Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-495rp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-06T13:21:59.422Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Meta-analysis of associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol regulation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 June 2023

Laura Perrone*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Daneele Thorpe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Grace Shariat Panahi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Yukihiro Kitagawa
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Oliver Lindhiem
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
Kristin Bernard
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook, NY, USA
*
Corresponding author: Laura Perrone; Email: laura.perrone@stonybrook.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Childhood adversity has been associated with hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis dysregulation, which is associated with mental and physical health consequences. However, associations between childhood adversity and cortisol regulation in the current literature vary in magnitude and direction. This multilevel meta-analysis examines the association between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol measures, as well as potential moderators of these effects (adversity timing and type, study or sample characteristics). A search was conducted in online databases PsycINFO and PubMed for papers written in English. After screening for exclusion criteria (papers examining animals, pregnant women, people receiving hormonal treatment, people with endocrine disorders, cortisol before age 2 months, or cortisol after an intervention), 303 papers were identified for inclusion. In total, 441 effect sizes were extracted from 156 manuscripts representing 104 studies. A significant overall effect was found between childhood adversity and bedtime cortisol, r = 0.047, 95% CI [0.005, 0.089], t = 2.231, p = 0.028. All other overall and moderation effects were not significant. The lack of overall effects may reflect the importance of the timing and nature of childhood adversity to adversity’s impact on cortisol regulation. Thus, we offer concrete recommendations for testing theoretical models linking early adversity and stress physiology.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Childhood adversity is associated with a wide range of negative mental and physical health outcomes (e.g., Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards and Marks1998). Thus, understanding the mechanisms through which childhood adversity disrupts well-being is of critical importance. One potential mechanism is the impact of childhood adversity on the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, a major component of the neuroendocrine system. The end product of the HPA axis, the glucocorticoid hormone cortisol, not only contributes to the body’s immediate response to stressors but also to the body’s overall diurnal regulation. As a result, alterations to the HPA axis may impact the ability to regulate key bodily systems, making it important to understand how childhood adversity contributes to diurnal cortisol regulation. However, to date, literature in this field has provided mixed results.

The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and cortisol

Cortisol is produced within the body when the hypothalamus releases corticotropin-releasing factor and arginine vasopressin. Corticotropin-releasing factor and arginine vasopressin then prompt the pituitary gland to release ACTH, which binds to the adrenal cortex and triggers the release of cortisol (Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). Cortisol may then bond with either mineralocorticoid receptors, which are involved in the maintenance of the HPA axis’s circadian rhythm and regulation of key bodily functions, or glucocorticoid receptors, which are activated during stress responses and often work in opposition to the effects of mineralocorticoid receptors (Gunnar & Quevedo, Reference Gunnar and Quevedo2007). The HPA axis’s circadian rhythm typically produces a diurnal cortisol pattern that involves a sharp peak in the morning approximately 30 minutes after awakening, known as the cortisol awakening response (CAR), followed by a decline in cortisol throughout the day (Fries et al., Reference Fries, Dettenborn and Kirschbaum2009; Van Cauter, Reference Van Cauter1990). The CAR constitutes a unique effect of the HPA axis that includes aspects of both reactivity and diurnal regulation (Stadler et al., Reference Stalder, Lupien, Kudielka, Adam, Pruessner, Wüst, Dockray, Smyth, Evans, Kirschbaum, Miller, Wetherell, Finke, Klucken and Clow2022). When faced with a stressor, the body may mount an additional cortisol stress response that appears to enhance cardiovascular activation, mediate metabolic responses, and suppress immune responses, memory formation, and reproduction (Sapolsky et al., Reference Sapolsky, Romero and Munck2000).

Although activation of the HPA axis in response to adversity may have immediate benefits in the face of a stressor, repeated activation of the HPA axis over time can lead to alterations in its overall functioning. When initially faced with stressors, individuals often experience increases in cortisol, or hypercortisolism; however, over time, responsiveness to stressors may decrease, resulting in hypocortisolism (Loman & Gunnar, Reference Loman and Gunnar2010). Furthermore, differences in the nature of the stressor (e.g., acute vs. chronic, physically vs. socially threatening) may also contribute to variability in patterns of cortisol dysregulation. For example, one meta-analysis found that although chronic stress broadly was associated with lower morning cortisol levels, higher afternoon/evening cortisol levels and a more blunted diurnal slope, stressors that involved social threat or that were potentially controllable were associated with higher morning cortisol levels (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007). Given that disruptions in the HPA axis are associated with various mental health and physical health outcomes (e.g., Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards and Marks1998), HPA axis functioning may provide an important link between adversity and well-being (e.g., Doom & Gunnar, Reference Doom and Gunnar2013).

Childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol

Adversity in childhood may be particularly impactful to HPA axis functioning. Childhood adversity has been defined as “negative environmental experiences that are likely to require significant adaptation by an average child and that represent a deviation from the expectable environment” (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019). Although there are a wide range of possible experiences that can be considered childhood adversities, many involve threat to one’s safety and well-being (e.g., abuse, exposure to violence) and/or deprivation of expected resources and stimulation (e.g., neglect, poverty, institutionalization; McLaughlin & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016). Because children are dependent on external sources for survival and regulation, children may be unable to manage adversity on their own and are reliant on a caregiver to buffer against adversity’s potential impact (e.g., Gunnar & Donzella, Reference Gunnar and Donzella2002). As a result, adversity that children face without support from a caregiver and adversity that disrupts sensitive caregiving (e.g., maltreatment, parental psychopathology) may have an especially strong impact on child development and later outcomes. Early social deprivation, in particular, has been identified as a key contributor to early HPA axis development (e.g., Koss et al., Reference Koss, Hostinar, Donzella and Gunnar2014). It has been proposed that early adversity involving caregiving stress may accelerate emotional development including self-regulation (Callaghan & Tottenham, Reference Callaghan and Tottenham2016).

Thus, a key aspect of childhood adversity is that it has the potential to alter normative developmental processes, which in turn can contribute to negative outcomes later in life, including psychopathology (McLaughlin, Reference McLaughlin2016). Children facing adversity are often exposed to high levels of stress, which can have a direct impact on the functioning of their HPA axis. Multiple theories aim to explain physiological changes that provide adaptation to adversity, such as the general adaptation syndrome model (Selye, Reference Selye1946), the biological sensitivity to context model (Boyce & Ellis, Reference Boyce and Ellis2005), the biological embedding model (Hertzman, Reference Hertzman1999; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Parker2011), the three-hit hypothesis (Daskalakis et al., Reference Daskalakis, Bagot, Parker, Vinkers and de Kloet2013) and the adaptive calibration model (Del Giudice et al., Reference Del Giudice, Ellis and Shirtcliff2011). These models are often based at least in part in the concept of adaptive calibration, which proposes that individuals respond to current stressors with biological adaptations that assist them in achieving allostasis amid adversity (Del Guidice et al., Reference Del Giudice, Ellis and Shirtcliff2011). These biological adaptations may differ (e.g., hyper- vs. hypocortisolism) based on the nature of the adversity faced. Importantly, both hyper- and hypocortisolism result in deviations from the typical diurnal cortisol pattern described above, which in turn have been associated with a wide range of physical and mental health problems (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke and Gilbert2017; Shirtcliff & Essex, Reference Shirtcliff and Essex2008). As a result, understanding early contributors to diurnal cortisol dysregulation, defined as either hyper- or hypocortisolism, may provide critical avenues of intervention to prevent significant downstream consequences.

Consistent with these theories, many studies have provided support for a link between childhood adversity and dysregulation of diurnal cortisol patterns. For example, among preschool-age children, higher cumulative risk comprised of indicators including adolescent parent status, single parent status and low education has been associated with lower morning cortisol levels and more blunted diurnal slopes (Zalewski et al., Reference Zalewski, Lengua, Thompson and Kiff2016). In addition, moderate amounts of cumulative adversity comprised of socioeconomic disadvantage, negative life events and traumatic events have been associated with a higher CAR and less steep diurnal slope among children (Gustafsson et al., Reference Gustafsson, Anckarsäter, Lichtenstein, Nelson and Gustafsson2010). However, results are often inconsistent across studies.

Previous studies examining adversity have included numerous indicators of adversity, such as socioeconomic status, parental mental health, parental marital status, parental criminal conviction and parental education (e.g., Atkinson et al., Reference Atkinson, Beitchman, Gonzalez, Young, Wilson, Escobar, Chisholm, Brownlie, Khoury, Ludmer, Villani and Eapen2015; Evans, Reference Evans2003). Many of these indicators are not direct measures of threat or deprivation but rather serve as proxies for adversities such as socioeconomic hardship or a lack of sensitive caregiving. For example, parental mental illness or having a single parent may not be an adversity if the parent is able to care for their child consistently and sensitively but rather becomes an adversity when it interferes with sensitive caregiving or results in a lack of necessary resources in the family. In addition, the impact of adversities may vary based on the extent to which the child is protected by other factors, such as sensitive caregiving. Given that a child’s experience of whether an event is an adversity may vary based on the child’s broader context, experiences and level of support from caregivers, measuring adversity directly can prove challenging. As a result, studies examining multiple adversities have often relied on indirect proxies for threat and deprivation (e.g., parental mental health, single parent status) as well as more direct measures of threat and deprivation (e.g., community-level stressors, discrimination, financial strain, maltreatment, difficulties in parenting and parent–child relationships, parental substance use, surrogate care), with the overarching goal of capturing experiences that frequently convey deviations from the expectable environment producing significant stress for the child (either directly or indirectly) and therefore requiring physiological adaptation within the HPA axis.

Characteristics of adversity

Studies examining childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol regulation are numerous and cover a wide range of childhood adversity. However, some studies have begun to identify aspects of childhood adversity that may be particularly important in adversity’s association with HPA axis functioning and diurnal cortisol patterns. For example, McLaughlin and Sheridan’s (Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016) dimensional model of childhood adversity suggests that categorizing adversities along dimensions of deprivation and threat may add additional insight beyond cumulative adversity models that simply tally the number of adversities. Although not yet examined with diurnal cortisol, this framework has revealed that childhood violence exposure, but not social deprivation, was associated with a blunted cortisol response to stress in urban adolescents, providing evidence that threat and deprivation may have different impacts on the HPA axis (Peckins et al., Reference Peckins, Roberts, Hein, Hyde, Mitchell, Brooks-Gunn, McLanahan, Monk and Lopez-Duran2020). Thus, examining specific characteristics of adversity is critical to understanding the impact of childhood adversity on diurnal cortisol regulation.

Timing of adversity

Many studies provide evidence that the timing of adversity exposure may be critical in associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol. For example, age at which adversity occurs, time since adversity onset and length of adversity have all been associated with diurnal cortisol pattern differences (e.g., Doom et al., Reference Doom, Cicchetti and Rogosch2014; Flannery et al., Reference Flannery, Gabard-Durnam, Shapiro, Goff, Caldera, Louie, Gee, Telzer, Humphreys, Lumian and Tottenham2017; Isenhour et al., Reference Isenhour, Raby and Dozier2020; Leneman et al., Reference Leneman, Donzella, Desjardins, Miller and Gunnar2018; Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, King, Meaney and McEwen2001; Quevedo et al., Reference Quevedo, Johnson, Loman, LaFavor and Gunnar2012). Time since onset and duration of adversity may play important roles in shifts from hypercortisolism to hypocortisolism. Sensitive periods and critical windows may also be important to understanding the impact of childhood adversity on diurnal cortisol regulation.

Previous research indicates the possibility of multiple such sensitive periods. Both animal and human models provide evidence of a potential hyporesponsive period early in life in which cortisol responses to stressors are lessened with parental care playing a critical role (Gunnar & Donzella, Reference Gunnar and Donzella2002), suggesting that the impact of adversity in the first few years of life may depend at least in part on the availability of sensitive caregiving for external stress regulation. In addition, exposure to adversity between ages 3 and 7 has been identified as an important period for CAR dysregulation in adulthood, which may be related to early amygdala development during this period (Raymond et al., Reference Raymond, Marin, Wolosianski, Journault, Longpré, Leclaire and Lupien2021). Puberty may also be an important developmental period given evidence for recalibration of cortisol reactivity during puberty (Gunnar et al., Reference Gunnar, DePasquale, Reid, Donzella and Miller2019), which suggests continued development of the HPA axis in adolescence.

Although much remains to be explored, these studies provide initial evidence that the timing and chronicity of childhood adversity may play a critical role in the impact of adversities on diurnal cortisol regulation. This may in part result from the critical role that caregivers play in helping their children to regulate stress, especially early in life, as well as the continued development and plasticity of biological structures and pathways related to stress regulation throughout childhood and adolescence.

