (pp. 6186). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10433-003 Google Scholar
Hymel, S., Rocke-Henderson, N., Bonanno, R.A. (2005). Moral disengagement: A framework for understanding bullying among adolescents. Journal of Social Sciences Special Issue, 8, 111.Google Scholar
Hymel, S., Schonert-Reichl, K.A., Bonanno, R.A., Vaillancourt, T., & Rocke Henderson, N. (2010). Bullying and morality: Understanding how good kids can behave badly. In Jimerson, S., Swearer, S., & Espelage, D. (Eds.), Handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective (pp. 101118). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hymel, S., & Swearer, S.M. (2015). Four decades of research on school bullying: An Introduction. American Psychologist, 70 (4), 293299. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038928 Google Scholar
Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D.P. (2004). Empathy and offending: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 441476. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2003.03.001 Google Scholar
Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D.P. (2006). Examining the relationship between low empathy and bullying. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 540550. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20154 Google Scholar
Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D.P. (2011). Is low empathy related to bullying after controlling for individual and social background variables? Journal of Adolescence, 34, 5971. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.02.001 Google Scholar
Kaukiainen, A., Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K., Osterman, K., Salmivalli, C., Rothberg, S., & Ahlbom, A. (1999). The relationships between social intelligence, empathy, and three types of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 8189. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-2337(1999)25:2%3C81::aid-ab1%3E3.0.co;2-m Google Scholar
Kohn, A. (1997). How not to teach values: A critical look at character education. Phi Delta Kappan, 78, 429439.Google Scholar
Laible, D., Eye, J., & Carlo, G. (2008). Dimensions of conscience in mid-adolescence: Links with social behavior, parenting, and temperament. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 875887. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9277-8 Google Scholar
Malti, T., Gasser, L., & Buchmann, M. (2009). Aggressive and prosocial children's emotion attributions and moral reasoning. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 90102. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20289 Google Scholar
Menesini, E., Sanchez, V., Fonzi, A., Ortega, R., Costabile, A., & Lo Feudo, G. (2003). Moral emotions and bullying: A cross-national comparison of differences between bullies, victims, and outsiders. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 515530. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.10060 Google Scholar
Merrell, K.W., Gueldner, B.A., Ross, S.W., & Isava, D.M. (2008). How effective are school bullying intervention programs? A meta-analysis of intervention research. School Psychology Quarterly, 23, 26. https://doi.org/10.1037/1045-3830.23.1.26 Google Scholar
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371378. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040525 Google Scholar
Miller, P.A., & Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 324. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.103.3.324 Google Scholar
Myors, B., & Wolach, A. (2014). Statistical power analysis: A simple and general model for traditional and modern hypothesis tests. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315773155 Google Scholar
Neiman, S., DeVoe, J.F., & Chandler, K. (2009). Crime, violence, discipline, and safety in U.S. public schools: Findings from the School Survey on Crime and Safety: 2007–08. Washington, DC: National Center for Educational Statistics.Google Scholar
Nucci, L. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511605987 Google Scholar
Nucci, L. (2009). Nice is not enough: Facilitating moral development. Mahwah, NJ: Pearson. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240902792793 Google Scholar
Nylund, K.L., Bellmore, A., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2007). Subtypes, severity, and structural stability of peer victimization: What does latent class analysis say? Child Development, 78, 17061722. https://doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01097 Google Scholar
Obermann, M.L. (2011a). Moral disengagement among bystanders to school bullying. Journal of School Violence, 10, 239257. https://doi.org/10.1080/15388220.2011.578276 Google Scholar
Obermann, M.L. (2011b). Moral disengagement in self‐reported and peer‐nominated school bullying. Aggressive Behavior, 37, 133144. https://doi:10.1002/ab.20378 Google Scholar
O'Connell, P., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (1999). Peer involvement in bullying: Insights and challenges for intervention. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 437–52. https://doi:10.1006/jado.1999.0238 Google Scholar
Oh, I., & Hazler, R.J. (2009). Contributions of personal and situational factors to bystanders’ reactions to school bullying. School Psychology International, 30, 291310. https://doi:10.1177/0143034309106499 Google Scholar
Olweus, D. (1991). Bully/victim problems among schoolchildren: Basic facts and effects of a school based intervention program. The Development and Treatment of Childhood Aggression, 17, 411448.Google Scholar
Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Olweus, D. (1994). Bullying at school: Basic facts and effects of a school based intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 11711190. https://doi:0021-9630/94$700+0.00 Google Scholar
Olweus, D. (1996). The revised Olweus Bully-victim Questionnaire [Mimeo]. Bergen, Norway: Research Center for Health Promotion, University of Bergen.Google Scholar
Olweus, D. (2010). Understanding and researching bullying: Some critical issues. In Jimerson, S.R., Swearer, S.M., & Espelage, D.L. (Eds.), Handbook of bullying in schools: An international perspective (pp. 934). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Osher, D., Dwyer, K., Jimerson, S.R., & Brown, J.A. (2012). Developing safe, supportive, and effective schools: Facilitating student success to reduce school violence. In Jimerson, S.R., Nickerson, A.B., Mayer, M.J., & Furlong, M.J. (Eds.), The handbook of school violence and school safety: International research and practice (2nd ed., pp. 2744). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Paciello, M., Fida, R., Tramontano, C., Lupinetti, C., & Caprara, G.V. (2008). Stability and change of moral disengagement and its impact on aggression and violence in late adolescence. Child Development, 79, 12881309. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01189.x Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature: Why violence has declined. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
