Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-m6qld Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-19T20:58:40.294Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Leaders but Not Authorities? Gender, Veterans, and Messages about National Security

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 April 2024

JONATHAN D. CAVERLEY*
Affiliation:
U.S. Naval War College, United States
YANNA KRUPNIKOV*
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, United States
*
Corresponding author: Jonathan D. Caverley, Professor, Strategic and Operational Research, U.S. Naval War College, United States, jon.caverley@usnwc.edu.
Yanna Krupnikov, Professor, Department of Communication and Media, University of Michigan, United States, yanna@umich.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Politicians’ ability to provide national security to the public is deeply enmeshed in conceptions of the state and of leadership. This article incorporates securitization, feminist, and political communication theories to consider whether gendered and militarized conceptions of national security have different effects for politicians who are women and those who are men. Although scholarship suggests that signaling military bona fides—such as invoking one’s veteran status—can help politicians claim that certain policies are a matter of national security, we consider whether this ability will be gendered. Relying on two national studies, we find results that are contrary to our original predictions. First, we find that military bona fides do help women be seen as leaders. However, we do not find evidence that bona fides increase the “authority” to identify and address national security threats for any politicians.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
This is a work of the US Government and is not subject to copyright protection within the United States. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of American Political Science Association.
Copyright
© United States Navy, 2024

Two of the century’s most famous American campaign advertisements address gender stereotypes and security concerns. Hillary Clinton’s 2008 “3 a.m. Phone Call” highlighted her foreign policy experience relative to the primary opponent Barack Obama. The ad showed a mother looking in on her sleeping children while a male narrator described Clinton as, “someone who already knows the world’s leaders, knows the military, someone tested and ready to lead in a dangerous world.” A decade later, Texas congressional candidate M. J. Hegar, “an Air Force combat veteran and a mom,” aired the viral “Doors” ad, focusing on her combat experience and decorations for valor. Both ads were praised for their effectiveness at advancing the candidates’ security bona fides (though both candidates lost).

That the two ads focused on security should not be surprising. Women often work to demonstrate expertise across numerous issues when running for office (Atkinson and Windett Reference Atkinson and Windett2019; Bauer Reference Bauer2020)—and women are often especially disadvantaged in the masculine policy space of security (Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022; Schramm and Stark Reference Schramm and Stark2020; Schwartz and Blair Reference Schwartz and Blair2020). Could signaling military bona fides open a way for women to enter the national discourse in a space that has long excluded them (Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022; Schramm and Stark Reference Schramm and Stark2020)?

Even as women are underrepresented in American politics relative to the U.S. population, veterans are overrepresented.Footnote 1 The six women veterans (joining 90 men) in the 117th Congress is a near-historic high (Bialik Reference Bialik2017), and this number is even more remarkable given that the first woman veteran was only elected in 1998 (Harberkorn Reference Harberkorn2018; Mitchell Reference Mitchell2018). As Seth Lynn, an advocate for veteran candidates, argues, “Women candidates, fair or unfair, are often questioned about whether they’re tough enough for the job, whereas male candidates aren’t.” If that woman is a veteran, according to Lynn, the question “never even comes up” (Mitchell Reference Mitchell2018).

Bridging a large body of International Relations (IR) theory with research on the role of gender in political communication, we consider the intersection of gender and veteran status. We find that highlighting one’s security bona fides—either through hawkish rhetoric or by highlighting veteran status—can help women and men receive higher leadership trait evaluations. These qualifications, however, are less effective when the goal is to persuade people that a policy is a matter of national security—a process that requires authority (Wæver Reference Wæver2011). Put another way, bona fides may help politicians, particularly women, be perceived as leaders but will not help them be perceived as authorities.

Our results come from two experiments conducted on national samples of American adults. The second study is our main focus, but we present the first study as it informs our argument.Footnote 2 These studies contribute to research programs in IR, gender and politics, and political communication, as well as reinforce the value of political psychology methods, used extensively in IR’s “behavioral revolution” (Hafner-Burton et al. Reference Hafner-Burton, Haggard, Lake and Victor2017; Kertzer and Tingley Reference Kertzer and Tingley2018), for more critical theoretical approaches to international politics (Baele and Thomson Reference Baele and Thomson2017; Jackson Reference Jackson2011, 105–6).

WAR, THE STATE, AND MAN

We begin by observing that the construction of security, defense, and war as the state’s primary function has powerful political consequences to this day (Hobbes Reference Hobbes1982; Schmitt Reference Schmitt2007; Tickner Reference Tickner1992; Tilly Reference Tilly1992). The provision of security for its citizens is traditionally regarded as the state’s most profound duty, a principal source of its legitimacy, a central obligation for aspiring leaders, and a justification for extracting resources from the public. We couple this concept of the state as security provider with feminist and critical IR claims that state legitimacy and coercion are founded through socially constructed power relations built on both militarism and gender exclusion (Elshtain Reference Elshtain1995; Enloe Reference Enloe2000; Sjoberg Reference Sjoberg2013; Tickner Reference Tickner1992).

The Process of “Securitizing”

This special status of security allows actors to “securitize” issues (Buzan, Wæver, and de Wilde Reference Buzan, Wæver and de Wilde1998, 23–4) on behalf of the state, thereby gaining additional political resources and avoiding normal political constraints. Wæver (Reference Wæver2011, 469) describes the process succinctly as “a securitizing actor claiming an existential threat to a valued referent object in order to make the audience tolerate extraordinary measures that otherwise would not have been acceptable.” The referent object is most traditionally the state. An audience ultimately decides if the threat merits an extraordinary response. Between these two components lies the “securitizing actor,” who strategically presents to the audience a case that the referent object is endangered.

Although the theory rests on the idea of authority, this securitization research program, often known as the “Copenhagen School,” has paid comparatively little attention to how an actor achieves the “authorization” to speak about security (Balzacq Reference Balzacq2005; Léonard and Kaunert Reference Léonard, Kaunert and Balzacq2010; McDonald Reference McDonald2008). Much of the research assumes that common securitizing actors include “political leaders, bureaucracies, governments, lobbyists, and pressure groups” (Buzan, Wæver, and de Wilde Reference Buzan, Wæver and de Wilde1998, 40). According to Buzan, Wæver, and de Wilde (Buzan, Wæver, and de Wilde Reference Buzan, Wæver and de Wilde1998, 41), a successful securitization process depends less on “who performs the speech than of what logic shapes the action.” This idea that the “who” is less important than the “what,” however, has come into question by important theoretic approaches in IR.

Who Gets to Securitize?

Securitizing, at its essence, means persuading an individual that something is related to national security. To have the authority to do so, however, a speaker needs to demonstrate one’s bona fides at providing security. There are different means of signaling these bona fides. The obvious example is that politicians may present themselves as hawks.

Another approach for signaling these bona fides is highlighting prior experience with the military. Indeed, the U.S. military’s immense public prestige relative to other institutions is a potent political tool when it comes to policy persuasion (Krebs, Ralston, and Rapport Reference Krebs, Ralston and Rapport2021). Golby, Feaver, and Dropp (Reference Golby, Feaver and Dropp2018) find that “senior military officers” can nudge respondents’ opinions on foreign military operations (both pro and con). Jost and Kertzer (Reference Jost and Kertzer2023, 15) find that the American “public is significantly more likely to defer to advisers with combat and (especially) military experience, even on non-military issues” (see also Motta, Ralston, and Spindel Reference Motta, Ralston and Spindel2021). Similarly, voters look favorably on veteran candidates over nonveterans (Hardy et al. Reference Hardy, Coker, Funk and Warner2019; Richardson Reference Richardson2022).

Can Women Securitize?

Hansen (Reference Hansen2000) highlights the Copenhagen School’s myopia on gender, observing that women’s voices in general are silenced due to their frequent exclusion from political power. Considerable evidence exists that leadership is generally associated with masculinity (Bauer Reference Bauer2020; Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2019) and that women are less likely to be assessed as “strong leaders” (Croco and Gartner Reference Croco and Gartner2014; Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2014). Issues like the military, counterterrorism, and security remain tied to men in politics (Dolan Reference Dolan2004; Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2011; Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022; Rosenwasser and Dean Reference Rosenwasser and Dean1989). Lawless (Reference Lawless2004, 479), for example, argues that an “atmosphere of war” interacts with voters’ perception about men’s competence at “legislating around issues of national security and military crises,” and Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister (Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022, 251) find that during a time of threat, “the public looks for masculine traits.” This work suggests that women politicians may not benefit when questions of national security arise.