Previous meta-analytic and systematic review evidence

Given the inconsistent literature, it is critical to examine previous meta-analytic findings to understand the current state of the field before planning future studies further examining associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol regulation. Fogelman and Canli (Reference Fogelman and Canli2018) found no significant overall associations between early life stress and the CAR. However, their results indicated significant heterogeneity among effects such that sexually, emotionally, or physically abusive forms of early life stress were associated with a heightened CAR. Similarly, Bernard et al. (Reference Bernard, Frost, Bennett and Lindhiem2017) found no significant overall associations between maltreatment and wake levels, the CAR, or diurnal slope; however, associations between maltreatment and lower wake levels were significant specifically for agency-referred samples (e.g., following child welfare involvement, rather than via self-report). Additionally, Hackman et al. (Reference Hackman, O'Brien and Zalewski2018) found no overall association between parenting and morning cortisol but significant heterogeneity among effects. Specifically, there were significant associations between warm/sensitive parenting and higher levels of morning cortisol within intervention (as opposed to observational) studies and among samples that experienced maltreatment. Furthermore, associations became more positive as the interval between the measurement of parenting and cortisol increased. Taken together, these meta-analyses indicate potential associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol patterns, although the overall associations remain unclear.

Additional insight can be gleaned from meta-analyses and systematic reviews examining childhood adversity and other aspects of cortisol. For example, hair cortisol serves as a proxy for cumulative levels of HPA axis activity indicating chronic stress over previous months (Gow et al., Reference Gow, Thomson, Rieder, Van Uum and Koren2010). Previous meta-analyses and systematic reviews have provided mixed results for the association between adversity and hair cortisol levels, including significant associations only for adversity that occurs in adulthood (Khoury et al., Reference Khoury, Enlow, Plamondon and Lyons-Ruth2019), limited associations for social adversity in childhood (Bryson et al., Reference Bryson, Price, Goldfeld and Mensah2021) and associations with childhood adversity that vary with the age at which hair levels are measured (Grant & Meyer, Reference Grant and Meyer2021). An additional cortisol measure, reactivity to stressors, reflects the HPA axis’s short-term response to more immediate stressors rather than the daily regulation captured in diurnal cortisol measures. Meta-analyses and systematic reviews examining childhood adversity and cortisol reactivity have also indicated mixed results, with some indicating that childhood adversity is associated with blunted cortisol reactivity (e.g., Brindle et al., Reference Brindle, Pearson and Ginty2022; Bunea et al., Reference Bunea, Szentágotai-Tătar and Miu2017; Hakamata et al., Reference Hakamata, Suzuki, Kobashikawa and Hori2022), others indicating mixed directions of effects (e.g., Hosseini-Kamkar et al., Reference Hosseini-Kamkar, Lowe and Morton2021; Hunter et al., Reference Hunter, Minnis and Wilson2011) and one indicating a possible lack of significant associations (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Lee and Leung2021). Although examination of both hair cortisol concentration and cortisol reactivity were beyond the scope of the present meta-analysis, findings in previous studies present a consistent picture of complex potential associations between childhood adversity and HPA axis functioning.

The present study

Given the substantial literature examining childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol, quantitative meta-analytic methods provide an opportunity to examine overall associations between childhood adversity, broadly defined, and indicators of diurnal cortisol regulation. To our knowledge, no meta-analysis has yet examined associations between childhood adversity broadly and all aspects of the diurnal cortisol pattern included herein. The present study sought to address this gap by examining associations between childhood adversity and concurrent or subsequent diurnal cortisol measures (i.e., wake levels, the CAR, diurnal cortisol change and bedtime levels) in nonintervention studies. Categories of adversity within this meta-analysis include community-level stressors, cumulative adversity, difficulties in parenting and parent–child relationships, discrimination, financial strain, maltreatment, parental status, parental mental health, parental substance use, surrogate care and other family/parenting stress. (See Supplemental Materials for additional details.) The primary aim of the present study was to estimate the magnitude of these associations. As exposure to adversity has been associated with both hypercortisolism and hypocortisolism and both are considered forms of dysregulation, we did not specify directional hypotheses. A secondary aim was to explore potential moderators of these associations, including type of adversity, timing of adversity, age at which diurnal cortisol regulation was assessed, study-level sociodemographic indicators, methodological approaches and publication year.

Method

Procedure

This meta-analysis was reported in accordance with Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff and Altman2010).

Search strategy

A systematic search of peer-reviewed journal articles and dissertations was conducted in PsycINFO and PubMed through February 2020. The search term “cortisol” was combined with terms related to a range of adverse childhood experiences (see Supplemental Materials for Boolean phrases).

Eligibility criteria

Papers were included if they were written in English and met the following criteria: (a) included at least one diurnal measure of salivary cortisol, (b) included at least one measure of childhood adversity experienced before the age of 18 years and (c) included human participants. Additionally, papers were excluded if: (a) cortisol was measured exclusively after an intervention; (b) cortisol was measured in the first two months after birth; (c) the sample was composed of pregnant women, people receiving hormonal treatment, or people with endocrine disorders; or (d) the paper was a meta-analysis, literature review, case study, or editorial.

Study selection

A total of 23,536 papers were initially screened for eligibility based on titles and abstracts by the first author. The first and second authors conducted a full-text eligibility assessment on all manuscripts initially screened for inclusion, yielding an inter-rater reliability Kappa of 0.63. All discrepancies were discussed to reach consensus for inclusion or exclusion, with the final author being consulted for any discrepancies that were difficult to resolve. Full-text eligibility assessment yielded 303 papers representing 209 unique studies for inclusion (see Figure 1 for PRISMA diagram).

Figure 1. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) Flowchart. Note. The broad and inclusive search terms likely led to an inflated number of initial records for screening, resulting in the large number of records excluded at the screening level.

Of these papers, 156 papers from 104 studies were included in the quantitative synthesis based on the availability of relevant effect sizes either reported in the paper or provided by contacted authors (see additional details below). Given the magnitude of the present meta-analysis, we included several overlapping studies from previous meta-analyses investigating the association between early life stress and cortisol regulation. However, the studies represented in this meta-analysis differed from those included in previous similar meta-analyses. Of the studies included in the present meta-analysis, 11.43% were included in the meta-analysis by Bernard et al. (Reference Bernard, Frost, Bennett and Lindhiem2017), 4.76% were included in the meta-analysis by Fogelman and Canli (Reference Fogelman and Canli2018) and 4.76% were included in the meta-analysis by Hackman et al. (Reference Hackman, O'Brien and Zalewski2018).

Decision rules and coding

The first and second authors coded all eligible studies for effect sizes and moderators, with the exception of the quality and threat/deprivation moderators, which were double-coded by the first, third and fourth authors. Discrepancies were discussed and resolved through conference for use in analyses. When the same sample was reported across multiple papers, authors first prioritized effect sizes from the largest sample and then effect sizes with the greatest time elapsed between assessment of childhood adversity and assessment of cortisol to prioritize longitudinal effects. Only samples that did not include individuals from the same family (e.g., siblings) were included to ensure independence of data. If siblings were included in a sample, authors were contacted to see if an effect size for a sample with one randomly selected individual in each family was available. One effect size was coded for each combination of childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol measure to avoid issues related to nonindependence of multiple effect sizes from the same study. However, following completion of coding, we modified our analysis plan to conduct multi-level meta-analyses following the guidelines of Assink and Wibbelink (Reference Assink and Wibbelink2016).

Childhood adversity. In order to capture a wide range of potential childhood adversities, childhood adversity was broadly defined as adverse experiences external to the child (i.e., not characteristics of the child such as psychopathology or illness). Studies examining cumulative adversity (e.g., Atkinson et al., Reference Atkinson, Beitchman, Gonzalez, Young, Wilson, Escobar, Chisholm, Brownlie, Khoury, Ludmer, Villani and Eapen2015; Evans, Reference Evans2003) were used as a basis for generating the categories included. Categories included cumulative adversity, community-level stressors (e.g., crime, low neighborhood socioeconomic status), discrimination, financial strain, maltreatment, difficulties in parenting and parent–child relationships, parental mental health, parental substance use, single parent status, surrogate care, other family stress and other (any adversity external to the child not encompassed in previous categories). See the Supplemental Materials for definitions from the coding guidelines used to identify variables included in each category. When multiple childhood adversity measures were available in the same category, decision rules prioritized, in order, measures that: (1) spanned the longest window of time in childhood, (2) were continuous, (3) were observed (rather than self-report), (4) included a greater proportion of individuals exposed to the adversity and (5) related to the primary caregiver (or mother when primary caregiver not noted). In addition, decision rules specific to each category were considered (see Supplemental Materials). When these rules did not yield a decision, the measure was selected randomly. When the same adversity measure was assessed at multiple time points, effect sizes were averaged when possible.

Diurnal cortisol. Although there are a wide range of measures for HPA axis cortisol production (e.g., reactivity in response to stressors, area under the curve, diurnal pattern), the present meta-analysis focused on measures that reflect the diurnal pattern of cortisol: wake levels (within 30 minutes of awakening), bedtime levels (within 30 minutes of bedtime), CAR and diurnal cortisol change. Of note, these measures are not independent since wake levels contribute to the CAR, and both wake and bedtime levels contribute to the diurnal cortisol change. A diurnal cortisol change effect size was included if the study included at least two samples that either were related to the participant’s wake-up time and bedtime or that spanned a range from morning to afternoon/evening on the same day. For studies in which samples were taken at specific times of the day rather than in relation to participants’ sleep patterns, diurnal cortisol change was included if at least one sample was planned to be collected at or before 10:00 AM and at least one same-day sample was planned to be collected at or after 3:00 PM. This approach was selected to include a wide range of the effect sizes available in the current literature while ensuring that effect sizes captured variability across the day. If cortisol was measured at multiple waves, the decision rules were to prioritize, in order: (1) the wave that included the greater number of days of cortisol samples and (2) the most recent wave to prioritize longitudinal effects. Only continuous cortisol measures were included. For the CAR and diurnal cortisol change, only measures representing the difference between earlier and later samples (e.g., simple/residualized difference scores, HLM slopes, regression lines, mean percentage increase) and not measures reflecting total cortisol production (area under the curve) were included.

Moderators. Several within-study moderators were coded to explore theoretical questions about the potential associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol patterns. Given that different types of childhood adversity may have different associations with diurnal cortisol patterns, category of childhood adversity was included as a within-study moderator. Each effect size was categorized as one of the childhood adversity categories described above, and this categorical moderator was included in moderation analyses. Category was selected as opposed to specific characteristics of adversity (e.g., controllability, threat to physical vs. social self) because information on categories of childhood adversity was more consistently available in manuscripts and therefore consistently codable.

At the same time, differences in the nature of childhood adversity may impact associated outcomes. One example is McLaughlin and Sheridan’s (Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016) threat and deprivation model of adversity. To examine threat vs. deprivation (which was added after an initial round of reviewer feedback), three coders independently reviewed all included studies to identify any coded effect sizes that reflected threat (i.e., physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse, witnessing domestic violence, exposure to violence outside the home, or bullying victimization) or deprivation (i.e., physical neglect, emotional neglect, food insecurity, low cognitive stimulation, institutional care, foster care, or poverty). Measures coded as threat or deprivation were based on guidelines from previous meta-analyses examining these dimensions (e.g., Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Policelli, Li, Dharamsi, Hu, Sheridan and Wade2021). Threat versus deprivation was then examined as a post hoc exploratory moderator within the subset of studies that included a measure of threat or deprivation.

Furthermore, given that timing may influence adversity’s impact on diurnal cortisol regulation, age at which adversity occurred was included as a within-study moderator. Age at adversity was coded for each effect size as one of the following categories: infancy (birth through age 2), early childhood (ages 3 through 8), middle childhood (ages 9 through 12), adolescence (ages 13 through 17), or multiple time periods (if the adversity spanned multiple of these age groups). In addition, mean age of participants at the time at which diurnal cortisol was assessed was coded as a between-study continuous moderator variable to capture the timing of diurnal cortisol regulation. More specific timing aspects (e.g., duration, chronicity and time since onset of adversity) were not coded as this information was not available consistently across studies.