Polman, H., de Castro, B.O., Koops, W., van Boxtel, H.W., & Merk, W.W. (2007). A meta-analysis of the distinction between reactive and proactive aggression in children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 522535. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9109-4 Google Scholar
Pöyhönen, V., Juvonen, J., & Salmivalli, C. (2010). What does it take to stand up for the victim of bullying? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 56, 143163. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.0.0046 Google Scholar
Rigby, K., & Slee, P.T. (1993). Dimensions of interpersonal relation among Australian children and implications for psychological well-being. The Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 3342. https://doi 10.1080/00224545.1993.9712116 Google Scholar
Rimm-Kaufman, S.E., Larsen, R.A., Baroody, A.E., Curby, T.W., Ko, M., Thomas, J.B., . . . DeCoster, J. (2014). Efficacy of the Responsive Classroom approach: Results from a 3-year, longitudinal randomized controlled trial. American Educational Research Journal, 51, 567603. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831214523821 Google Scholar
Rossi, J.S. (1990). Statistical power of psychological research: What have we gained in 20 years? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 646656. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.58.5.646 Google Scholar
Ryan, K. (1989). In defense of character education. In Nucci, L. (ed.), Moral development and character education: A dialogue (pp. 318). Berkeley, CA: McCutchan Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying as a group process: Participant roles and their relations to social status within the group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 115. https://doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1996)22:1<1::AID-AB1>3.0.CO;2-T Google Scholar
Sherif, M. (1958). Superordinate goals in the reduction of intergroup conflict. American Journal of Sociology, 349356. https://doi.org/10.1086/222258 Google Scholar
Sherif, M., Harvey, O.J., White, B.J., Hood, W.R., & Sherif, C.W. (1961). Intergroup conflict and cooperation: The Robbers Cave experiment (vol. 10). Norman, OK: University Book Exchange.Google Scholar
Slee, P.T. (1993). Bullying: A preliminary investigation of the nature and effects on social cognition. Early Child Development and Care, 87, 4757. https://doi10.1080/0300443930870105 Google Scholar
Smith, P.K., & Morita, Y. (1999). Introduction. In Smith, P.K., Morita, Y., JungerTas, J., Olweus, D., Catalano, R., & Slee, P. (Eds.), The nature of school bullying: A cross-national perspective (pp. 14). London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Stavrinides, P., Georgiou, S., & Theofanous, V. (2010). Bullying and empathy: A short-term longitudinal investigation. Educational Psychology, 30, 793802. https://doi:10.1080/01443410.2010.506004 Google Scholar
Summers, K.H., & Demaray, M.K. (2009). The relationship among bullying participant roles, social support and school climate. Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations.Google Scholar
Sutton, J., Smith, P.K., & Swettenham, J. (1999). Social cognition and bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 435450. https://doi10.1348/026151099165384 Google Scholar
Tangney, J.P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D.J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Psychology, 58, 345372. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145 Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1989). Multifaceted social reasoning and educating for character, culture, and development. In Nucci, L. (Ed.), Moral development and character education: A dialogue (pp. 161182). Berkeley, CA: McCutchan Publishing.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (2002). The culture of morality: Social development, context, and conflict. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511613500 Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (2006). The development of morality. In Damon, W., Lerner, R.M., Eisenberg, N. (Eds.), The handbook of childhood psychology, volume 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 789857). Wiley.Google Scholar
Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., & McDougall, P. (2003). Bullying is power: Implications for school-based intervention strategies. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 19, 157176. https://doi.org/10.1300/j008v19n02_10 Google Scholar
Vaillancourt, T., McDougall, P., Hymel, S., Krygsman, A., Miller, J., Stiver, K., Davis, C. (2008). Bullying: Are researchers and children/youth talking about the same thing? International Journal of Behavior, 32, 486495. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408095553 Google Scholar
Warden, D., & Mackinnon, S. (2003). Prosocial children, bullies and victims: An investigation of their sociometric status, empathy and social problem-solving strategies. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 367385. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151003322277757 Google Scholar
Warner, R.M. (2008). Applied statistics: From bivariate through multivariate techniques. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Williford, A.P., Brisson, D., Bender, K.A., Jenson, J.M., & Forrest-Bank, S. (2011). Patterns of aggressive behavior and peer victimization from childhood to early adolescence: A latent class analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 644655. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-010-9583-9 Google Scholar
Wynne, E.A. (1989). Transmitting traditional values in contemporary schools. In Nucci, L. (ed.), Moral development and character education: A dialogue (pp. 1936). Berkeley, CA: McCutchan Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
Yadav, A., Sharma, N.R., & Gandhi, A. (2001). Aggression and moral disengagement. Journal of Personality and Clinical Studies, 17, 9599.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Summary of Participants

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Bullying Groups Across Gender and Grade

Figure 2

TABLE 3 Correlations Between Moral Disengagement, Cognitive Empathy, and Affective Empathy

Figure 3

TABLE 4 Means and Standard Deviations of Outcome Variables Across Participant Roles

Figure 4

TABLE 5 Analysis of Variance for Outcome Variables Across Participant Roles