Knowing this, women may anticipate that they will receive less support (Butler and Preece Reference Butler and Preece2016) and thus preemptively work to overcome any political handicap (Anzia and Berry Reference Anzia and Berry2011; Bauer Reference Bauer2020). This may mean deliberately sending masculine signals (Bauer Reference Bauer2017). One such signal is military bona fides (Hansen Reference Hansen2000; Huddy and Terkildsen Reference Huddy and Terkildsen1993; Meeks Reference Meeks2012). Swers (Reference Swers2007), for example, finds that women more frequently sponsor defense-related U.S. Senate bills. At the executive level, research suggests that women may have an incentive to send international military signals as a way of addressing questions of leadership (Schwartz and Blair Reference Schwartz and Blair2020). Perhaps most directly, Thomsen (Reference Thomsen2011) argues that women deliberately take on hawkish positions to counteract such public perceptions.

Sending these types of military cues may not be purely advantageous for women. Research points to a version of the “double bind”: women must send masculine signals to succeed, but in doing so may seem unlikeable to voters (Bauer Reference Bauer2017; Swers Reference Swers2007). Hawkish signals specifically lead women to be perceived as more capable in this area (Thomsen Reference Thomsen2011), but Bauer (Reference Bauer2017) finds that women who engage in counter-stereotypic behavior do face more backlash from the opposing party.

But while leadership is a trait that is militarized and gendered, security as an issue is itself gendered as well. If, as Anderson-Nilsson and Clayton (Reference Anderson-Nilsson and Clayton2021, 820) argue, people are most persuasive in “contexts that fit well with salient aspects of their identity,” a military bona fides cue may not be enough to overcome the idea that security and foreign policy are generally perceived as masculine (Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022). In other words, people may have a positive association with veterans that transcends gender, but that positivity may not translate to policy persuasion in a traditionally masculine issue area.

LEADERSHIP VERSUS AUTHORITY

To consider the role of gender in securitizing, we begin with two ideas: leadership, a trait evaluation, and authority, the ability to persuade on policy. Being perceived as someone who has leadership qualities can make one a more attractive political candidate, while being an authority can raise the salience of a political issue and mobilize the public to act upon it, “mak[ing] the audience tolerate extraordinary measures that otherwise would not have been acceptable” (Wæver Reference Wæver2011, 469). Demonstrating security bona fides may help women (and men) signal leadership capability. Yet existing scholarship is less clear whether the same bona fide signals can overcome gender gaps in authority. Footnote 3

Gender, Security, and Leadership

People often evaluate politicians across a variety of traits, chief among them leadership (Ditonto Reference Ditonto2017; Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2019). Indeed, one of the barriers women face in politics is that “there is a high level of incongruence between being female and being a leader” (Bauer Reference Bauer2020, 4). Women politicians are likely to gain more support when they send leadership cues (Bauer Reference Bauer2015; Reference Bauer2017; Huddy and Terkildsen Reference Huddy and Terkildsen1993). This suggests that women who send signals about their security bona fides will be evaluated as superior leaders and candidates relative to women who do not.

Sending signals about military bona fides can happen in different ways. Previous research, for example, has suggested that women may make hawkish statements to send clear security and leadership signals (e.g., Swers Reference Swers2007; Thomsen Reference Thomsen2011). Discussing prior military experience—for example, veteran status—would be another, likely more direct path to signaling military bona fides (Hardy et al. Reference Hardy, Coker, Funk and Warner2019).

Certainly, the link between veteran experience and gender is unlikely to be straightforward. Indeed, as Thomas and Hunter (Reference Thomas and Hunter2019, ix) write, “the very word ‘veteran’ calls to mind the image of a man, especially combat veteran.” Research suggests that women veterans may be especially “invisible” within American society and that people perceive that women veterans’ military experiences differ from those of men (Best, Hunter, and Thomas Reference Best, Hunter and Thomas2021). It is possible, then, that while veteran status is likely to increase leadership evaluations for both men and women (Hardy et al. Reference Hardy, Coker, Funk and Warner2019), the ultimate leadership evaluation may still be higher for men who are veterans. Indeed, this is the expectation we preregistered prior to fielding Study 2: being a veteran would generally increase leadership perceptions, but men who are veterans will be perceived as stronger leaders than women veterans.

Gender, Security, and Authority

Leadership, however, is one outcome; another is policy persuasion (authority). Research suggests that messages are more persuasive when the source is credible (Druckman Reference Druckman2004; Ryan Reference Ryan2013).Footnote 4 A source is viewed as credible when people believe that the source is both capable of and intends to carry out some behavior (Madsen Reference Madsen2019). Could military bona fides overcome these issues by signaling greater capability? Prior to fielding Study 2, we preregistered this idea: having bona fides would help a politician be persuasive on security—that is, have authority.

Yet persuasion also depends on other characteristics of the source (Chaiken and Maheswaran Reference Chaiken and Maheswaran1994; Druckman Reference Druckman2022). A source’s credibility, for example, may also depend on context, be it a policy area or times of heightened international threat. Given the association between security and masculinity, then, women may be disadvantaged in the security context (Schramm and Stark Reference Schramm and Stark2020). The gendered idea of the security state may be so pervasive that even when women manage, in Disraeli’s words, to climb to the top of the greasy pole, “international politics is such a thoroughly masculinized sphere of activity that women’s voices are considered inauthentic” (Tickner Reference Tickner1992, 4). Therefore, while military bona fides increase the ability to persuade, our preregistered expectation was that this again should lead to more success for men than women. Our initial expectations, then, were parallel for both leadership and authority.

EMPIRICAL STRATEGY

The intersection of gender and securitization suggests two outcomes. One is the role of gender and military bona fides in determining perceived leadership. The second is in determining authority—the ability to persuade on a security policy. We track the connection between these different factors using experiments (Druckman et al. Reference Druckman, Green, Kuklinski, Lupia, Druckman, Green, Kuklinski and Lupia2011; Hyde Reference Hyde2015).

We rely on trait evaluations to consider the extent to which a politician is perceived as a “leader.” This is a desired trait in elected officials which, while deliberately vague, contains the potential for masculine and militarized biases (e.g., Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022). Second, we consider measures of the candidate’s ability to shift policy preferences—that is, “authority.” We note that our goal is not to track whether a person will vote for a particular candidate. Rather, our goal is to consider whether a candidate can persuade people through a security statement. We test these ideas using two national samples of American adults.

Our first, preliminary study considered the intersection of gender and a securitizing statement. Participants in this study were randomly assigned to either a man or a woman candidate and saw either a hawkish or a neutral statement from the candidate. In this study, the hawkish statement serves as both a signal of military bona fides and the securitizing statement. The goal of this first study is to track the effect of the statement on our two key outcomes: leadership (measured through an evaluation of the candidate as a leader) and authority (measured as the extent to which the candidate persuaded the participant).

Among the first study’s limitations, the most important is that a single statement cues both bona fides and securitization. Therefore, we turn to a second, main study, which differs in several important ways. First, it tests a series of preregistered hypotheses. Second, it separately randomizes the military bona fides cue and the securitizing statement. Rather than relying on a hawkish statement to signal bona fides, we use a cue that is less partisan and that research suggests to be quite powerful: previous military experience (Golby, Feaver, and Dropp Reference Golby, Feaver and Dropp2018). Finally, given that people are more likely to follow a co-partisan’s cues (Berinsky Reference Berinsky2007) and that partisanship profoundly and simultaneously affects assessments of gender and security (Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2011), we address partisanship more directly in the second study.

Both studies share the same basic approach to balance between internal and external validity. We present the participants with a fictitious first-time candidate for the U.S. Congress, and gender is cued through candidate images, names, and pronouns.Footnote 5 In the United States, both constitutionally and in the popular perception, the President is by far the actor most likely to engage in securitizing. Other executive officials such as Secretary of Defense, and perhaps more visible legislators such as the Speaker of the House, may also engage in this behavior to some extent. However, presenting study participants a “generic” president or defense secretary poses several problems, not the least of which is that there never has been a woman in either position. Further, one’s attitude to well-known figures such as Nancy Pelosi or Mitch McConnell, much less Donald Trump or Joe Biden, is likely to be heavily pretreated (Druckman and Leeper Reference Druckman and Leeper2012). Therefore, to track the role of gender in this process, we rely on a design that allows us the most control with minimal pretreatment effects.