Several additional study-level moderators were coded. Because diurnal cortisol patterns have been found to vary with sex (e.g., Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Gullberg, Råstam and Lindblad2009; Netherton et al., Reference Netherton, Goodyer, Tamplin and Herbert2004), percent of the sample identified as female was coded as a continuous moderator variable. Additionally, race/ethnicity has been associated with differences in diurnal cortisol patterns when examining potential adversity or stressors (e.g., DeSantis, Adam et al., Reference DeSantis, Adam, Hawkley, Kudielka and Cacioppo2015; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Hoyt and Adam2014). Thus, the percent of the sample identified as a racial/ethnic minority was coded as a continuous moderator variable. Racial/ethnic minority status was defined as any race/ethnicity other than non-Hispanic/Latin White for samples in the United States or based on the authors’ definition for samples outside the United States. Furthermore, several design characteristics likely to increase the methodological rigor of diurnal cortisol collection were coded as categorical moderators to reduce potential noise from methodological variability: number of days of cortisol collection (one or multiple), whether cortisol was transformed, time of diurnal cortisol change (whether included samples at both wake and evening/bedtime defined as after 5:00 PM as opposed to sample(s) taken in the morning and/or in the afternoon), whether participants were instructed to take wake samples at awakening (as opposed to within a certain number of minutes after awakening), and a variable reflecting study quality. The study quality variable was defined as in previous meta-analyses examining diurnal cortisol (e.g., Adam et al., Reference Adam, Quinn, Tavernier, McQuillan, Dahlke and Gilbert2017; Chida & Steptoe, Reference Chida and Steptoe2009) as a count of how many of the following covariates were accounted for within each study: age, sex, smoking, use of steroid-based medications, wake time, sampling day (weekday or weekend), self-reported adherence with sampling times, objective adherence with sampling times (e.g., electronic monitoring) and clear sampling instructions given to participants. Finally, two variables were coded for exploratory moderator analyses: whether data were received from authors or coded from the paper and publication year (to examine if effect sizes reported in the literature have changed over time).

Kappas or ICCs for these moderators were above 0.90 for all variables except time of diurnal cortisol change, which had a Kappa of 0.69; age at adversity, which had a Kappa of 0.60; and whether data was received from authors, which was not double-coded. Given the nature of our approach to coding threat vs. deprivation (retrospectively identifying effect sizes that met criteria), reliability could not be calculated. Any discrepancies for threat versus deprivation were resolved through consensus. To decrease the likelihood of Type I errors given the number of moderators examined, a Bonferroni correction was applied to the alpha level by dividing by the number of moderators, yielding a significance cutoff of p < 0.004 for analyses related to wake levels and diurnal cortisol change and p < 0.005 for analyses related to the CAR and bedtime levels.

Contacting authors. Authors were contacted if any relevant effect sizes could not be coded from the paper. Although at least one effect size could be estimated for 49 (23.44%) of studies screened for inclusion, authors were contacted for all 209 studies (representing the 303 papers included) since at least one effect size was not codable from each study. Authors provided additional data for 78 studies (37.32% of those requested), including additional effect sizes for some studies for which at least one effect size was already estimated from the text. Means and standard deviations were requested for categorical adversity variables and correlations for continuous adversity variables. Authors were asked to provide simple difference scores for the CAR and diurnal cortisol change whenever possible as this measure was most likely to be available across studies (compared to more complex measures such as change over time). In addition, missing moderator variables were requested if authors were contacted for effect sizes.

Calculating effect sizes. Effect sizes were calculated as correlation coefficients (r). Means and standard deviations and t-values were converted to correlation coefficients using the Practical Meta-Analysis Effect Size Calculator (Wilson). In addition, based on Peterson and Brown’s (Reference Peterson and Brown2005) findings that using beta coefficients to impute missing correlation coefficients produces relatively accurate effect size estimates, 14 standardized beta coefficients were converted to correlation coefficients using the conversion formula they recommend as producing the best approximation of the relation between correlation and beta coefficients:

$$r = \beta + .05\lambda $$

where λ equals 1 when β is nonnegative and 0 when β is negative. This formula accounts for the tendency of nonnegative beta coefficients to be somewhat smaller than corresponding correlation coefficients. Inter-rater reliability for effect sizes was acceptable as indicated by an ICC of .64. All effect sizes were coded such that positive effects indicate adversity is associated with elevated (higher) wake-up cortisol, a greater CAR, less cortisol change and elevated (higher) bedtime cortisol levels.

Data analyses

Analyses were conducted in R (Version 4.0.3). First, outlying effect sizes defined by Tabachnick and Fidell’s (Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013) guidelines of a standardized z-score greater than 3.29 or smaller than −3.29 were “winsorized” to three standard deviations from the mean, which included three effect sizes (two wake and one bed). All correlations were converted to Fisher’s Z scores for analyses using the escalc function of the metafor package (Viechtbauer, Reference Viechtbauer2010). Results were converted back to correlation coefficients (r) for ease of interpretation.

Because multiple effect sizes were often included from the same study (i.e., one for each type of adversity examined) and were therefore dependent, we utilized the multilevel approach to meta-analysis describe by Assink and Wibbelink (Reference Assink and Wibbelink2016) with effect sizes nested within study samples. This approach allowed us to examine variance in effect sizes across three levels: level 1 examined variance between participants within each study (sampling variance), level 2 examined variance between effect sizes within the same study (within-study variance) and level 3 examined variance in effect sizes between studies (between-study variance). An overall effect for childhood adversity was calculated for each measure of diurnal cortisol (i.e., wake level, CAR, diurnal cortisol change, bedtime level) with a random-effects three-level meta-analytic model using Restricted Maximum Likelihood (REML) estimation method through the rma.mv function of the metafor package. The Knapp and Hartung (Reference Knapp and Hartung2003) adjustment was applied to decrease the likelihood of obtaining unjustified significant results. Next, overall inconsistency of results of studies was assessed using the I 2 statistic (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Thompson, Deeks and Altman2003), and heterogeneity of within-study variance (level 2) and between-study variance (level 3) was examined using one-sided log-likelihood-ratio tests.

Moderators related to the primary aims of this study (i.e., type of adversity, threat vs. deprivation, age at adversity and age at cortisol collection) were examined using omnibus tests as described by Assink and Wibbelink (Reference Assink and Wibbelink2016). In addition, if one-sided log-likelihood-ratio tests indicated significant heterogeneity, additional study-level potential primary (i.e., sex, racial/ethnic minority, number of days of cortisol collection, cortisol transformation, diurnal cortisol change timing, whether participants were instructed to take wake samples at awakening, study quality) and exploratory (whether data was received from authors, publication year) moderators were examined. Finally, a post hoc analysis examining the study methodological quality variable as a moderator was conducted. Additional post hoc moderation analyses examining individual indicators of methodological quality (e.g., objective monitoring of awakening, compliance monitoring, instructions about eating/drinking/brushing teeth, assessment of endocrine condition, quality of sleep, etc.) were also performed (please see Supplemental Materials for additional details and results). For categorical moderators, a reference category was chosen for each analysis. (Of note, selection of reference category does not make a statistical difference in overall moderation effect.) If an initial omnibus test for a moderator with multiple categories was significant, additional analyses were conducted examining each individual category as the reference category to determine which pairs of categories were associated with significantly different effects. For type of childhood adversity, only categories of adversity for which there were more than 5 studies were included in moderator analyses for each cortisol measure to ensure sufficient representation for each category; categories with 5 or fewer studies were excluded using listwise deletion for that moderator analysis. For threat versus deprivation, moderation was examined within the subset of effect sizes that reflect threat or deprivation. In addition, given that the sociopolitical context varies across countries, having a racial/ethnic minority status likely conveys different impacts in each country. As a result, moderation analyses for the percentage of participants identified as having a racial/ethnic minority status were repeated using only the studies conducted in the United States. The United States was selected both because enough studies were conducted in the United States to examine that location uniquely (56 studies in the United States, compared to 12 in Canada, the next highest nation) and because significant racism and systemic discrimination have been documented for individuals with a racial/ethnic minority status within the United States (e.g., Wright et al., Reference Wright, Jarvis, Pachter and Walker-Harding2020).

Finally, analyses were conducted to assess for potential publication bias and missing data. Funnel plot asymmetry was examined using Egger’s regression test (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Smith, Schneider and Minder1997). Additionally, Rosenthal’s fail-safe N was calculated for significant effects to assess the number of missing or unpublished effect sizes needed to produce a nonsignificant effect (Rosenthal, Reference Rosenthal1979). Based on Rosenthal’s (Reference Rosenthal1979) suggestion, an effect was considered resistant to the file drawer problem if the fail-safe N was greater than 5k + 10, where k is the number of studies included. These analyses were included to be consistent with previous meta-analyses and to provide potential indicators of publication bias, even though these methods have not been tested for a multilevel approach to meta-analysis (Assink & Wibbelink, Reference Assink and Wibbelink2016).

Results

Wake levels

A total of 148 effect sizes from 83 distinct study samples were included in the meta-analysis examining childhood adversity and wake cortisol levels. The sample sizes ranged from 14 to 2,162 with a median of 102. See Table 1 for details about the studies included and Table 2 for the number of effect sizes in each category of childhood adversity.

Table 1. Study characteristics

Note. Papers are grouped together based on study. If effect sizes were pulled from a specific paper, that paper is included. If authors provided data for a study, all papers related to that study are included. Papers are listed in alphabetical order.

1 The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5; 2Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; 3Childhood Trauma Questionnaire – Short Form; 4Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; 5Beck Depression Inventory-II; 6Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia

Table 2. Number of effect sizes in each category of childhood adversity by diurnal cortisol measure

Overall effect

The overall effect for childhood adversity and wake levels of cortisol was not significant, r = −0.008, 95% CI [−0.024, 0.008], t = −0.949, p = 0.344 (see Supplemental Figure 1). One-sided log-likelihood-ratio tests indicated that there was not significant variation within, σ2 < 0.001, χ2(1) = 1.372, p = 0.121, or between, σ2 = 0.012, χ2(1) = 1.186, p = 0.138, studies. The distribution of variance across levels was 81.36% at level 1 (i.e., 81.36% of the total variance can be attributed to sampling variance within studies), 10.66% at level 2 (i.e., 10.66% of the total variance can be attributed to differences between effect sizes within studies) and 7.98% at level 3 (i.e., 7.98% of the total variance could be attributed to differences in effect sizes between studies). The overall I 2 reflecting heterogeneity in effect sizes across both levels 2 (within studies) and 3 (between studies) was 18.64% (i.e., 18.64% of the variability in effect size estimates resulted from differences in effect sizes within and between studies rather than sampling error).

Moderators

See Table 3 for information on moderator variables for each study and Table 4 for descriptive statistics for moderator variables. None of the moderators related to primary aims yielded significant results: type of adversity, F(6, 126) = 1.911, p = 0.084; age at adversity, F(4, 143) = 1.064, p = 0.377; and age at cortisol collection, F(1, 142) = 0.122, p = 0.727. Given that the between-study variance was not significant, none of the additional between-study moderators were examined. Finally, neither the post hoc analysis examining threat vs. deprivation as a moderator in the subset of studies that included threat or deprivation effect sizes (n = 18), F(1, 16) = 0.581, p = 0.457, nor the post hoc analysis examining study methodological quality, F(1, 146) = 0.024, p = 0.876, were significant. (See Supplemental Materials for more detailed methodological quality analyses.)

Table 3. Additional information on moderator variables by study

Note. This table includes information on moderator variables not already provided in Table 1. Papers are grouped together based on study. If effect sizes were pulled from a specific paper, that paper is included. If authors provided data for a study, all papers related to that study are included. Papers are listed in alphabetical order.

Table 4. Moderator variable descriptive statistics

Publication bias

Egger’s regression test did not indicate significant funnel plot asymmetry, z = 0.567, p = 0.571 (see Figure 2).

Figure 2. Funnel Plots with Trim and Fill. Note. In each figure, the shaded area represents significant effect sizes (p < .05). Black circles represent coded effect sizes. White circles represent effect sizes added by trim and fill analysis.

Cortisol awakening response

A total of 76 effect sizes from 45 distinct study samples were included in the meta-analysis examining childhood adversity and the CAR. Sample sizes ranged from 15 to 2,162 with a median of 89. See Table 1 for details about the studies included and Table 2 for the number of effect sizes in each category of childhood adversity.

The overall effect of childhood adversity on the CAR was not significant, r = 0.021, 95% CI [−0.013, 0.054], t = 1.223, p = 0.225 (see Supplemental Figure 2). One-sided log-likelihood-ratio tests indicated that variation was significant between, σ2 = 0.006, χ2(1) = 6.019, p = 0.007, but not within, σ2 < 0.001, χ2(1) < 0.001, p > 0.500, studies. The distribution of variance across levels was 50.59% at level 1 (sampling variance), <0.01% at level 2 (within-study variance) and 49.41% at level 3 (between-study variance), with an overall I 2 of 49.41%.

Moderators

None of the moderators related to primary aims yielded significant results: type of adversity, F(5, 60) = 0.674, p = 0.645; age at adversity, F(4, 71) = 1.129, p = 0.350; and age at cortisol collection, F(1, 73) = 0.174, p = 0.678. Given the significant between-study variance, study-level variables were also examined as potential moderators of the overall effect. None of the primary or exploratory moderator variables were significant (see Supplemental Table 1). Finally, neither the post hoc analysis examining threat vs. deprivation as a moderator in the subset of studies that included threat or deprivation effect sizes (n = 9), F(1, 7) = 0.279, p = 0.613, nor the post hoc analysis examining study methodological quality, F(1, 74) = 1.979, p = 0.164, were significant. (See Supplemental Materials for more detailed methodological quality analyses.)

Publication bias

Egger’s regression test did not indicate significant funnel plot asymmetry, z = −0.506, p = 0.613 (see Figure 2).