Study 1: Preliminary Results

Our first study randomized participants along three factors: (1) candidate gender, (2) “hawkish” or neutral statement, and (3) candidate party, producing a total of eight randomly assigned treatments (Figure 1). We focus on the relationship between the gender of the candidate and the statement. Specifically, we assess whether making the statement affects people’s perceptions of the candidates’ leadership characteristics and whether people will follow the policy in the statement. We have no a priori expectations about the role of partisanship but include partisanship as a factor to ensure external validity (i.e., people rarely encounter candidates for national office without also encountering their party) and to avoid confounds as people often assume that women are Democrats (Dolan Reference Dolan2005).

Figure 1. Experimental Tasks, Study 1

For this study, conducted via the Internet, participants saw a treatment as an article on a screen. Our participants ( $ N=1,\hskip-0.15em 029 $ ) were members of a national adult panel provided by Survey Sampling International (SSI; since re-branded as Dynata); SSI samples approximate the U.S. population.Footnote 6

Participants were given a sample news article about a generic candidate for Congress laying out their views on foreign policy. Within the text, depending on the treatment, the candidate is described as a “rising star” in the Democratic (Republican) Party, presented in the text and accompanying photo as a white man (woman), and gives a Defense (Neutral) statement.Footnote 7 In this study, the treatment includes both a statement of threat and of a need for security; we therefore interpret the results in light of both.Footnote 8

We first consider whether assessments of politicians’ leadership change when they invoke defense. To consider leadership, we asked participants to rate how well they believe the politician in the treatment fits the description “strong leader” on a seven-point scale, where a score of 1 means that the term does not describe the politician well at all, to 7, which means the term describes them very well. Table 1 presents the results by partisanship/gender of the politician, as well as by gender only (treating partisanship as a nuisance factor given randomization). We see that men are rated more positively than women, but mentioning threat and defense increases women’s ratings—but has no effect for men.Footnote 9

Table 1. Change in Believing Candidate Is a “Strong Leader,” Study 1

Note: Each comparison is a two-tailed t-test. Outcome measure: mean belief that a politician is a strong leader, on a 1–7 scale. $ N=1,\hskip-0.10em 009 $ , but more participants were randomized to the conditions with women.

We also asked “What is the most important issue of the day?” Participants were asked to select just one from a set of issues, and we present these results graphically in Figure 2.Footnote 10

Figure 2. Defense Importance by Candidate Treatment, Study 1

Note: OLS coefficients shown in SI D.1. Republican women: difference = 0.037 ( $ p=0.31 $ ); Democratic women: difference= $ -0.003 $ ( $ p=0.94 $ ). Women overall (both parties): difference = 0.010 ( $ p=0.70 $ ). Republican men: difference = 0.151 ( $ p=0.02 $ ); Democratic men: difference = 0.093 ( $ p=0.08 $ ). Men overall (both parties): difference = 0.12 ( $ p=0.004 $ ).

As a check, we find that there are no differences in the percentage of participants who list defense as an important issue in the neutral conditions. Participants who are randomly assigned to the neutral treatment report that defense is important at equal rates regardless of the gender of the candidate ( $ p=0.69 $ ).Footnote 11 We next consider how participants in the defense treatments differed from those in the neutral treatments.

First, we see that women have little influence on perceptions of defense as an important issue. When the candidate is a Democratic woman, the treatment has no effect on whether participants find defense to be an important issue. In contrast, where the politician is a man, mentions of threat/defense increase the belief that defense is an important issue (Figure 2).Footnote 12

Study 1 suggests that when women send a bona fide signal—through invoking threat/defense—they are perceived as stronger leaders. On the other hand, women are less likely to be able to “securitize” with that very same statement.

Study 2: Experimental Design

Our first, preliminary, study demonstrates a leadership benefit to military bona fides, but does not suggest a parallel one for authority. Yet it is possible that this difference is due to the study’s considerable limitations. Thus, we turn to a second study, this article’s focal experiment. While Study 1 combined the securitization statement and defense bona fides as a single treatment, Study 2 ( $ N=1,\hskip-0.15em 665 $ , recruited through Research Now Survey Sampling International (RNSSI) sample statistics in SI E.2) distinguishes a candidate’s securitization statement and the bona fide cue as separate treatments.Footnote 13 We use veteran status as it has been suggested as a powerful cue about military bona fides for political candidates (McDermott and Panagopoulos Reference McDermott and Panagopoulos2015; Richardson Reference Richardson2022).

A second difference between the preliminary and focal studies is how we address political party. Since partisanship remains a crucial factor in participants’ perception of candidates, but balancing the power and parsimony of our study, we hold party constant by using a primary election context and sorting participants by their party identification at the outset. Therefore, participants who identify as Republicans (strong, weak, and leaning) see a Republican primary, while those who identify as Democrats are assigned to a Democratic primary. Those who identify as “pure” Independents, and people who did not give a party, were randomly assigned to either a Republican or Democratic primary.Footnote 14

We do this for several reasons. First, defense has generally been considered a Republican-owned issue (Petrocik Reference Petrocik1996). As a result, if we ignored party entirely, a participant might assume that a politician speaking about defense was a Republican. This assumption would then muddy the comparison between the defense and neutral conditions. We avoid this potential for bias by deliberately clarifying partisanship in all conditions. Second, not only does partisanship sometimes overwhelm gender effects (Bauer Reference Bauer2018; King and Matland Reference King and Matland2003) but people often assume that a woman candidate is a Democrat (Dolan Reference Dolan2005). Finally, asking participants to rate only in-party candidates offers a more externally valid approach. Over the last decade, Americans have become highly unlikely to follow cues from the opposing party (Druckman et al. Reference Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov, Levendusky and Ryan2021). As a result, the authority to set the agenda—of the type we theorize here—likely happens within party.

Study 2 randomized participants along three factors: (1) gender, (2) combat veteran status (e.g., military bona fides), and (3) securitization speech act. The gender cue in this study is identical to that of the first. We cued combat veteran status within the treatment by telling a randomly selected group of participants that “[Candidate name] is a combat veteran.”Footnote 15

Our final factor randomizes whether the participant received a statement from the candidate. In cases where the participant is randomly assigned to receive a statement, the candidate’s securitization statement draws attention to a need to “increase our security” and proposing a policy. We cued international competition by mentioning three countries widely viewed as adversaries by most Americans: China, Russia, and Iran. In Study 1, the defense treatment had every candidate make an explicit hawkish statement to signal bona fides. Here, our goal was to have a statement that was not hawkish as much as focused on connecting a policy idea to national security—that is, securitizing. Moreover, to further measure the ability of security discourse to shift “normal” political preferences, we deliberately made the statement run counter to the relative preference most partisans place on defense and infrastructure spending. Thus, for Democrats (and randomly selected Independents), the securitization treatment consisted of the following statement:

The candidate released the following statement on the federal budget: “shifting resources away from infrastructure and towards defense will grow the economy by creating 120,000 additional jobs and increase our security by allowing us to better compete internationally against China, Russia, and Iran.”

This represents a clear securitization move—a budgetary decision framed through national security. The Republican treatment is an even tougher test; it uses the language of security to get respondents to change a core Republican belief and shift resources away from defense. Republican participants (and randomly assigned Independents) received the following treatment (italics added here to emphasize the key partisan difference):

The candidate released the following statement on the federal budget: “shifting resources away from defense and towards infrastructure will grow the economy by creating 120,000 additional jobs and increase our security by allowing us to better compete internationally against China, Russia, and Iran.”

Clearly, while both statements have a securitization element, there is a substantive difference in policy goals. Therefore, to ensure that this does not produce additional confounds—for example, if one statement seems more unexpected given the source (e.g., Baum and Groeling Reference Baum and Groeling2009) or if response to this unexpectedness depends on source gender (e.g., Vraga Reference Vraga2017)—we conducted additional post hoc checks (SI E.4).Footnote 16 We do not observe evidence of this type of confound. We present the full set of experimental tasks in Figure 3.Footnote 17

Figure 3. Experimental Tasks, Study 2

Study 2: Preregistered Hypotheses

Building on our first study, the design of Study 2 leads us to a set of preregistered expectations.Footnote 18 We noted these expectations earlier in the manuscript, but specify the hypotheses here as well.Footnote 19

Hypothesis 1 (leadership): Candidates (across both genders) who are combat veterans are perceived as more likely to be strong leaders relative to candidates who are not combat veterans.

Next, we follow our theoretic arguments, which suggest that while being a veteran is generally beneficial for all candidates, the advantage may differ based on gender:

Hypothesis 2 (leadership): Men who are combat veterans will be perceived as stronger leaders than women candidates who are combat veterans.