Diurnal cortisol change

A total of 137 effect sizes from 71 distinct study samples were included in the meta-analysis examining childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol change levels. The sample sizes ranged from 15 to 2,088 with a median of 100. See Table 1 for details about the studies included and Table 2 for the number of effect sizes in each category of childhood adversity.

The overall effect of childhood adversity on diurnal cortisol change was not significant, r = 0.017, 95% CI [−0.010, 0.043], t = 1.264, p = 0.215 (see Supplemental Figure 3). One-sided log-likelihood-ratio tests indicated significant variation between, σ2 = 0.006, χ2(1) = 9.960, p = 0.001, but not within, σ2 = 0.001, χ2(1) = 0.879, p = 0.174, studies. The distribution of variance across levels was 47.77% at level 1 (sampling variance), 6.01% at level 2 (within-study variance) and 46.21% at level 3 (between-study variance), with an overall I 2 of 52.23%.

Moderators

None of the moderators related to primary aims yielded significant results: type of adversity, F(7, 122) = 0.321, p = 0.943; age at adversity, F(4, 132) = 2.402, p = 0.053; and age at cortisol collection, F(1, 134) = 1.351, p = 0.247. Because there was significant between-study variance, study-level variables were also examined as potential moderators. None of the primary or exploratory moderator variables were significant (see Supplemental Table 1). Finally, neither the post hoc analysis examining threat vs. deprivation as a moderator in the subset of studies that included threat or deprivation effect sizes (n = 15), F(1, 13) = 0.072, p = 0.793, nor the post hoc analysis examining study methodological quality, F(1, 135) = 1.711, p = 0.362, were significant. (See Supplemental Materials for more detailed methodological quality analyses.)

Publication bias

Egger’s regression test did not indicate significant funnel plot asymmetry, z = −0.021, p = 0.983 (see Figure 2).

Bedtime levels

A total of 80 effect sizes from 42 distinct study samples were included in the meta-analysis examining childhood adversity and bedtime levels of cortisol. The sample sizes ranged from 26 to 580 with a median of 103.5. See Table 2 for the number of effect sizes in each category of childhood adversity and Table 1 for details about the studies included.

A significant effect was found for childhood adversity and bedtime levels of cortisol, r = 0.047, 95% CI [0.005, 0.089], t = 2.231, p = 0.028, indicating that children exposed to higher levels of adversity had higher bedtime cortisol levels than children exposed to lower levels of adversity (see Supplemental Figure 4). One-sided log-likelihood-ratio tests indicated significant variation between, σ2 = 0.012, χ2(1) = 20.727, p < .001, but not within, σ2 < 0.001, χ2(1) < 0.001, p > .500, studies. The distribution of variances was 39.09% at level 1 (sampling variance), <0.01% at level 2 (within-study variance) and 60.91% at level 3 (between-study variance), with an overall I 2 of 60.91%.

Moderators

None of the moderators related to primary aims yielded significant results: type of adversity, F(6, 68) = 0.877, p = 0.516; age at adversity, F(3, 76) = 1.175, p = 0.325; and age at cortisol collection, F(1, 76) = 0.513, p = 0.476. Given the significant between-study variance, study-level variables were also examined as potential moderators of the overall effect. None of the primary or exploratory moderator variables were significant (see Supplemental Table 1). Finally, neither the post hoc analysis examining threat vs. deprivation as a moderator in the subset of studies that included threat or deprivation effect sizes (n = 9), F(1, 7) = 0.355, p = 0.570, nor the post hoc analysis examining study methodological quality, F(1, 78) = 0.090, p = 0.765, were significant. (See Supplemental Materials for more detailed methodological quality analyses.)

Publication bias

Egger’s regression test did not indicate significant funnel plot asymmetry, z = 0.902, p = 0.367 (see Figure 2). In addition, Rosenthal’s (Reference Rosenthal1979) fail-safe N indicated that 299 unpublished or yet-to-be-conducted studies with nonsignificant findings would be necessary to produce a null overall effect. Since this is above the cutoff of 220, the fail-safe N indicates that the effect is not likely to be due to publication bias alone.

Discussion

Meta-analytic findings

Within this meta-analysis, the only significant overall effect was the association between childhood adversity and higher bedtime cortisol levels, which could not be accounted for by publication bias alone. The lack of significant overall effects for wake, the CAR and diurnal cortisol change is consistent with previous meta-analyses that have not yielded significant overall associations between childhood adversities of early life stress, maltreatment and parenting and components of the diurnal cortisol pattern (Bernard et al., Reference Bernard, Frost, Bennett and Lindhiem2017; Fogelman & Canli, Reference Fogelman and Canli2018; Hackman et al., Reference Hackman, O'Brien and Zalewski2018). However, the significant association between childhood adversity and bedtime cortisol levels is a novel meta-analytic contribution that suggests those who have experienced childhood adversity have more difficulty downregulating cortisol production as the day ends.

Understanding the magnitude of this effect is challenging as the current literature does not provide clear guidelines as to what magnitude of change in diurnal cortisol regulation is associated with clinically significant outcomes. Although this effect would be categorized as “small” by Cohen’s benchmarks, Cohen also emphasized the necessity of considering the research area and methodology when examining the magnitude of effect sizes (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). This meta-analysis examines bedtime cortisol values on a small sample of days. Though the effect of childhood adversity on cortisol levels measured at a single bedtime (or small sample of bedtimes) is clearly small, the cumulative effect over thousands of bedtimes (365 each year) may be consequential (see Funder & Ozer, Reference Funder and Ozer2019). Future studies examining the relationship between diurnal cortisol effect sizes and the magnitude of corresponding clinical impacts, especially cumulatively, will enhance our understanding of the significance of the overall effect presented here.

Despite significant heterogeneity within and between studies, no moderation effects were identified. This heterogeneity may be explained by substantial variability in methods utilized to assess diurnal cortisol, including timing of samples collected, number of collection days, use of adherence protocols, methods for data cleaning/preparation and calculation of outcome measures, which may contribute to mixed results. Heterogeneity in diurnal cortisol methodology has been found within randomized controlled trials that include diurnal cortisol as an outcome, highlighting the need for more consistent methods (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Booth, Spathis, Mollart and Clow2016). We attempted to capture this methodological heterogeneity by examining multiple moderators related to methods. As can be seen in Table 4, there was substantial variability across most methodological moderators examined herein; however, these moderators do not provide an indicator as to which methodological characteristics may be of particular importance as none were significant.

Findings in the context of current theoretical models

Interpreting these results within the context of the current literature on diurnal cortisol regulation may add insight into these meta-analytic results. Many theoretical models, including the cumulative adversity, biological embedding and three-hit models, suggest that the impact of childhood adversity on diurnal cortisol regulation may change over time. This is consistent with the concept of adaptive calibration (Del Giudice et al., Reference Del Giudice, Ellis and Shirtcliff2011), in which the body adapts in response to stressors and changes in the environment to maintain stability, such as by producing an initial increase in cortisol production (hypercortisolism) in response to an adversity at onset but shifting to a blunted response (hypercortisolism) with prolonged adversity duration. Thus, any attempt to capture overall associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol regulation may include effects that differ in magnitude and direction based on the adversity’s timing of onset, duration, chronicity, current presence or absence and time since initial onset.

One implication of this is that the associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol regulation may not be linear. Previous studies have provided evidence for such potential nonlinear relationships. For example, the study by Zalewski et al. (Reference Zalewski, Lengua, Thompson and Kiff2016) examining associations between cumulative risk and diurnal cortisol among preschool-age children found that both high and low levels of cumulative risk were associated with lower morning levels of cortisol and more blunted cortisol slopes compared to moderate levels of cumulative risk, suggesting that the association between amount of adversity and diurnal cortisol response may not be linear. Similarly, studies have indicated that the association between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol regulation may not be linear across development, such as a study by VanTieghem et al. (Reference VanTieghem, Korom, Flannery, Choy, Caldera, Humphreys, Gabard-Durnam, Goff, Gee, Telzer, Shapiro, Louie, Fareri, Bolger and Tottenham2021) that found morning cortisol levels in previously institutionalized children shift across development from blunted during childhood to heightened in adolescence. As a result, understanding the associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol may require analytic methods that examine possible nonlinear relationships that could not be captured in the present meta-analysis.

An additional implication is that including elements of adversity timing is crucial to understanding childhood adversity’s impact on diurnal cortisol regulation. Although the current meta-analysis aimed to capture aspects of childhood adversity timing, these attempts were limited by information available in the current literature. Studies do not consistently include information on the onset, duration, chronicity and current presence (except when childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol are measured concurrently) of childhood adversities, and few studies examine these associations longitudinally. As a result, we were restricted to coding timing of adversity as the developmental period for which the adversity was being assessed (e.g., infancy, adolescence, multiple periods). This methodological approach carried significant limitations, as it only provides information on whether the adversity was present during a particular developmental window and does not provide information on the adversity’s duration or presence before or after that developmental period. In addition, most studies examined childhood adversity across multiple developmental periods (e.g., at any point in childhood). Similarly, time elapsed between experiencing childhood adversity and assessing diurnal cortisol is not consistently reported across studies. This meta-analysis examined mean age at time of cortisol assessment as a potential moderator since it is plausible that older samples, especially those including adults, have more time elapsed since childhood adversity onset than younger samples. However, this measure is neither consistent nor precise and therefore provides limited insight. Of note, the feasibility of examining early adversity within the context of sensitive period models has been questioned given the complexity of identifying the nature, timing and duration of early adversities (which are often overlapping) and the possibility that some adversities may impact neurobiology through experience-dependent mechanisms (Gabard-Durnam & McLaughlin, Reference Gabard-Durnam and McLaughlin2019). Thus, the potential impact of adversity’s timing on HPA axis regulation remains a complex question requiring further investigation.

Additionally, a variety of adversity characteristics have been proposed to impact associations between adversity and HPA axis regulation, including traumatic nature, threat to physical self, uncontrollability, elicitation of emotions such as shame or loss, and whether the impact is threat or deprivation (McLaughlin & Sheridan, Reference McLaughlin and Sheridan2016; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007). These characteristics are not consistently reported across studies. Although the present study examined category of adversity as a moderator, these categories were broad and likely contained significant heterogeneity in type, intensity and duration of stressor, which may have obscured effects. Furthermore, measures of adversity sometimes fit into multiple categories. For example, low levels of conflict at home is an example of family stress, but high levels of conflict at home may take the form of domestic violence, which is both a family stressor and a form of maltreatment. Because many measures of family conflict within this meta-analysis included a range of behaviors that could not be considered exclusively domestic violence, we categorized family conflict under other family stress. However, two studies categorized as other family stress did focus specifically on measures of domestic violence (i.e., Hibel et al., Reference Hibel, Nuttall and Valentino2020; Theall et al., Reference Theall, Shirtcliff, Dismukes, Wallace and Drury2017) and therefore would also have been a good fit for the maltreatment category. Similarly, the category of surrogate care includes both foster care or institutionalization and adoption, which may convey different experiences of caregiving and different levels of adversity. (Of note, all children included in the surrogate care category experienced foster care, institutional care and/or maltreatment resulting in separation from biological parents with the possible exception of a subset of Gunnar et al. (Reference Gunnar, Morison, Chisholm and Schuder2001) sample who were adopted so early they had not yet been placed in orphanages.) We attempted to place adversities in the categories with which they would match most consistently, but these instances of overlap mean that categories of adversity are not fully independent.

A further limitation of this approach is the likelihood that some of these categories serve as proxies for childhood adversity but do not capture true “deviation from the expectable environment” (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019). A better way to assess childhood adversity, as suggested by McLaughlin (Reference McLaughlin2016), may be to include only events that result in deviations from expected caregiving or other significant adversity for the child. Although we could not make this distinction within the present meta-analysis based on the information consistently available in manuscripts, future studies that consider whether potential adversities constitute significant deviations from the expectable environment will likely enhance our understanding of the impact of early adversity. In addition, it is important to note that intervention efforts should be aimed at addressing sources of adversity directly (e.g., increasing financial and social support for overburdened parents) rather than the proxies for adversity (e.g., incentivizing single parents to be married).

Although we attempted to include a broad range of childhood adversity, other forms of adversity are likely missing. In particular, there was a lack of studies directly examining the impact of systemic racism and discrimination experienced during childhood on diurnal cortisol regulation. Racial and ethnic health disparities have been theorized to stem from increased allostatic load, to which systemic racism and discrimination are likely contributors (Carlson & Chamberlain, Reference Carlson and Chamberlain2005). Understanding associations between experiences of discrimination and diurnal cortisol regulation could help explain racial/ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol patterns (e.g., DeSantis et al., 2007; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Bruce and Fisher2012) and provide insight into pathways through which discrimination may impact health and well-being. Some studies have already provided preliminary evidence in identifying associations between experiences of discrimination and blunted diurnal slopes among individuals with a racial/ethnic minority status (Adam et al., Reference Adam, Heissel, Zeiders, Richeson, Ross, Ehrlich, Levy, Kemeny, Brodish, Malanchuk, Peck, Fuller-Rowell and Eccles2015; Zeiders et al., Reference Zeiders, Hoyt and Adam2014). In addition, it is essential to examine the impact of discrimination on minoritized youth more broadly, such as those with sexual and gender minority identities (Williams & Mann, Reference Williams and Mann2017). In one example, experience of greater LGBT stressors throughout the week was associated with elevated cortisol at awakening and 45 minutes after awakening in young adults (Figueroa et al., Reference Figueroa, Zoccola, Manigault, Hamilton, Scanlin and Johnson2021). Future studies further examining the impact of discrimination on diurnal cortisol patterns among youth with minoritized identities may increase our understanding of pathways contributing to alteration in the HPA axis and possible health disparities.

Finally, the random selection of a single measure of adversity for each category of childhood adversity from each study in the present meta-analysis meant that some effect sizes were excluded due to the study design; however, the use of random selection and the large number of effect sizes included likely provide a representative sample. To supplement our analyses utilizing broad adversity categories, we also attempted to examine whether categorization of adversity as threat or deprivation was a significant moderator of overall effect sizes in post hoc analyses. However, these analyses were limited by the small number of effect sizes included as a result of our retrospective approach to identifying threat and deprivation and of the difficulty of distinguishing between threat and deprivation given that many measures of adversity assessed both threat and deprivation together. As a result, we were likely underpowered to detect significant moderation effects related to threat vs. deprivation. Overall, a more nuanced examination of the characteristics of adversity that was beyond the scope of this meta-analysis may be required to understand complex associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol regulation in future studies.

Strengths and limitations

Several methodological decisions strengthened our ability to capture a broad overall picture of the association between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol. First, we defined childhood adversity broadly, including a wide range of adversity categories. Further, we utilized multilevel meta-analysis techniques, allowing the inclusion of multiple effects from the same study while accounting for interdependence of effects.

This meta-analysis also included several limitations in addition to those related to assessment of childhood adversity timing and nature discussed above. First, although the use of simple difference scores to calculate diurnal cortisol change and the CAR allowed us to include findings across a wide range of studies, these measures did not reflect change over time. Given the importance of timing to diurnal cortisol patterns, this may have resulted in inconsistencies in the included cortisol measures. In particular, timing is of critical importance to the accurate measurement of the CAR, and current recommendations include repeated sampling across the period after awakening, particularly at wake-up and from 30 to 45 minutes after awakening, as well as objective monitoring of participant adherence (Stalder et al., Reference Stalder, Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Adam, Pruessner, Wüst, Dockray, Smyth, Evans, Hellhammer, Miller, Wetherell, Lupien and Clow2016). This meta-analysis relied on authors’ determinations of timing for their measure of the CAR and therefore did not restrict inclusion of measures of the CAR based on sample timing. As a result, variability in the timing of the CAR measurements included herein may be creating additional noise in the data that could obscure true effects.

Similarly, diurnal cortisol change was included if cortisol samples from at least two time points (one at awakening or in the morning and one in the afternoon, evening, or at bedtime) were available to include the wide range of the effect sizes available in the present literature while also capturing variability across the waking day. However, it is important to note that there are numerous approaches for measuring changes in cortisol across the day, including wake to bedtime, peak to bedtime and morning to afternoon among others. The true effect for diurnal cortisol change may be obscured by the range of methodologies of the studies included in this meta-analysis. In a preliminary exploration of this possibility, whether diurnal cortisol change samples were collected at awakening and bedtime was included as a moderator and was not significant; however, the design of this meta-analysis precluded more nuanced examination of differences in diurnal cortisol change measurement. Furthermore, more comprehensive measures of change in cortisol throughout the day such as diurnal cortisol slope may provide a clearer picture of the association between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol patterns. Examination of cortisol collection methodology suggests the diurnal cortisol slope is well approximated by methods that include fixed samples at awakening and bedtime as well as at three additional points in the day, providing guidance for future studies (Hoyt et al., Reference Hoyt, Ehrlich, Cham and Adam2016). In addition, we did not examine other biomarkers or genetic factors that may contribute to an individual’s response to stress and interact with the diurnal cortisol pattern.

Another limitation is that our examination of racial/ethnic minority status as a study-level moderator may be impacted by differences in the experiences of those with racial/ethnic minority statuses that likely vary within and across countries. Although we attempted to explore this by examining this moderator in the subset of studies specific to United States, this moderator may be capturing diverse experiences even within a single country.

Finally, our measure of study quality was limited by the information reported on methodology within each manuscript. It is possible that some studies did account for covariates included within this measure that were not reported in their manuscripts, resulting in a lower quality score and adding noise to this measure of study quality. In addition, this measure weights every covariate equally and does not account for variability in the rigor of methods used to account for these covariates. Furthermore, although our post hoc analyses examined whether biobehavioral variables were accounted for by authors in the original study (i.e., included as a covariate, used as an exclusion criterion, or examined in follow-up analyses), they do not reflect whether these variables were accounted for as covariates in the effect sizes reported in this meta-analysis. As a result, although the majority of the post hoc moderation analyses examining study methodological quality and individual biobehavioral variables presented here were not significant, the potential noise within these variables in combination with the extensive previous literature indicating the importance of these variables prevents us from concluding that they are not relevant to cortisol analyses related to early adversity. Both consistent use of rigorous and high-quality methodological approaches and consistent reporting of such measures taken will be important for future studies.

Implications and future directions

In the present meta-analysis, the association between childhood adversity and bedtime cortisol levels emerged as the only significant association. This finding indicates that measuring bedtime cortisol levels may provide an important opportunity to capture the impact of early adversity on diurnal cortisol regulation in future studies. Clinically, developing interventions that ameliorate the negative impact of childhood adversity on bedtime cortisol levels is also important.

A consistent result throughout this meta-analysis was the lack of significant overall and moderation effects. Given that current theoretical models emphasize the importance of the timing and nature of childhood adversity, the lack of significant findings may in part result from the nuanced and complex nature of associations between adversity and HPA axis regulation. Many of these nuances were unable to be examined on a meta-analytic level, in part because they are not consistently examined or reported in the current literature. As a result, this meta-analysis highlights the importance of capturing specificity in the timing and nature of childhood adversity when examining associations with diurnal cortisol. When possible and relevant, future studies should make efforts to assess and report:

  1. 1. Age of onset of adversity

  2. 2. Ages at which adversity occurred

  3. 3. Duration/chronicity of adversity

  4. 4. Whether the adversity is concurrent

  5. 5. Time between adversity onset/termination and diurnal cortisol measurement

  6. 6. Characteristics of adversity, including whether it was traumatic, threatened physical integrity and involved deprivation or threat

  7. 7. Participants’ perceptions of adversity, including whether it was uncontrollable, whether it elicited emotions such as shame or loss, and its intensity

Furthermore, when considering age it may be important not only to consider chronological age but also to consider pubertal status since puberty may be an important developmental window for HPA axis functioning. Although it likely will not be feasible to assess each of these characteristics in every study, moving toward greater inclusion of these factors and considering the implications when they are unknown will likely strengthen the literature.

In addition to these recommendations specific to examining childhood adversity, it is of critical importance that studies continue to follow methodological guidelines for the accurate assessment of diurnal cortisol (e.g., Hoyt et al., Reference Hoyt, Ehrlich, Cham and Adam2016; Stalder et al., Reference Stalder, Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Adam, Pruessner, Wüst, Dockray, Smyth, Evans, Hellhammer, Miller, Wetherell, Lupien and Clow2016). Given the importance of timing to the accurate assessment of diurnal cortisol, collecting wake samples immediately upon awakening and utilizing objective monitoring of sample timing (e.g., with MEMS caps) are necessary for consistent and accurate measurements of the diurnal cortisol pattern. Of note, instructions to take samples immediately upon awakening were included in the study description for only 72.79% of wake level effect sizes in the present meta-analysis, and objective monitoring of adherence to sample timing was included in 18.37% of studies included in this meta-analysis, suggesting that inconsistencies in diurnal cortisol measurement may be a methodological limitation of the literature broadly. Furthermore, it is important to account for covariates related to the day of sampling (e.g., sleep quality, weekday vs. weekend) and traits of the individual (e.g., age, sex, contraception use) that may impact diurnal cortisol production, as well as to consider possible exclusion criteria for factors that cannot sufficiently be controlled (Stalder et al., Reference Stalder, Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Adam, Pruessner, Wüst, Dockray, Smyth, Evans, Hellhammer, Miller, Wetherell, Lupien and Clow2016). Consistent adherence to methodological recommendations in future diurnal cortisol studies will reduce potential noise that may interfere with identifying true effects.

In addition, it is important to note that diurnal cortisol patterns also reflect individual differences and day-to-day variability. As a result, person-centered approaches to examining individual diurnal cortisol profiles (e.g., Hoyt et al., Reference Hoyt, Zeiders, Chaku, Niu and Cook2021), particularly longitudinally, may provide additional insight. Furthermore, longitudinal examinations of associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol will be crucial to our ability to understand the impact of timing, intensity, chronicity and type of adversity on diurnal cortisol regulation.

Finally, this meta-analysis only examined diurnal cortisol as defined as cortisol levels at awakening or bedtime, the CAR, or diurnal cortisol change and did not capture measures of overall cortisol output (e.g., area under the curve), cumulative measures of cortisol (e.g., hair cortisol), or cortisol reactivity, which may provide additional insight into the impact of childhood adversity on HPA axis functioning. As discussed earlier, nonsignificant, significant positive and significant negative effects have all been found for associations between childhood adversity and hair cortisol levels (Bryson et al., Reference Bryson, Price, Goldfeld and Mensah2021; Grant & Meyer, Reference Grant and Meyer2021; Khoury et al., Reference Khoury, Enlow, Plamondon and Lyons-Ruth2019) or cortisol reactivity (Bunea et al., Reference Bunea, Szentágotai-Tătar and Miu2017; Hakamata et al., Reference Hakamata, Suzuki, Kobashikawa and Hori2022; Hosseini-Kamkar et al., Reference Hosseini-Kamkar, Lowe and Morton2021; Hunter et al., Reference Hunter, Minnis and Wilson2011; Lai et al., Reference Lai, Lee and Leung2021), suggesting many complexities remain to be untangled related to childhood adversity’s impact on HPA axis functioning. As a result, future studies examining these additional cortisol measures may be important to our understanding of childhood adversity and stress regulation.

Conclusions

In summary, the present meta-analysis found a significant association between childhood adversity and higher bedtime cortisol levels, with no significant moderation effects. These findings highlight that childhood adversity may particularly impact the ability to downregulate cortisol levels throughout the day, resulting in higher bedtime levels. In contrast, the overall effects for childhood adversity and other measures of diurnal cortisol (e.g., morning levels, the CAR and diurnal cortisol changes) were not significant. Given the limitations discussed above, it is not yet clear whether these null effects result from the complexity of these relationships or are a true reflection of overall associations. Associations between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol may be too complex to capture with studies, including the present meta-analysis, that examine adversity broadly defined and diurnal cortisol regulation measured across developmental stages at varying lengths from the onset of adversity. As it is possible that the lack of stronger effects resulted from variability in the timing and characteristics of childhood adversity assessed, future longitudinal studies utilizing consistent rigorous methods and adversity assessment that allow for the possibility of nonlinear relationships will be necessary to clarify possible nuances in the relation between childhood adversity and diurnal cortisol. Such studies have great potential for increasing our understanding of the likely complex impact of childhood adversity on diurnal cortisol regulation.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000561.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Christopher Facompré for providing consultation, resources and advice in preparing a meta-analysis.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (LP and DT, grant number 1315232).

Competing interests

None.