In other words, we expect that women will benefit from military bona fides (H1) relative to women who cannot signal bona fides, but we acknowledge the possibility that the benefit may not be as strong as it is for men (H2).

Our next two preregistered hypotheses address candidates’ ability to shape respondent policy preferences—their authority—via their statements. While H3 addresses the basic premise that military bona fides increase authority, H4 is the intersection of gender, military bona fides and authority.

Hypothesis 3 (authority): Candidates (across both genders) who are combat veterans will be more successful at changing policy preferences than those who are not combat veterans.

Hypothesis 4 (authority): Men who are combat veterans will be more successful at changing policy preferences than women candidates who are combat veterans.

STUDY 2: RESULTS

We first consider how participants evaluate politicians’ leadership, and whether these evaluations are affected by a politician’s gender and veteran status (preregistered H1 and H2). Second, we consider how gender and military bona fides shape authority with a securitizing statement (preregistered H3 and H4).

We note that our experimental design produces more potential comparisons than the key hypotheses we preregistered. Given the possibility of these other comparisons (e.g., we can test the way making a statement affects perceptions of leadership, though we have not preregistered any hypotheses or analyses to this end), we conduct additional exploratory analyses which consider all possible interactions of our experimental factors. We also include the means across all variables by condition in SI F.1. Given our specification of treatment effects as interactions, we present the results as marginal effects of a shift in one of our treatments.

Candidate Leadership

We begin by considering perceptions of the candidate’s leadership. As with Study 1, our measure is a rating of how well the politician in the treatment fits the description “strong leader” on a seven-point scale, where 1 means that the term strong leader does not describe the politician well at all, and 7 means that the term describes them very well. We also asked about favorability and qualification for office with results shown in SI E.5. In addition to these direct measures of leadership, we also ask participants two measures as checks: perceptions of knowledge on defense and on the economy. We use these to ensure that veteran status is leading people to focus on defense; results are shown in SI E.6. To give a sense of the baseline across the entire sample, while the male candidate has a higher mean leadership evaluation than the woman, the difference is not significant (0.07, $ p=0.32 $ , two-tailed).

H1 predicted that being described as a combat veteran should be beneficial for both men and women.Footnote 20 As this hypothesis does not specify any particular role for our third experimental factor—the candidate’s statement—we follow previous research and treat it as a nuisance factor in our first set of analyses, a benefit of randomization (e.g., Druckman et al. Reference Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov and Ryan2022).Footnote 21

Figure 4a shows the marginal effect of the candidate being a combat veteran; Figure 4b presents the mean leadership values. The figure demonstrates that for both men and women, the effect of being a combat vet is always positive, though it is only significant for women. As SI E.5 shows, these effects are robust across other measures of candidate traits and to controlling for demographic pretreatment measures. Overall, the results in Figure 4 suggest that a candidate is better off being a combat veteran than not in terms of leadership perceptions regardless of gender.

Figure 4. Study 2, Candidate Leadership due to Veteran Status for Men and Women Candidates

Note: Left panel: marginal effects of veteran status by candidate gender; coefficient estimates (OLS) are in SI E.5. Positive effects mean that veteran status increases perceived candidate quality; negative mean a decrease; 95% confidence intervals and one-tailed p-values (following hypothesized directional predictions), seven-point scale. Right panel: mean leadership by gender and veteran status.

Figure 5. Study 2, Gender Differences in Perceived Candidate Leadership for Veteran and Nonveteran Candidates

Note: Marginal effects of gender by candidate veteran status; coefficient estimates (OLS) are in SI E.5. Positive effects mean that women are perceived more positively than men; negative mean that men are perceived more positively than women; 95% confidence intervals, seven-point scale.

We also hypothesized that men who are combat veterans will have higher leadership evaluations than women who are combat veterans. As the results in Figure 4 hint, we do not see evidence to this point—contrary to our preregistration. Indeed, there is almost no difference between leadership evaluations of men and women who are veterans.Footnote 22

In summary, we see two patterns. First, study participants give higher leadership ratings to veterans; they also rate veterans more positively on other trait dimensions. Moreover, the results on our check measures—SI E.6 —show shifts in perceptions of defense knowledge but null effects for perceptions of economic knowledge, suggesting that being described as a veteran does bring to mind defense and security. This reinforces the idea that veteran status signals military bona fides.

We find no evidence in support of our second preregistered hypothesis: there are no differences in participants’ ratings of men and women veterans. Women veterans are just as likely as their male counterparts to be viewed as “strong leaders.” In a research question posed in our preregistration, we asked about the possible gender differences in the effect of being described as a veteran. The boost from being a veteran is significant only for women, although the confidence intervals overlap.

Candidate Authority

Our next step is to consider whether military bona fides also influence authority. We therefore turn to our second set of analyses that track the effect of the securitization statement on people’s posttreatment issue preferences.Footnote 23

As our securitization statement either advocated for security through infrastructure or security through defense, we measure the extent to which a candidate is an authority by presenting the participant with a zero-sum choice between these two priorities (Caverley and Krupnikov Reference Caverley and Krupnikov2017). In our analyses, we code participants who select the option that matches the statement advocated by the candidate they were assigned to see in the treatment as 1, and those who do not as 0. An additional measure—the perceived importance of spending on defense versus infrastructure—was also included; we note the results in text, and present them in full in SI E.7.

We first test whether the statement is more effective when made by someone with military bona fides. Here, results in line with H3 would show that people are more likely to follow the statement when that candidate is a veteran. We then test whether there are gender differences in the extent to which military bona fides help with this authority. Here, we consider how the intersection of gender and veteran status influence candidates have the most authority, our preregistered H4.

Veteran Status and Authority

We begin by considering whether veteran candidates have greater authority when making statements about national security. Here, we focus on the marginal effect of making a statement on support for the statement’s position, differentiating between candidates who are veterans and those who are not. For now, we do not consider gender—we will address the intersection of veteran status and gender more explicitly later on. We see little evidence that people are more likely to follow the position of a candidate who is a veteran rather than one who is not—contrary to preregistered expectations.

If participants view a candidate as authorized to speak about security, then participants who read the candidate’s statement should be more likely to adopt the espoused position relative to those who were not exposed to the statement. A signal of authority would be a positive, statistically significant marginal effect of the candidate’s statement. As we show in the top panel of Figure 6, we do see that exposure to the statement has a positive marginal effect on support, but only when the candidate is not a veteran. While for nonveteran candidates the marginal effect of the statement is both positive and significant ( $ M=0.08,p=0.015 $ ), for veterans the effect of the statement is null ( $ M=0.03,p=0.435). $ This is somewhat unexpected given previous research, as well as the fact that respondents regard veterans as more knowledgeable on defense issues (SI E.6).

Figure 6. Statement on Policy Support, Veteran Status, Study 2

Note: Left panel: marginal effects of making a statement on support for the position in the statement by veteran status. Positive marginal effects mean that exposure to the statement increases support for the position in the statement. Right panel: predicted probabilities of supporting the position in the statement. Both figures show 95% confidence intervals. Full coefficients from the OLS model which produced these estimates as well as a logit model showing similar results are in SI E.7.

A possible explanation for these results could be that participants in the veteran conditions are already primed by the candidate’s status as a combat veteran, thereby undermining the effectiveness of the statement. We address this potential alternative explanation by considering mean support for the policy in the statement by condition (Figure 6b). Some evidence of this alternative explanation would be that people assigned to veteran conditions generally differ regardless of exposure to the statement.

We see no evidence for this alternative explanation. First, when we compare participants not exposed to a statement, we see that exposure to the veteran treatment shows no significant differences relative to those seeing the nonveteran.Footnote 24 Indeed, the greatest differences emerge in response to the statement. While the statement has a strong effect on policy support when it is made by a nonveteran candidate, it has little effect when made by a veteran.Footnote 25

Gender and Authority

Our next step is an exploratory analysis (which was not part of our preregistration) considering the relationship between authority and gender. First, we note that in the conditions without a statement, there are no differences in policy preferences between participants who see a woman and those who see a man.Footnote 26 Relative to a condition with no statement, making a statement has no effect on participants’ policy preferences when the candidate is a woman ( $ M=0.003 $ , $ p=0.941 $ ), but when the candidate is a man, making a statement produces a significant increase ( $ M=0.11 $ , $ p=0.002 $ ) in support for the position in the statement (Figure 7a).

Figure 7. Effect of Statement on Policy Support, Gender, Study 2

Note: Left panel: marginal effects of making a statement on support for the position in the statement by gender, treating veteran status as a nuisance factor. Positive marginal effects mean that exposure to the statement increases support for the position in the statement. Right panel: mean supporting the position in the statement. Both figures show 95% confidence intervals. Full coefficients from the OLS model which produced these estimates as well as a logit model showing similar results are in SI E.7.