References

Adam, E. K., Heissel, J. A., Zeiders, K. H., Richeson, J. A., Ross, E. C., Ehrlich, K. B., Levy, D. J., Kemeny, M., Brodish, A. B., Malanchuk, O., Peck, S. C., Fuller-Rowell, T. E., Eccles, J. S. (2015). Developmental histories of perceived racial discrimination and diurnal cortisol profiles in adulthood: A 20-year prospective study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 279291, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.018,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adam, E. K., Quinn, M. E., Tavernier, R., McQuillan, M. T., Dahlke, K. A., & Gilbert, K. E. (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 2541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albers, E. M., Beijers, R., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., Sweep, F. C., & de Weerth, C. (2016). Cortisol levels of infants in center care across the first year of life: Links with quality of care and infant temperament. Stress-the International Journal on The Biology of Stress, 19(1), 817. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2015.1089230 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alink, L. R., Cicchetti, D., Kim, J., & Rogosch, F. A. (2012). Longitudinal associations among child maltreatment, social functioning, and cortisol regulation. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 224236. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024892 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashman, S. B., Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., Yamada, E., & Wilkinson, C. W. (2002). Stress hormone levels of children of depressed mothers. Development and Psychopathology, 14(2), 333349. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579402002080 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Assink, M., & Wibbelink, C. J. (2016). Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: A step-by-step tutorial. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 12(3), 154174. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.12.3.p154 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, L., Beitchman, J., Gonzalez, A., Young, A., Wilson, B., Escobar, M., Chisholm, V., Brownlie, E., Khoury, J. E., Ludmer, J., Villani, V., Eapen, V. (2015). Cumulative risk, cumulative outcome: A 20-year longitudinal study. PloS One, 10(6), e0127650, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0127650,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badanes, L. S., Dmitrieva, J., & Watamura, S. E. (2012). Understanding cortisol reactivity across the day at child care: The potential buffering role of secure attachments to caregivers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(1), 156165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.05.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badanes, L. S., Watamura, S. E., & Hankin, B. L. (2011). Hypocortisolism as a potential marker of allostatic load in children: Associations with family risk and internalizing disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 23(3), 881896. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941100037X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basu, A., Levendosky, A. A., & Lonstein, J. S. (2013). Trauma sequelae and cortisol levels in women exposed to intimate partner violence. Psychodynamic Psychiatry, 41(2), 247275. https://doi.org/10.1521/pdps.2013.41.2.247 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beijers, R., Daehn, D., Shalev, I., Belsky, J., & de Weerth, C. (2020). Biological embedding of maternal postpartum depressive symptoms: The potential role of cortisol and telomere length. Biological Psychology, 150, 107809. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.107809 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berger, M., Leicht, A., Slatcher, A., Kraeuter, A. K., Ketheesan, S., Larkins, S., & Sarnyai, Z. (2017). Cortisol awakening response and acute stress reactivity in first nations people. Scientific Reports, 7(1 https://doi.org/10.1038/srep41760 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernard, K., Butzin-Dozier, Z., Rittenhouse, J., & Dozier, M. (2010). Cortisol production patterns in young children living with birth parents vs children placed in foster care following involvement of Child Protective Services. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 164(5), 438443. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.54 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernard, K., Frost, A., Bennett, C. B., & Lindhiem, O. (2017). Maltreatment and diurnal cortisol regulation: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 78, 5767. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17(2), 271301. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050145 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyer, B. P., & Nelson, J. A. (2015). Longitudinal associations of childhood parenting and adolescent health: The mediating influence of social competence. Child Development, 86(3), 828843. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12347 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braehler, C., Holowka, D., Brunet, A., Beaulieu, S., Baptista, T., DebruilleI, B., Walker, C., & King, S. (2005). Diurnal cortisol in schizophrenia patients with childhood trauma. Schizophrenia Research, 79(2-3), 353354, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2004.07.007,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brendgen, M., Ouellet-Morin, I., Lupien, S. J., Vitaro, F., Dionne, G., & Boivin, M. (2017). Environmental influence of problematic social relationships on adolescents’ daily cortisol secretion: A monozygotic twin-difference study. Psychological Medicine, 47(3), 460470. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171600252X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewer-Smyth, K., Burgess, A. W., & Shults, J. (2004). Physical and sexual abuse, salivary cortisol, and neurologic correlates of violent criminal behavior in female prison inmates. Biological Psychiatry, 55(1), 2131. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00705-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brindle, R. C., Pearson, A., & Ginty, A. T. (2022). Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) relate to blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity to acute laboratory stress: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 134, 104530. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104530 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brummelte, S., Chau, C. M., Cepeda, I. L., Degenhardt, A., Weinberg, J., Synnes, A. R., & Grunau, R. E. (2015). Cortisol levels in former preterm children at school age are predicted by neonatal procedural pain-related stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 51, 151163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.09.018Get CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryson, H. E., Price, A. M., Goldfeld, S., & Mensah, F. (2021). Associations between social adversity and young children’s hair cortisol: A systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 127, 105176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105176 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunea, I. M., Szentágotai-Tătar, A., & Miu, A. C. (2017). Early-life adversity and cortisol response to social stress: A meta-analysis. Translational Psychiatry, 7(12 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-017-0032-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, K., Klaus, K., Edwards, L., & Pennington, K. (2017). Elevated cortisol awakening response associated with early life stress and impaired executive function in healthy adult males. Hormones and Behavior, 95, 1321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.07.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callaghan, B. L., & Tottenham, N. (2016). The stress acceleration hypothesis: Effects of early-life adversity on emotion circuits and behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 7, 7681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.11.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, E. D., & Chamberlain, R. M. (2005). Allostatic load and health disparities: A theoretical orientation. Research in Nursing & Health, 28(4), 306315. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.20084 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrion, V. G., Weems, C. F., Richert, K., Hoffman, B. C., & Reiss, A. L. (2010). Decreased prefrontal cortical volume associated with increased bedtime cortisol in traumatized youth. Biological Psychiatry, 68(5), 491493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.05.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, F. R., Stroud, C. B., Vrshek-Schallhorn, S., Doane, L. D., & Granger, D. A. (2017). Individual differences in early adolescents’ latent trait cortisol: Interaction of early adversity and 5-HTTLPR. Biological Psychology, 129, 815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.07.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chernego, D., Martin, C., Bernard, K., Muhamedrahimov, R., Gordon, M. K., & Dozier, M. (2019). Effects of institutional rearing on children’s diurnal cortisol production. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 106, 161164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.04.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiang, J. J., Tsai, K. M., Park, H., Bower, J. E., Almeida, D. M., Dahl, R. E., Irwin, M. R., Seeman, T. E., Fuligni, A. J. (2016). Daily family stress and HPA axis functioning during adolescence: The moderating role of sleep. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 71, 4353, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.05.009,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chida, Y., & Steptoe, A. (2009). Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 80(3), 265278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.10.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001a). Diverse patterns of neuroendocrine activity in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 13(3), 677693. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401003145 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001b). The impact of child maltreatment and psychopathology on neuroendocrine functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 13(4), 783804. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401004035 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2007). Personality, adrenal steroid hormones, and resilience in maltreated children: A multi-level perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 19(3), 787809. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407000399 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Gunnar, M. R., & Toth, S. L. (2010). The differential impacts of early physical and sexual abuse and internalizing problems on daytime cortisol rhythm in school-aged children. Child Development, 81(1), 252269. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01393.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Oshri, A. (2011). Interactive effects of corticotropin releasing hormone receptor 1, serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region, and child maltreatment on diurnal cortisol regulation and internalizing symptomatology. Development and Psychopathology, 23(4), 11251138. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000599 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cima, M., Smeets, T., & Jelicic, M. (2008). Self-reported trauma, cortisol levels, and aggression in psychopathic and non-psychopathic prison inmates. Biological Psychology, 78(1), 7586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.12.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clearfield, M. W., Carter-Rodriguez, A., Merali, A. R., & Shober, R. (2014). The effects of SES on infant and maternal diurnal salivary cortisol output. Infant Behavior and Development, 37(3), 298304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.04.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clowtis, L. M., Kang, D. H., Padhye, N. S., Rozmus, C., & Barratt, M. S. (2016). Biobehavioral factors in child health outcomes: The roles of maternal stress, maternal-child engagement, salivary cortisol, and salivary testosterone. Nursing Research, 65(5), 340351. https://doi.org/10.1097/NNR.0000000000000172 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd edn. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cordero, M. I., Moser, D. A., Manini, A., Suardi, F., Sancho-Rossignol, A., Torrisi, R., Rossier, M. F., Ansermet, Fçois, Dayer, A. G., Rusconi-Serpa, S., Schechter, D. S. (2017). Effects of interpersonal violence-related post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) on mother and child diurnal cortisol rhythm and cortisol reactivity to a laboratory stressor involving separation. Hormones and Behavior, 90, 1524, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.02.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cullen, A. E., Zunszain, P. A., Dickson, H., Roberts, R. E., Fisher, H. L., Pariante, C. M., & Laurens, K. R. (2014). Cortisol awakening response and diurnal cortisol among children at elevated risk for schizophrenia: Relationship to psychosocial stress and cognition. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 46, 113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.03.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daskalakis, N. P., Bagot, R. C., Parker, K. J., Vinkers, C. H., & de Kloet, E. R. (2013). The three-hit concept of vulnerability and resilience: Toward understanding adaptation to early-life adversity outcome. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(9), 18581873. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCaro, J. A., & Worthman, C. M. (2008). Return to school accompanied by changing associations between family ecology and cortisol. Developmental Psychobiology, 50(2), 183195. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20255 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2011). The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(7), 15621592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeSantis, A. S., Adam, E. K., Hawkley, L. C., Kudielka, B. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2015). Racial and ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol rhythms: Are they consistent over time? Psychosomatic Medicine, 77(1), 615. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000131 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donoho, C. J., Weigensberg, M. J., Emken, B. A., Hsu, J. W., & Spruijt-Metz, D. (2011). Stress and abdominal fat: Preliminary evidence of moderation by the cortisol awakening response in Hispanic peripubertal girls. Obesity, 19(5), 946952. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2010.287 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doom, J. R., Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2014). Longitudinal patterns of cortisol regulation differ in maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(11), 12061215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.08.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doom, J. R., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Dackis, M. N. (2013). Child maltreatment and gender interactions as predictors of differential neuroendocrine profiles. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(8), 14421454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doom, J. R., Cook, S. H., Sturza, J., Kaciroti, N., Gearhardt, A. N., Vazquez, D. M., Lumeng, J. C., Miller, A. L. (2018). Family conflict, chaos, and negative life events predict cortisol activity in low-income children. Developmental Psychobiology, 60(4), 364379, https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21602 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doom, J. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2013). Stress physiology and developmental psychopathology: Past, present, and future. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4pt2), 13591373. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000667 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dougherty, L. R., Klein, D. N., Olino, T. M., Dyson, M., & Rose, S. (2009). Increased waking salivary cortisol and depression risk in preschoolers: The role of maternal history of melancholic depression and early child temperament. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(12), 14951503. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02116.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dougherty, L. R., Smith, V. C., Olino, T. M., Dyson, M. W., Bufferd, S. J., Rose, S. A., & Klein, D. N. (2013). Maternal psychopathology and early child temperament predict young children’s salivary cortisol 3 years later. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(4), 531542. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9703-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dozier, M., Manni, M., Gordon, M. K., Peloso, E., Gunnar, M. R., Stovall-McClough, K. C., Eldreth, D., Levine, S. (2006). Foster children’s diurnal production of cortisol: An exploratory study. Child Maltreatment, 11(2), 189197, https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559505285779,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315(7109), 629634. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elhassan, M. E., Miller, A. L., Vazquez, D. M., & Lumeng, J. C. (2015). Associations of prenatal and perinatal factors with cortisol diurnal pattern and reactivity to stress at preschool age among children living in poverty. Journal of Clinical Research in Pediatric Endocrinology, 7(2), 114120. https://doi.org/10.4274/jcrpe.1685 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellenbogen, M. A., & Hodgins, S. (2009). Structure provided by parents in middle childhood predicts cortisol reactivity in adolescence among the offspring of parents with bipolar disorder and controls. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(5), 773785. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.12.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellenbogen, M. A., Hodgins, S., & Walker, C. D. (2004). High levels of cortisol among adolescent offspring of parents with bipolar disorder: A pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(1), 99106. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00135-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellenbogen, M. A., Hodgins, S., Walker, C. D., Couture, S., & Adam, S. (2006). Daytime cortisol and stress reactivity in the offspring of parents with bipolar disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(10), 11641180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.08.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engert, V., Efanov, S. I., Dedovic, K., Dagher, A., & Pruessner, J. C. (2011). Increased cortisol awakening response and afternoon/evening cortisol output in healthy young adults with low early life parental care. Psychopharmacology, 214(1), 261268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-1918-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, C. M., Houfek, J. F., Rice, M. J., Weiss, S. J., French, J. A., Kupzyk, K. A., Hammer, S. J., Pullen, C. H. (2019). Early life adversity and depressive symptoms predict cortisol in p’egnancy. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 23(3), 379389, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00737-019-00983-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Essex, M. J., Shirtcliff, E. A., Burk, L. R., Ruttle, P. L., Klein, M. H., Slattery, M. J., Kalin, N. H., Armstrong, J. M. (2011). Influence of early life stress on later hypothalamic-pituitary–adrenal axis functioning and its covariation with mental health symptoms: A study of the allostatic process from childhood into adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 23(4), 10391058, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000484,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, B. E., Greaves-Lord, K., Euser, A. S., Franken, I. H., & Huizink, A. C. (2013). Cortisol levels in children of parents with a substance use disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(10), 21092120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.03.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, B. E., van der Ende, J., Greaves-Lord, K., Huizink, A. C., Beijers, R., & de Weerth, C. (2020). Urbanicity, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning, and behavioral and emotional problems in children: A path analysis. BMC Psychology, 8(12 https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-019-0364-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. W. (2003). A multimethodological analysis of cumulative risk and allostatic load among rural children. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 924933. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.924 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrell, , Doolin, K., O’ Leary, N., Jairaj, C., Roddy, D., Tozzi, L., Morris, D., Harkin, A., Frodl, T., Nemoda, Zófia, Szyf, M., Booij, L., O’Keane, V. (2018). DNA methylation differences at the glucocorticoid receptor gene in depression are related to functional alterations in hypothalamic-pituitary–adrenal axis activity and to early life emotional abuse. Psychiatry Research, 265, 341348, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.04.064 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., & Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figueroa, W. S., Zoccola, P. M., Manigault, A. W., Hamilton, K. R., Scanlin, M. C., & Johnson, R. C. (2021). Daily stressors and diurnal cortisol among sexual and gender minority young adults. Health Psychology, 40(2), 145154. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0001054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, P. A., Stoolmiller, M., Gunnar, M. R., & Burraston, B. O. (2007). Effects of a therapeutic intervention for foster preschoolers on diurnal cortisol activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32(8-10), 892905. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.06.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flannery, J. E., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Shapiro, M., Goff, B., Caldera, C., Louie, J., Gee, D. G., Telzer, E. H., Humphreys, K. L., Lumian, D. S., Tottenham, N. (2017). Diurnal cort-sol after early institutional care - Age matters. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 160166, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.03.006,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flom, M., St. John, A. M., Meyer, J. S., & Tarullo, A. R. (2017). Infant hair cortisol: Associations with salivary cortisol and environmental context. Developmental Psychobiology, 59(1), 2638. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21449 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fogelman, N., & Canli, T. (2018). Early life stress and cortisol: A meta-analysis. Hormones and Behavior, 98, 6376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.12.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foland-Ross, L. C., Kircanski, K., & Gotlib, I. H. (2014). Coping with having a depressed mother: The role of stress and coping in hypothalamic-pituitary–adrenal axis dysfunction in girls at familial risk for major depression. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4pt2), 14011409. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414001102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, E., Dettenborn, L., & Kirschbaum, C. (2009). The cortisol awakening response (CAR): Facts and future directions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72(1), 6773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, A., Moehler, E., Resch, F., & Kaess, M. (2017). The effect of a maternal history of childhood abuse on adrenocortical attunement in mothers and their toddlers. Developmental Psychobiology, 59(5), 639652. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21531 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Funder, D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2(2), 156168. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919847202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabard-Durnam, L. J., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2019). Do sensitive periods exist for exposure to adversity? Biological Psychiatry, 85(10), 789791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.03.975 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gartstein, M. A., Seamon, E., Thompson, S. F., & Lengua, L. J. (2018). Parenting matters: Moderation of biological and community risk for obesity. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 56, 2134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2018.01.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerritsen, L., Milaneschi, Y., Vinkers, C. H., van Hemert, A. M., van Velzen, L., Schmaal, L., Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2017). HPA axis genes, and their interaction with childhood maltreatment, are related to cortisol levels and stress-related phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(12), 24462455, https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2017.118,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B. L., Perlman, G., Kotov, R., Broderick, J. E., Liu, K., Ruggero, C., & Klein, D. N. (2017). Etiologic specificity of waking cortisol: Links with maternal history of depression and anxiety in adolescent girls. Journal of Affective Disorders, 208, 103109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.079 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gow, R., Thomson, S., Rieder, M., Van Uum, S., & Koren, G. (2010). An assessment of cortisol analysis in hair and its clinical applications. Forensic Science International, 196(1-3), 3237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.forsciint.2009.12.040 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granger, D. A., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Hibel, L. C., Teisl, M., & Flores, E. (2007). Blood contamination in children’s saliva: Prevalence, stability, and impact on the measurement of salivary cortisol, testosterone, and dehydroepiandrosterone. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32(6), 724733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.05.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, B., & Meyer, D. (2021). Childhood adversity and hair cortisol concentration: A systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 131, 105536. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105536 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 145173. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., DePasquale, C. E., Reid, B. M., Donzella, B., & Miller, B. S. (2019). Pubertal stress recalibration reverses the effects of early life stress in postinstitutionalized children. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 116(48), 2398423988. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1909699116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B. (2002). Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27(1-2), 199220. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(01)00045-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M. R., Morison, S. J., Chisholm, K. I. M., & Schuder, M. (2001). Salivary cortisol levels in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology, 13(3), 611628. https://doi.org/10.1017/s095457940100311x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustafsson, P. E., Anckarsäter, H., Lichtenstein, P., Nelson, N., & Gustafsson, P. A. (2010). Does quantity have a quality all its own? Cumulative adversity and up-and down-regulation of circadian salivary cortisol levels in healthy children. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(9), 14101415. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.04.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Habersaat, S., Borghini, A., Nessi, J., Forcada-Guex, M., Müller-Nix, C., Pierrehumbert, B., & Ansermet, F. (2014). Effects of perinatal stress and maternal traumatic stress on the cortisol regulation of preterm infants. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 27(4), 488491. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.21939 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., O'Brien, J. R., & Zalewski, M. (2018). Enduring association between parenting and cortisol: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 89(5), 14851503. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13077 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakamata, Y., Suzuki, Y., Kobashikawa, H., & Hori, H. (2022). Neurobiology of early life adversity: A systematic review of meta-analyses towards an integrative account of its neurobiological trajectories to mental disorders. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 65, 100994. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2022.100994 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halligan, S. L., Herbert, J., Goodyer, I. M., & Murray, L. (2004). Exposure to postnatal depression predicts elevated cortisol in adolescent offspring. Biological Psychiatry, 55(4), 376381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.09.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanson, M. D., & Chen, E. (2010). Daily stress, cortisol, and sleep: The moderating role of childhood psychosocial environments. Health Psychology, 29(4), 394402. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019879 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, M. A., Cox, S. R., Brett, C. E., Deary, I. J., & MacLullich, A. M. (2017). Stress in childhood, adolescence and early adulthood, and cortisol levels in older age. Stress - the International Journal on The Biology of Stress, 20(2), 140148. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2017.1289168 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, M. W., Farrell, A. K., Imami, L., Carré, J. M., & Slatcher, R. B. (2019). Maternal attachment avoidance is linked to youth diurnal cortisol slopes in children with asthma. Attachment & Human Development, 21(1), 2337. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2018.1541514 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertzman, C. (1999). The biological embedding of early experience and its effects on health in adulthood. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 896(1), 8595. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08107.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hibel, L. C., Mercado, E., & Valentino, K. (2019). Child maltreatment and mother-child transmission of stress physiology. Child Maltreatment, 24(4), 340352. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559519826295 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hibel, L. C., Nuttall, A. K., & Valentino, K. (2020). Intimate partner violence indirectly dysregulates child diurnal adrenocortical functioning through positive parenting. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 80(1), 2841. https://doi.org/10.1002/jdn.10002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327(7414), 557560. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holleman, M., Vreeburg, S. A., Dekker, J. J., & Penninx, B. W. (2012). The relationships of working conditions, recent stressors and childhood trauma with salivary cortisol levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(6), 801809. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.09.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosseini-Kamkar, N., Lowe, C., & Morton, J. B. (2021). The differential calibration of the HPA axis as a function of trauma versus adversity: A systematic review and p-curve meta-analyses. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 127, 54135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoyt, L. T., Ehrlich, K. B., Cham, H., & Adam, E. K. (2016). Balancing scientific accuracy and participant burden: Testing the impact of sampling intensity on diurnal cortisol indices. Stress - the International Journal on The Biology of Stress, 19(5), 476485. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2016.1206884 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoyt, L. T., Zeiders, K. H., Chaku, N., Niu, L., & Cook, S. H. (2021). Identifying diurnal cortisol profiles among young adults: Physiological signatures of mental health trajectories. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 128, 105204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2021.105204 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huizink, A. C., Greaves-Lord, K., Oldehinkel, A. J., Ormel, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2009). Hypothalamic-pituitary–adrenal axis and smoking and drinking onset among adolescents: The longitudinal cohort TRacking Adolescents' Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS). Addiction, 104(11), 19271936. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02685.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, A. L., Minnis, H., & Wilson, P. (2011). Altered stress responses in children exposed to early adversity: A systematic review of salivary cortisol studies. Stress - the International Journal on The Biology of Stress, 14(6), 614626. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2011.577848 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hustedt, J. T., Vu, J. A., Bargreen, K. N., Hallam, R. A., & Han, M. (2017). Early Head Start families’ experiences with stress: Understanding variations within a high-risk, low-income sample. Infant Mental Health Journal, 38(5), 602616. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21667 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huynh, V. W., Guan, S. S. A., Almeida, D. M., McCreath, H., & Fuligni, A. J. (2016). Everyday discrimination and diurnal cortisol during adolescence. Hormones and Behavior, 80, 7681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.01.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isenhour, J., Raby, K. L., & Dozier, M. (2020). The persistent associations between early institutional care and diurnal cortisol outcomes among children adopted internationally. Developmental Psychobiology, 63(5), 11561166. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22069 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, A. E., Bruce, J., Tarullo, A. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2011). Growth delay as an index of allostatic load in young children: Predictions to disinhibited social approach and diurnal cortisol activity. Development and Psychopathology, 23(3), 859871. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000356 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, A. J., & Tottenham, N. (2015). Regulatory skill as a resilience factor for adults with a history of foster care: A pilot study. Developmental Psychobiology, 57(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.2122 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, D., Policelli, J., Li, M., Dharamsi, A., Hu, Q., Sheridan, M. A., & Wade, M. (2021). Associations of early-life threat and deprivation with executive functioning in childhood and adolescence: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(11), e212511. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.2511 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keeshin, B. R., Strawn, J. R., Out, D., Granger, D. A., & Putnam, F. W. (2014). Cortisol awakening response in adolescents with acute sexual abuse related posttraumatic stress disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 31(2), 107114. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22154 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kertes, D. A., Gunnar, M. R., Madsen, N. J., & Long, J. D. (2008). Early deprivation and home basal cortisol levels: A study of internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology, 20(2), 473491. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000230 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khoury, J. E., Enlow, M. B., Plamondon, A., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2019). The association between adversity and hair cortisol levels in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 103, 104117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.01.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiel, E. J., Hummel, A. C., & Luebbe, A. M. (2015). Cortisol secretion and change in sleep problems in early childhood: Moderation by maternal overcontrol. Biological Psychology, 107, 5260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, L. S., Colich, N. L., LeMoult, J., Humphreys, K. L., Ordaz, S. J., Price, A. N., & Gotlib, I. H. (2017). The impact of the severity of early life stress on diurnal cortisol: The role of puberty. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 77, 6874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.024Get CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kliewer, W. (2006). Violence exposure and cortisol responses in urban youth. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 13(2), 109120. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327558ijbm1302_2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kliewer, W., Reid-Quiñones, K., Shields, B. J., & Foutz, L. (2009). Multiple risks, emotion regulation skill, and cortisol in low-income African American youth: A prospective study. Journal of Black Psychology, 35(1), 2443. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095798408323355 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knapp, G., & Hartung, J. (2003). Improved tests for a random effects meta-regression with a single covariate. Statistics in Medicine, 22(17), 26932710. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.1482 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohrt, B. A., Worthman, C. M., Ressler, K. J., Mercer, K. B., Upadhaya, N., Koirala, S., & Binder, E. B. (2015). Cross-cultural gene− environment interactions in depression, post-traumatic stress disorder, and the cortisol awakening response: FKBP5 polymorphisms and childhood trauma in South Asia: GxE interactions in South Asia. International Review of Psychiatry, 27(3), 180196. https://doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2015.1020052 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korpa, T., Pervanidou, P., Angeli, E., Apostolakou, F., Papanikolaou, K., Papassotiriou, I., & Kolaitis, G. (2017). Mothers' parenting stress is associated with salivary cortisol profiles in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Stress - the International Journal on The Biology of Stress, 20(2), 149158. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2017.1303472 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koss, K. J., Hostinar, C. E., Donzella, B., & Gunnar, M. R. (2014). Social deprivation and the HPA axis in early development. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50, 113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.028 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koss, K. J., Mliner, S. B., Donzella, B., & Gunnar, M. R. (2016). Early adversity, hypocortisolism, and behavior problems at school entry: A study of internationally adopted children. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 66, 3138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.12.