We can consider these patterns in a different way. In Figure 7b, we present the probability of supporting the position in the statement by condition. The highest probability support is in the condition where a man has made the statement. This level of support is different from the level of support offered when a woman has made the statement at $ p=0.039 $ . As we noted earlier, there are no gender differences in level of support when no statement is made.

Gender, Veteran Status, and Authority

To this point, we consider levels of authority by both candidate veteran status and candidate gender. Contrary to our preregistered hypothesis, we do not find that candidates who are veterans have more authority. We now turn to the intersection of veteran status and gender.

In our preregistered hypothesis, we stated the expectation that men who are veterans should have the highest authority relative to other types of candidates. In our final set of results, we see no evidence to this point.

We present our final set of results (Figure 8) as support for the position in the securitizing statement, by condition. Given that we have eight conditions, for ease of presentation, we depict the results for Men in Figure 8a and Women in Figure 8b. First, we see that (contrary to our hypothesis) candidates who are not combat veterans appear to have more authority. This is true for both men and women. Men who make securitizing statements are not significantly more likely to move people’s positions—and the results are not consistent.Footnote 27 Moreover, we do not see that men achieve significantly higher authority when making statements.Footnote 28 In sum, military bona fides—here in the form of veteran status—do not appear to increase authority for any politician.

Figure 8. Statement and Policy Support, Gender and Veteran Status, Study 2

Note: Figures show support for position in the statement, 95% confidence intervals. Full coefficients from the OLS model which produced these estimates are in SI E.7.

Using our additional spending measure we also do not see any evidence of the power of bona fides (SI Table E.15). Moreover, we also see that the statement has null effects regardless of candidate gender—another suggestion of limited findings in authority.

Additional Analyses and Alternative Explanations

In our preregistration, we consider the possibility that different types of participants may respond differently to our treatments. In the main text, we discuss two covariates: partisanship and gender.Footnote 29 We address partisanship because election patterns suggest Democratic participants treat a woman candidate as a more routine state of affairs than our Republican participants.Footnote 30 Moreover, people may perceive the securitization statement differently depending on the gender and partisanship of the candidate (e.g., Sanbonmatsu and Dolan Reference Sanbonmatsu and Dolan2009).Footnote 31 We consider participants’ gender as there is research to suggest people are more likely to support candidates who share their identity (Dolan Reference Dolan1998). We preregistered no a priori expectations and treat these analyses of subgroups as exploratory efforts.

We present the full exploratory results in SI F.3. In this section, we concentrate on party, because in order to pull respondents from their preferred position with the candidate’s statement, the treatments differed based on the respondents’ party identification (i.e., the candidates asked fellow Republicans to shift spending from defense to infrastructure in response to a threat to the United States). To do so, we consider an interaction between our three factors (Candidate Gender, Veteran Status, and Statement), but estimate separate models by party. We present the results in SI Table F.2, only reporting the substantive effects here.

Among Democratic participants, the effects are null. The effect of the statement for nonveteran women is $ 0.098 $ ( $ p=0.167 $ ); for the veteran women, the effect of the statement is $ -0.047 $ ( $ p=0.511 $ ). For nonveteran men, the change is $ 0.107 $ ( $ p=0.123) $ , and for veteran men, the change is $ 0.109 $ ( $ p=0.126 $ ). Among Republican participants, regardless of veteran status, women candidates’ statements produce an effect that is not statistically significant. Both types of male candidates increase support for infrastructure over defense, but the effect is only significant for nonveterans ( $ 0.168 $ , $ p=0.031 $ ).Footnote 32

Summing up this exploratory section, even when we limited the analysis to the partisan identity, we assume to be most receptive to a given treatment (i.e., Democrats and women, Republicans and veterans), we see the same effects: veterans have no advantage in changing policy preferences in the name of security.

WHO MAKES 3 A.M. PHONE CALLS?

Before discussing the implications of the article, we want to reinforce the findings. In an experiment that separately manipulated military bona fides and the securitizing statement, we see that military bona fides—communicated through veteran status—did boost perceptions of women as leaders; there is an increase for men as well, though it does not reach significance. Contrary to our preregistered expectations, however, we see no difference in leadership ratings between men and women who are combat veterans. Moreover, contrary to our preregistered expectations, we do not see any evidence that being a veteran is especially helpful in increasing the authority to securitize. If women are disadvantaged when it comes to having authority in security spaces (Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022), our results suggest that military bona fides may not be the ticket to obtaining that authority. We see the same pattern, however, for men as well.

Our preregistered expectations were parallel patterns in leadership and authority, yet it is important to note that these are two distinct outcomes. Indeed, it is likely that the policy persuasion at the heart of authority is a much higher barrier—after all, persuading a person to follow a politician’s policy position may be more challenging than changing their belief that a politician could be a good leader (e.g., Druckman Reference Druckman2022). Given the importance of authority in theories of securitization, further exploring this distinction between leadership and authority as an a priori expectation is key for understanding who can persuade in a security context.

Our results also carry implications for future work on veteran status. Research suggests the possibility that veteran status is, in itself, gendered (Thomas and Hunter Reference Thomas and Hunter2019) and that veteran status is perceived differently for men and women (e.g., Best, Hunter, and Thomas Reference Best, Hunter and Thomas2021). In our study, veteran status did not seem to increase authority for women, yet the same was also true for men. This is an unexpected result given existing theories on the role of military bona fides and security in shaping public opinion. Yet while we focus on veterans, the experimental literature on security policy persuasion operationalizes military bona fides in other ways: “soldier,” “junior/senior officer,” “military general/retired general/retired military officer,” and “Chairman of the Joint Chiefs of Staff (CJCS) and the regional combatant commander” (Golby, Feaver, and Dropp Reference Golby, Feaver and Dropp2018; Jost and Kertzer Reference Jost and Kertzer2023; Motta, Ralston, and Spindel Reference Motta, Ralston and Spindel2021; Tomz, Weeks, and Yarhi-Milo Reference Tomz, Weeks and Yarhi-Milo2020, respectively). Our results, then, do conform with recent empirical work showing a more complicated, contingent relationship between veteran status and politics (Richardson Reference Richardson2022). Jointly, these patterns point to the possibility that there may be more nuance to both bona fides and authority—a fruitful consideration for future research.

In bringing together discussions of security, veteran status, and gender, this article synthesizes three scholarly approaches. First, in the American political context, we consider the constraints facing women who work to influence the political agenda. Engaging with feminist theories, we consider how masculinity and security are inherent to the public’s conception of the state and its leadership. For the Copenhagen School, the consideration of gender and veteran status in our studies speaks to the importance of focusing as much on the speaker and audience as on the speech act itself. Moreover, we suggest experiments as a useful means of addressing important aspects of securitization (and feminist) theory (Baele and Thomson Reference Baele and Thomson2017). To date, empirical work in this field has largely excluded such an approach (Baele and Jalea Reference Baele and Jalea2022; Balzacq Reference Balzacq and Balzacq2011), leading critics within the Copenhagen School to worry about its methodological “stagnation,” especially its tendency to focus on successful securitization outcomes (Potenz Reference Potenz2019).Footnote 33 An experimental approach may help to empirically disaggregate the securitization speech act from the policy itself, a central Copenhagen School insight.

It is also important to acknowledge the constraints of our findings. Experiments must always balance external and internal validity, and we acknowledge that security may not be the foremost factor in evaluating congressional candidates. The primary “securitizer” in the United States is the very recognizable (and historically male) occupant of the Oval Office. Perhaps when a woman achieves this position in the United States, the halo of being formally recognized as an authority figure may shift how we consider securitization. Still, as recent research outside the American context suggests, even when a woman holds an executive office, she may still be limited during times of security threats (e.g., Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022). Nonetheless, we note that this article confines itself to the American case, and the effect we identify may not be universal given variations in threat perception, partisanship, and gender norms around the world.

Future research should also examine how the ideas of leadership and authority affect those who are already in office. The ideas we raise could have implications for political diversion “rally ‘round the flag” attempts (Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022) as well as pursuits of conciliatory policies with international rivals (Mattes and Weeks Reference Mattes and Weeks2019). Many IR theories place tremendous weight on the importance to international politics of statements of resolve to domestic audiences (Kertzer Reference Kertzer2016; McManus Reference McManus2017)—what happens when the characteristics of the leader affect the extent to which the public is willing to follow these statements?

Finally, it is also possible that salience of statements on other international issues may be associated with gender in different directions than presented in this article. Women might be perceived as having more standing to securitize issues such as human trafficking, the environment, or global pandemics.Footnote 34 Hillary Clinton, for example, left her position as Secretary of State with both a reputation as a forceful advocate on international “women’s issues” as well as one of Obama’s more hawkish advisers. Perhaps women may be more effective than men at mobilizing extraordinary, militarized political responses to human trafficking, for example. Yet if this is the case, feminist theory suggests this to be part of the problem.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055424000182.

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

Research documentation and data that support the findings of this study are openly available at the American Political Science Review Dataverse: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/OEHBYH.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors would like to thank Nichole Bauer, Lene Hansen, Michael Kenwick, Megan MacKenzie, Oriana Skyler Mastro, Michelle Murray, Sarah Sewell, and Rachel Stein for their essential comments.

FUNDING STATEMENT

This research was funded by a grant from the Kellogg Dispute Resolution Center.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no ethical issues or conflicts of interest in this research.

ETHICAL STANDARDS

The authors declare the human subjects research in this article was reviewed and approved by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology Committee on the Use of Humans as Experimental Subjects and Stony Brook Office of Research Compliance. Certificates are available in the Dataverse. The authors affirm that this article adheres to the principles concerning research with human participants laid out in APSA’s Principles and Guidance on Human Subject Research (2020).

Footnotes

1 Veterans make up 17% of the 117th Congress compared to approximately 7% of the public. While the 117th has a record number of women, they occupy 27% of its seats.

2 Our preregistration for Study 2 is here: http://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=mw6ir9, as well as in the Supplementary Information (SI) G, which also lists deviations from the preregistration.

3 We are grateful for a review process that helped us to better conceptualize and communicate the difference between “leadership” and “authority”; we are also grateful for the reviewer suggestions about additional scholarship on these ideas.

4 The large amount of IR work on credibility focuses on the interactions between states, such as whether one state actor believes the coercive threats of another (Jervis, Yarhi-Milo, and Casler Reference Jervis, Yarhi-Milo and Casler2021). That said, almost all of the empirical work on interstate credibility focuses on a leader’s ability to generate domestic “audience costs” (Fearon Reference Fearon1994), often through survey experiments (Kertzer Reference Kertzer2016; Tomz, Weeks, and Yarhi-Milo Reference Tomz, Weeks and Yarhi-Milo2020).

5 The images used were pretested with a different sample to ensure that both candidates appeared of similar attractiveness and temperament (see SI C). We also conducted two post hoc checks with a different sample to ensure that the images did not produce differential nationalism and patriotism responses by the gender of the candidate (SI C.2).

6 Note, we requested 1,200 completed responses from SSI but were left with 1,031 who were assigned to treatment and completed at least one response. In the programming of the study, an error led to uneven randomization—more participants were assigned to the conditions with women; we consider this issue through additional analyses in SI D.1.2 and find no evidence that it affected randomization. Still, 75% of our participants were assigned to the conditions with the woman candidate, with a higher likelihood of ending up in the Democratic conditions. We address this issue by conducting additional analyses treating candidate partisanship as a nuisance factor—since we have no a priori expectations—which increases the N per condition. Full results are in SI D.1.4 .

7 The full treatment texts are posted alongside our data in the APSR Dataverse (Caverley and Krupnikov Reference Caverley and Krupnikov2024).

8 Given that our treatment manipulated defense using the headline and lead paragraph, it is possible that people who read more carefully to the end formed a different impression of the treatment. We address this in SI D.1.1 and find no evidence to this point.

9 We see similar patterns on two additional outcomes: favorability and vote likelihood, shown in SI Table D.5 .

10 Full set of issues posted along with the data.

11 As another check, we can consider which issue participants in the neutral treatments find most important. Across all four neutral treatments, the modal issue selected is the budget deficit.

12 As an additional check, we consider whether participants saw a candidate of their own party or of the other party (see SI D.1).

13 RNSSI recruited a nationally balanced, non-probability sample of adults; the company has since rebranded as Dynata.

14 In our sample, 15.2% of participants identified as “pure” independents. This is in line with the 2016 American National Election Study in which 14.7% of respondents were pure independents (weighted). Seven participants did not give a party but were assigned to a treatment.

15 We pretested whether there was a difference between “combat veteran” and “veteran” in separate study ( $ N=149 $ , MTurk; if we account for duplicate location identifiers, $ N=137 $ ). About half of the participants informed that a male candidate was a “veteran” assumed he had been in combat. We therefore explicitly use the term “combat veteran” to ensure that there is no confounding.

16 There are different considerations of unexpectedness. One speaks to the gendered components. There is research to suggest that people may assume that women regardless of party should be less likely to suggest increasing defense (Sanbonmatsu and Dolan Reference Sanbonmatsu and Dolan2009). We address this idea of typicality in SI E.4.1. The other is expectations of the party—is it more unexpected for the Democrats to argue for increasing defense, for example, than for Republicans to argue for increasing infrastructure? We address this idea in SI E.4.1 .

17 The full treatments and question-wording is posted along with our data.

18 Anonymized preregistration here: http://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=mw6ir9 and in the SI.

19 While in the hypotheses we focus on leadership, we analyzed other traits which were included in our preregistration: measures of favorability, and perceived knowledge on the issue of defense. These measures reflect previous approaches to capturing traits in a gendered context (e.g., Dolan and Lynch Reference Dolan and Lynch2018). These additional analyses are in SI E.5 .

20 The intersection of gender and veteran status raises another question: do people perceive men and women veterans differently? It is possible, e.g., that people may assume that women would have been less likely to experience combat. We address this idea with an additional study, shown in SI E.4.3. We find that perceptions of valor are not conditional on gender, but do depend on partisanship. The partisan pattern does not affect our results given the design of Study 2.

21 We present exploratory analyses that do consider the effect of the statement in SI F .

22 The difference between men and women who are veterans is $ -0.01 $ on the leadership scale, $ p=0.89 $ (see Figure 5).

23 In these analyses, we consider positions that rely on ideology, which means we could control for participant ideology. We also randomly assigned independents to a party. Our results are robust to the inclusion and exclusion of these controls; the results in the text do not include controls and models with controls are in SI E.7 .

24 The difference between groups is $ -0.05 $ , $ p=0.177 $ .

25 The difference in support of defense (regardless of whether defense is in the statement) between veteran and nonveteran conditions is $ 0.02 $ , $ p=0.523 $ using a two-tailed t-test.

26 The difference is $ 0.042 $ , $ p=0.222 $ .

27 The difference between participants assigned to a woman, nonveteran, who has not made a statement and a man, nonveteran, who has not made a statement is $ 0.039 $ , $ p=0.422 $ . The difference between participants assigned to a woman veteran who has not made a statement and a man veteran who has not made a statement is 0.045, $ p=0.346 $ .

28 Null results, however, often raise questions of power. We consider power in SI E.1.

29 We preregistered additional subgroup checks that we include in SI F .

30 At the time of this study, nearly 40% of Democratic Congress members were women, compared to 8% of Republican Congress members.

31 We also address this idea in SI E.4 .

32 Reported p-values are two-tailed.

33 While Wibben (Reference Wibben2011, 592) writes “It is, above all, feminists’ methodological commitments that distinguish Feminist Security Studies from other approaches,” methods need not have an exclusive relationship to a given philosophical position (Aradau and Huysmans Reference Aradau and Huysmans2014, 3; Friedrichs and Kratochwil Reference Friedrichs and Kratochwil2009). Jackson (Reference Jackson2011, 101–2) finds “laboratory experiments” to be a more appropriate method for “critical realist” approaches to social life than for what he calls “neopositivism.” We argue that experiments are particularly well suited to securitization research, much of which makes explicit, causal claims from a non-positivist and non-rationalist standpoint (Guzzini Reference Guzzini2011). Reiter (Reference Reiter2015) reviews a massive amount of “positivist” work on gender, and also makes the larger point that sociological theories are amenable to positivist testing.

34 Indeed, other research on military bona fides and security has shown their role in nondefense issues (Motta, Ralston, and Spindel Reference Motta, Ralston and Spindel2021).

References

REFERENCES

Anderson-Nilsson, Georgia, and Clayton, Amanda. 2021. “Gender and Policy Persuasion.” Political Science Research and Methods 9 (4): 818–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anzia, Sarah, and Berry, Christopher. 2011. “The Jackie (and Jill) Robinson Effect: Why Do Congresswomen Outperform Congressmen?American Journal of Political Science 55 (3): 478–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aradau, Claudia, and Huysmans, Jef. 2014. “Critical Methods in International Relations: The Politics of Techniques, Devices and Acts.” European Journal of International Relations 20 (3): 596619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, Mary Layton, and Windett, Jason Harold. 2019. “Gender Stereotypes and the Policy Priorities of Women in Congress.” Political Behavior 41: 769–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baele, Stephane J., and Jalea, Diana. 2022. “Twenty-Five Years of Securitization Theory: A Corpus-Based Review.” Political Studies Review 21 (2): 376–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baele, Stephane J., and Thomson, Catarina. 2017. “An Experimental Agenda for Securitization Theory.” International Studies Review 19 (4): 646–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balzacq, Thierry. 2005. “The Three Faces of Securitization: Political Agency, Audience and Context.” European Journal of International Relations 11 (2): 171201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balzacq, Thierry. 2011. “Enquiries into Methods: A New Framework for Securitization Analysis.” In Securitization Theory: How Security Problems Emerge and Dissolve, ed. Balzacq, Thierry, 3153. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bauer, Nichole. 2020. The Qualifications Gap Why Women Must Be Better Than Men to Win Political Office. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nichole M. 2015. “Emotional, Sensitive, and Unfit for Office? Gender Stereotype Activation and Support Female Candidates.” Political Psychology 36 (6): 691708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nichole M. 2017. “The Effects of Counterstereotypic Gender Strategies on Candidate Evaluations.” Political Psychology 28 (2): 279–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nichole M. 2018. “Untangling the Relationship between Partisanship, Gender Stereotypes, and Support for Female Candidates.” Journal of Women, Politics and Policy 39 (1): 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baum, Matthew A., and Groeling, Tim. 2009. “Shot by the Messenger: Partisan Cues and Public Opinion Regarding National Security and War.” Political Behavior 31: 157–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berinsky, Adam J. 2007. “Assuming the Costs of War: Events, Elites, and American Public Support for Military Conflict.” Journal of Politics 69 (4): 975–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Best, Rebecca H., Hunter, Kyleanne, and Thomas, Katherine Hendricks. 2021. “Fighting for a Seat at the Table: Women’s Military Service and Political Representation.” Journal of Veterans Studies 7 (2): 1933.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialik, Kristen. 2017. “The Changing Face of America’s Veteran Population.” Technical Report, Pew Research Center. http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/11/10/the-changing-face-of-americas-veteran-population/.Google Scholar
Butler, Daniel, and Preece, Jessica. 2016. “Recruitment and Perceptions of Gender Bias in Party Leader Support.” Political Research Quarterly 69 (4): 842–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buzan, Barry, Wæver, Ole, and de Wilde, Jaap. 1998. Security: A New Framework for Analysis. Boulder, CO: Lynne Rienner.Google Scholar
Caverley, Jonathan D., and Krupnikov, Yanna. 2017. “Aiming at Doves: Experimental Evidence of Military Images’ Political Effects.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 61 (7): 1482–509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caverley, Jonathan D., and Krupnikov, Yanna. 2024. “Replication Data for: Leaders but Not Authorities? Gender, Veterans, and Messages about National Security.” Harvard Dataverse. Dataset. https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/OEHBYH.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaiken, Shelly, and Maheswaran, Durairaj. 1994. “Heuristic Processing Can Bias Systematic Processing: Effects of Source Credibility, Argument Ambiguity, and Task Importance on Attitude Judgment.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66 (3): 460–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croco, Sarah E., and Gartner, Scott S.. 2014. “Flip-Flops and High Heels: An Experimental Analysis of Elite Position Change and Gender on Wartime Public Support.” International Interactions 40 (1): 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ditonto, Tessa. 2017. “A High Bar or a Double Standard? Gender, Competence, and Information in Political Campaigns.” Political Behavior 39: 301–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, Kathleen. 1998. “Voting for Women in the ‘Year of the Woman’.” American Journal of Political Science 42 (1): 272–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, Kathleen. 2004. Voting for Women: How the Public Evaluates Women Candidates. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Dolan, Kathleen. 2005. “Do Women Candidates Play to Gender Stereotypes? Do Men Candidates Play to Women? Candidate Sex and Issues Priorities on Campaign Websites.” Political Research Quarterly 58 (1): 3144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, Kathleen, and Lynch, Timothy. 2018. “The Impact of Gender Stereotypes on Voting for Women Candidates by Level and Type of Office.” Politics & Gender 12 (3): 573–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James, Klar, Samara, Krupnikov, Yanna, Levendusky, Matthew, and Ryan, John. 2021. “Affective Polarization, Local Contexts and Public Opinion in America.” Nature Human Behaviour 5: 2838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Druckman, James, and Leeper, Thomas. 2012. “Learning More from Political Communication Experiments: Pretreatment and Its Effects.” American Journal of Political Science 56 (4): 875–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N. 2004. “Priming the Vote: Campaign Effects in a U.S. Senate Election.” Political Psychology 25 (4): 577–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N. 2022. “A Framework for the Study of Persuasion.” Annual Review of Political Science 25: 6588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N., Green, Donald P., Kuklinski, James H., and Lupia, Arthur. 2011. “Introduction.” In Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science, eds. Druckman, James N., Green, Donald P., Kuklinski, James H., and Lupia, Arthur, 12. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N., Klar, Samara, Krupnikov, Matthew Levendusky Yanna, and Ryan, John Barry. 2022. “(Mis)estimating Affective Polarization.” Journal of Politics 84 (2): 1106–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elshtain, Jean Bethke. 1995. Women and War, with a New Epilogue. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Enloe, Cynthia. 2000. Bananas, Beaches and Bases: Making Feminist Sense of International Politics. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Fearon, James. 1994. “Domestic Political Audiences and the Escalation of International Disputes.” American Political Science Review 88 (3): 577–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedrichs, Jörg, and Kratochwil, Friedrich. 2009. “On Acting and Knowing: How Pragmatism Can Advance International Relations Research and Methodology.” International Organization 63 (4): 701–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golby, James, Feaver, Peter, and Dropp, Kyle. 2018. “Elite Military Cues and Public Opinion about the Use of Military Force.” Armed Forces & Society 44 (1): 4471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guzzini, Stefano. 2011. “Securitization as a Causal Mechanism.” Security Dialogue 42 (4–5): 329–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hafner-Burton, Emilie M., Haggard, Stephan, Lake, David A., and Victor, David G.. 2017. “The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations.” International Organization 71 (S1): S1S31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, Lene. 2000. “The Little Mermaid’s Silent Security Dilemma and the Absence of Gender in the Copenhagen School.” Millenium 29 (2): 285306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harberkorn, Jennifer. 2018. “Men Have Done It since the Founding Fathers. Now Female Vets Are Hoping to Parlay Military Service into Politics.” Los Angeles Times, October 17.Google Scholar
Hardy, Molly M., Coker, Calvin R., Funk, Michelle E., and Warner, Benjamin R.. 2019. “Which Ingroup, When? Effects of Gender, Partisanship, Veteran Status, and Evaluator Identities on Candidate Evaluations.” Communication Quarterly 67 (2): 199220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobbes, Thomas. 1982. Leviathan. London: Penguin Classes.Google Scholar
Holman, Mirya, Merolla, Jennifer, and Zechmeister, Elizabeth. 2011. “Sex, Stereotypes, and Security: A Study of the Effects of Terrorist Threat on Assessments of Female Leadership.” Journal of Women, Politics & Policy 32 (3): 173–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holman, Mirya R., Merolla, Jennifer L., and Zechmeister, Elizabeth J.. 2022. “The Curious Case of Theresa May and the Public That Did Not Rally: Gendered Reactions to Terrorist Attacks Can Cause Slumps Not Bumps.” American Political Science Review 116 (1): 249–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddy, Leonie, and Terkildsen, Nayda. 1993. “The Consequences of Gender Stereotypes for Women Candidates at Different Levels and Types of Office.” Political Research Quarterly 46 (3): 503–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, Susan D. 2015. “Experiments in International Relations: Lab, Survey, and Field.” Annual Review of Political Science 18: 403–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, Patrick Thaddeus. 2011. The Conduct of Inquiry in International Relations. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jervis, Robert, Yarhi-Milo, Keren, and Casler, Don. 2021. “Redefining the Debate over Reputation and Credibility in International Security.” World Politics 73 (1): 167203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, Tyler, and Kertzer, Joshua D.. 2023 “Armies and Influence: Elite Experience and Public Opinion on Foreign Policy.” Journal of Conflict Resolution. https://doi.org/10.1177/00220027231203565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kertzer, Joshua D. 2016. Resolve in International Politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kertzer, Joshua D., and Tingley, Dustin. 2018. “Political Psychology in International Relations: Beyond the Paradigms.” Annual Review of Political Science 21: 319–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, David C., and Matland, Richard E.. 2003. “Sex and the Grand Old Party: An Experimental Investigation of the Effect of Candidate Sex on Support for a Republican Candidate.” American Politics Research 31 (5): 595612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, Ronald R., Ralston, Robert, and Rapport, Aaron. 2021. “No Right to Be Wrong: What Americans Think about Civil–Military Relations.” Perspectives on Politics 21 (2): 606–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawless, Jennifer. 2004. “Women, War, and Winning Elections: Gender Stereotyping in the Post-September 11th Era.” Political Research Quarterly 57 (3): 479–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Léonard, Sarah, and Kaunert, Christian. 2010. “Reconceptualizing the Audience in Securitization Theory.” In Securitization Theory: How Security Problems Emerge and Dissolve, ed. Balzacq, Thierry, 5776. New York: Taylor and Francis Group.Google Scholar
Madsen, Jens Koed. 2019. “Voter Reasoning Bias When Evaluating Statements from Female and Male Political Candidates.” Politics & Gender 15 (2): 310–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattes, Michaela, and Weeks, Jessica L. P.. 2019. “Hawks, Doves, and Peace: An Experimental Approach.” American Journal of Political Science 63 (1): 5366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, Monika L., and Panagopoulos, Costas. 2015. “Be All That You Can Be: The Electoral Impact of Military Service as an Information Cue.” Political Research Quarterly 68 (2): 293305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, Matt. 2008. “Securitization and the Construction of Security.” European Journal of International Relations 14 (4): 563–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McManus, Roseanne. 2017. Statements of Resolve: Achieving Coercive Credibility in International Conflict. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meeks, Lindsay. 2012. “Is She ‘Man Enough’? Women Candidates, Executive Political Offices, and News Coverage.” Journal of Communication 62 (1): 175–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, Ellen. 2018. “Record Number of Female Veterans to Serve in Next Congress.” The Hill, November 13.Google Scholar
Motta, Matthew, Ralston, Robert, and Spindel, Jennifer. 2021. “A Call to Arms for Climate Change? How Military Service Member Concern about Climate Change Can Inform Effective Climate Communication.” Environmental Communication 15 (1): 8598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrocik, John. 1996. “Issue Ownership in Presidential Elections, with a 1980 Case Study.” American Journal of Political Science 40 (3): 825–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potenz, Timothy James. 2019. “Introduction: The Rational and Constitution of This Symposium.” Polity 51 (2): 321–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiter, Dan. 2015. “The Positivist Study of Gender and International Relations.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 59 (7): 1301–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, David. 2022. “The Electoral Impact of Military Experience: Evidence from U.S. Senate Elections (1982–2016).” Armed Forces & Society 48 (4): 961–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenwasser, Shirley M., and Dean, Norma G.. 1989. “Gender Role and Political Office: Effects of Perceived Masculinity/Femininity of Candidate and Political Office.” Psychology of Women Quarterly 13 (1): 7785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, John Barry. 2013. “An Experimental Study of Persuasive Social Communication.” Political Communication 30 (1): 100–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanbonmatsu, Kira, and Dolan, Kathleen. 2009. “Do Gender Stereotypes Transcend Party?Political Research Quarterly 62 (4): 485–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, Carl. 2007. The Concept of the Political: Expanded Edition. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, Monica, and Bos, Angela. 2014. “Measuring Stereotypes of Female Politicians.” Political Psychology 35 (2): 245–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, Monica, and Bos, Angela. 2019. “The Application of Social Role Theory to the Study of Gender in Politics.” Political Psychology 40 (S1): 173213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schramm, Madison, and Stark, Alexandra. 2020. “Peacemakers or Iron Ladies? A Cross-National Study of Gender and International Conflict.” Security Studies 29 (3): 199220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, Joshua A., and Blair, Christopher W.. 2020. “Do Women Make More Credible Threats? Gender Stereotypes, Audience Costs, and Crisis Bargaining.” International Organization 74 (4): 872–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sjoberg, Laura. 2013. Gendering Global Conflict: Toward a Feminist Theory of War. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Swers, Michele. 2007. “Building a Reputation on National Security: The Impact of Stereotypes Related to Gender and Military Experience.” Legislative Studies Quarterly 32 (4): 559–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, Kate Hendricks, and Hunter, Kyleanne. 2019. Invisible Veterans: What Happens When Military Women Become Civilians Again. Westport, CT: Praeger.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomsen, Danielle. 2011. “Overcoming Gender Stereotypes: How Candidate Signaling Affects Voter Evaluations.” Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association.Google Scholar
Tickner, J. Ann. 1992. Gender in International Relations: Feminist Perspectives on Achieving International Security. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Tilly, Charles. 1992. Coercion, Capital, and European States, AD 990–1992. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tomz, Michael, Weeks, Jessica L. P., and Yarhi-Milo, Keren. 2020. “Public Opinion and Decisions about Military Force in Democracies.” International Organization 74 (1): 119–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vraga, Emily K. 2017. “Which Candidates Can Be Mavericks? The Effects of Issue Disagreement and Gender on Candidate Evaluations.” Politics & Policy 45 (1): 430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wæver, Ole. 2011. “Politics, Security, Theory.” Security Dialogue 42 (4): 465–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wibben, Annick T. R. 2011. “Feminist Politics in Feminist Security Studies.” Politics & Gender 7 (4): 590–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Experimental Tasks, Study 1

Figure 1

Table 1. Change in Believing Candidate Is a “Strong Leader,” Study 1

Figure 2

Figure 2. Defense Importance by Candidate Treatment, Study 1Note: OLS coefficients shown in SI D.1. Republican women: difference = 0.037 ($ p=0.31 $); Democratic women: difference= $ -0.003 $ ($ p=0.94 $). Women overall (both parties): difference = 0.010 ($ p=0.70 $). Republican men: difference = 0.151 ($ p=0.02 $); Democratic men: difference = 0.093 ($ p=0.08 $). Men overall (both parties): difference = 0.12 ($ p=0.004 $).

Figure 3

Figure 3. Experimental Tasks, Study 2

Figure 4

Figure 4. Study 2, Candidate Leadership due to Veteran Status for Men and Women CandidatesNote: Left panel: marginal effects of veteran status by candidate gender; coefficient estimates (OLS) are in SI E.5. Positive effects mean that veteran status increases perceived candidate quality; negative mean a decrease; 95% confidence intervals and one-tailed p-values (following hypothesized directional predictions), seven-point scale. Right panel: mean leadership by gender and veteran status.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Study 2, Gender Differences in Perceived Candidate Leadership for Veteran and Nonveteran CandidatesNote: Marginal effects of gender by candidate veteran status; coefficient estimates (OLS) are in SI E.5. Positive effects mean that women are perceived more positively than men; negative mean that men are perceived more positively than women; 95% confidence intervals, seven-point scale.

Figure 6

Figure 6. Statement on Policy Support, Veteran Status, Study 2Note: Left panel: marginal effects of making a statement on support for the position in the statement by veteran status. Positive marginal effects mean that exposure to the statement increases support for the position in the statement. Right panel: predicted probabilities of supporting the position in the statement. Both figures show 95% confidence intervals. Full coefficients from the OLS model which produced these estimates as well as a logit model showing similar results are in SI E.7.

Figure 7

Figure 7. Effect of Statement on Policy Support, Gender, Study 2Note: Left panel: marginal effects of making a statement on support for the position in the statement by gender, treating veteran status as a nuisance factor. Positive marginal effects mean that exposure to the statement increases support for the position in the statement. Right panel: mean supporting the position in the statement. Both figures show 95% confidence intervals. Full coefficients from the OLS model which produced these estimates as well as a logit model showing similar results are in SI E.7.

Figure 8

Figure 8. Statement and Policy Support, Gender and Veteran Status, Study 2Note: Figures show support for position in the statement, 95% confidence intervals. Full coefficients from the OLS model which produced these estimates are in SI E.7.

Supplementary material: File

Caverley and Krupnikov supplementary material

Caverley and Krupnikov supplementary material
Download Caverley and Krupnikov supplementary material(File)
File 911 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Caverley and Krupnikov Dataset

Link
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.