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhlman, K. R., Geiss, E. G., Vargas, I., & Lopez-Duran, N. L. (2015). Differential associations between childhood trauma subtypes and adolescent HPA-axis functioning. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 54, 103114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.01.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhlman, K. R., Vargas, I., Geiss, E. G., & Lopez-Duran, N. L. (2015). Age of trauma onset and HPA axis dysregulation among trauma-exposed youth. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 28(6), 572579. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.22054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumsta, R., Schlotz, W., Golm, D., Moser, D., Kennedy, M., Knights, N., Kreppner, J., Maughan, B., Rutter, M., Sonuga-Barke, E. (2017). HPA axis dysregulation in adult adoptees twenty years after severe institutional deprivation in childhood. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 86, 196202, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.09.021,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwak, Y., Taylor, Z. E., Anaya, L. Y., Feng, Y., Evich, C. D., & Jones, B. L. (2017). Cumulative family stress and diurnal cortisol responses in Midwest Latino families. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 39(1), 8297. https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986316684130 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laceulle, O. M., Nederhof, E., van Aken, M. A., & Ormel, J. (2017). Adversity-driven changes in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning during adolescence. The trails study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 85, 4955. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.08.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, C. L. J., Lee, D. Y. H., & Leung, M. O. Y. (2021). Childhood adversities and salivary cortisol responses to the Trier Social Stress Test: A systematic review of studies using the Children Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ). International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(1), 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18010029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsson, C. A., Gullberg, B., Råstam, L., & Lindblad, U. (2009). Salivary cortisol differs with age and sex and shows inverse associations with WHR in Swedish women: A cross-sectional study. BMC Endocrine Disorders, 9(1 https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6823-9-16 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laurent, H. K., Leve, L. D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., Shaw, D. S., Fisher, P. A., & Reiss, D. (2013). Effects of parental depressive symptoms on child adjustment moderated by hypothalamic pituitary adrenal activity: Within-and between-family risk. Child Development, 84(2), 528542. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01859.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laurent, H. K., Leve, L. D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., Shaw, D. S., Harold, G. T., & Reiss, D. (2013). Effects of prenatal and postnatal parent depressive symptoms on adopted child HPA regulation: Independent and moderated influences. Developmental Psychology, 49(5), 876886. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028800 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laurent, H. K., Neiderhiser, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., Shaw, D. S., Fisher, P. A., Reiss, D., & Leve, L. D. (2014). Stress system development from age 4.5 to 6: Family environment predictors and adjustment implications of HPA activity stability versus change. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(3), 340354. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21103 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeMoult, J., Chen, M. C., Foland-Ross, L. C., Burley, H. W., & Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Concordance of mother-daughter diurnal cortisol production: Understanding the intergenerational transmission of risk for depression. Biological Psychology, 108, 98104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeMoult, J., Ordaz, S. J., Kircanski, K., Singh, M. K., & Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Predicting first onset of depression in young girls: Interaction of diurnal cortisol and negative life events. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(4), 850859. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000087 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leneman, K. B., Donzella, B., Desjardins, C. D., Miller, B. S., & Gunnar, M. R. (2018). The slope of cortisol from awakening to 30 min post-wake in post-institutionalized children and early adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 96, 9399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.06.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lengua, L. J., Zalewski, M., Fisher, P., & Moran, L. (2013). Does HPA-Axis dysregulation account for the effects of income on effortful control and adjustment in preschool children? Infant and Child Development, 22(5), 439458. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1805 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppert, K. A., Smith, V. C., Merwin, S. M., Kushner, M., & Dougherty, L. R. (2018). Cortisol rhythm in preschoolers: Relations with maternal depression and child temperament. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 40(3), 386401. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-018-9650-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Letourneau, N., Watson, B., Duffett-Leger, L., Hegadoren, K., & Tryphonopoulos, P. (2011). Cortisol patterns of depressed mothers and their infants are related to maternal-infant interactive behaviours. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 29(5), 439459. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646838.2011.649474 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, K., Ruggero, C. J., Goldstein, B., Klein, D. N., Perlman, G., Broderick, J., & Kotov, R. (2016). Elevated cortisol in healthy female adolescent offspring of mothers with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 40, 3743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.04.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loman, M. M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2010). Early experience and the development of stress reactivity and regulation in children. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 867876. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovallo, W. R., Cohoon, A. J., Acheson, A., Sorocco, K. H., & Vincent, A. S. (2019). Blunted stress reactivity reveals vulnerability to early life adversity in young adults with a family history of alcoholism. Addiction, 114(5), 798806. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14501 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lumeng, J. C., Miller, A., Peterson, K. E., Kaciroti, N., Sturza, J., Rosenblum, K., & Vazquez, D. M. (2014). Diurnal cortisol pattern, eating behaviors and overweight in low-income preschool-aged children. Appetite, 73, 6572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., King, S., Meaney, M. J., & McEwen, B. S. (2001). Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Development and Psychopathology, 13(3), 653676. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401003133 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangold, D., Wand, G., Javors, M., & Mintz, J. (2010). Acculturation, childhood trauma and the cortisol awakening response in Mexican-American adults. Hormones and Behavior, 58(4), 637646. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marceau, K., Ram, N., Neiderhiser, J. M., Laurent, H. K., Shaw, D. S., Fisher, P., & Leve, L. D. (2013). Disentangling the effects of genetic, prenatal and parenting influences on children’s cortisol variability. Stress - the International Journal on The Biology of Stress, 16(6), 607615. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2013.825766 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsman, R., Nederhof, E., Rosmalen, J. G., Oldehinkel, A. J., Ormel, J., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2012). Family environment is associated with HPA-axis activity in adolescents. The TRAILS study. Biological Psychology, 89(2), 460466. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.12.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, C. G., Bruce, J., & Fisher, P. A. (2012). Racial and ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol rhythms in preadolescents: The role of parental psychosocial risk and monitoring. Hormones and Behavior, 61(5), 661668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLachlan, K., Rasmussen, C., Oberlander, T. F., Loock, C., Pei, J., Andrew, G., Reynolds, J., Weinberg, J. (2016). Dysregulation of the cortisol diurnal rhythm following prenatal alcohol exposure and early life adversity. Alcohol, 53, 918, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.alcohol.2016.03.003,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A. (2016). Future directions in childhood adversity and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 45(3), 361382. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1110823 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2016). Beyond cumulative risk: A dimensional approach to childhood adversity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(4), 239245. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416655883 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, K. A., Weissman, D., & Bitrán, D. (2019). Childhood adversity and neural development: A systematic review. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 277312. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084950 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meinlschmidt, G., & Heim, C. (2005). Decreased cortisol awakening response after early loss experience. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(6), 568576. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.01.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merwin, S. Psychological and neurobiological outcomes of parent-child adrenocortical concordance, 2017, (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from Digital Repository at the University of Maryland, https://doi.org/10.13016/M2P84402R CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merwin, S. M., Barrios, C., Smith, V. C., Lemay, E. P. Jr., & Dougherty, L. R. (2018). Outcomes of early parent-child adrenocortical attunement in the high-risk offspring of depressed parents. Developmental Psychobiology, 60(4), 468482. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21623 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merwin, S. M., Leppert, K. A., Smith, V. C., & Dougherty, L. R. (2017). Parental depression and parent and child stress physiology: Moderation by parental hostility. Developmental Psychobiology, 59(8), 9971009. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21556 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merwin, S. M., Smith, V. C., Kushner, M., Lemay, E. P. Jr, & Dougherty, L. R. (2017). Parent-child adrenocortical concordance in early childhood: The moderating role of parental depression and child temperament. Biological Psychology, 124, 100110, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.01.013,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messerli-Bürgy, N., Stülb, K., Kakebeeke, T. H., Arhab, A., Zysset, A. E., Leeger-Aschmann, C. S., Schmutz, E. A., Meyer, A. H., Ehlert, U., Garcia-Burgos, D., Kriemler, S., Jenni, O. G., Puder, J. J., & Munsch, S. (2018). Emotional eating is related with temperament but not with stress biomarkers in preschool children. Appetite, 120, 256264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.08.032Get,CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, E. M., Dmitrieva, J., Hurwich-Reiss, E., Badanes, L., Mendoza, M. M., Perreira, K. M., & Watamura, S. E. (2018). Evidence for a physiologic home-school gap in children of Latina immigrants. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 52, 86100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.03.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. L., Song, J. H., Sturza, J., Lumeng, J. C., Rosenblum, K., Kaciroti, N., & Vazquez, D. M. (2017). Child cortisol moderates the association between family routines and emotion regulation in low-income children. Developmental Psychobiology, 59(1), 99110. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21471 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Parker, K. J. (2011). Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 137(6), 959997. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024768 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Zhou, E. S. (2007). If it goes up, must it come down? Chronic stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 2545. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.25 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, K. F., Arbel, R., Shapiro, L. S., Han, S. C., & Margolin, G. (2018). Does the cortisol awakening response link childhood adversity to adult BMI? Health Psychology, 37(6), 526529. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000601 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, K. F., Margolin, G., Shapiro, L. S., & Timmons, A. C. (2017). Adolescent life stress and the cortisol awakening response: The moderating roles of attachment and sex. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 27(1), 3448. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12250 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2010). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. International Journal of Surgery, 8(5), 336341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2010.02.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monteleone, A. M., Monteleone, P., Serino, I., Scognamiglio, P., Di Genio, M., & Maj, M. (2015). Childhood trauma and cortisol awakening response in symptomatic patients with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48(6), 615621. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22375 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, L., Halligan, S. L., Goodyer, I., & Herbert, J. (2010). Disturbances in early parenting of depressed mothers and cortisol secretion in offspring: A preliminary study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 122(3), 218223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray-Close, D., Han, G., Cicchetti, D., Crick, N. R., & Rogosch, F. A. (2008). Neuroendocrine regulation and physical and relational aggression: The moderating roles of child maltreatment and gender. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 11601176. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012564 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Netherton, C., Goodyer, I., Tamplin, A., & Herbert, J. (2004). Salivary cortisol and dehydroepiandrosterone in relation to puberty and gender. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(2), 125140. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00150-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, T. G., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Heron, J., Golding, J., Adams, D., & Glover, V. (2005). Prenatal anxiety predicts individual differences in cortisol in pre-adolescent children. Biological Psychiatry, 58(3), 211217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.032 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Loughlin, J. I., Rellini, A. H., & Brotto, L. A. (2020). How does childhood trauma impact women’s sexual desire? Role of depression, stress, and cortisol. The Journal of Sex Research, 57(7), 836847. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2019.1693490 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ostiguy, C. S., Ellenbogen, M. A., Walker, C. D., Walker, E. F., & Hodgins, S. (2011). Sensitivity to stress among the offspring of parents with bipolar disorder: A study of daytime cortisol levels. Psychological Medicine, 41(11), 24472457. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711000523 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ouellet-Morin, I., Dionne, G., Pérusse, D., Lupien, S. J., Arseneault, L., Barr, R. G., & Boivin, M. (2009). Daytime cortisol secretion in 6-month-old twins: Genetic and environmental contributions as a function of early familial adversity. Biological Psychiatry, 65(5), 409416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pawluski, J. L., Brain, U. M., Underhill, C. M., Hammond, G. L., & Oberlander, T. F. (2012). Prenatal SSRI exposure alters neonatal corticosteroid binding globulin, infant cortisol levels, and emerging HPA function. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(7), 10191028. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.11.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peckins, M. K., Roberts, A. G., Hein, T. C., Hyde, L. W., Mitchell, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLanahan, S. S., Monk, C. S., Lopez-Duran, N. L. (2020). Violence exposure and social deprivation is associated with cortisol reactivity in urban adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 111, 104426, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104426,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pendry, P., & Adam, E. K. (2007). Associations between parents' marital functioning, maternal parenting quality, maternal emotion and child cortisol levels. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(3), 218231. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025407074634 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, H., Long, Y., Li, J., Guo, Y., Wu, H., Yang, Y. L., Ding, Y., He, J., Ning, Y. (2014). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning and dysfunctional attitude in depressed patients with and without childhood neglect. BMC Psychiatry, 14(45, https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-14-45 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, N. B., DePasquale, C. E., Fisher, P. H., & Gunnar, M. R. (2019). Comparison of institutionally reared and maltreated children on socioemotional and biological functioning. Child Maltreatment, 24(3), 235243. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559518823074 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, R. A., & Brown, S. P. (2005). On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(1), 175181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philbrook, L. E., & Teti, D. M. (2016). Associations between bedtime and nighttime parenting and infant cortisol in the first year. Developmental Psychobiology, 58(8), 10871100. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21442 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitula, C. E., DePasquale, C. E., Mliner, S. B., & Gunnar, M. R. (2019). Peer problems among postinstitutionalized, internationally adopted children: Relations to hypocortisolism, parenting quality, and ADHD symptoms. Child Development, 90(3), e339e355. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12986 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plant, D. T., Pawlby, S., Sharp, D., Zunszain, P. A., & Pariante, C. M. (2016). Prenatal maternal depression is associated with offspring inflammation at 25 years: A prospective longitudinal cohort study. Translational Psychiatry, 6(11), e936e936. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.155 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruessner, M., Vracotas, N., Joober, R., Pruessner, J. C., & Malla, A. K. (2013). Blunted cortisol awakening response in men with first episode psychosis: Relationship to parental bonding. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(2), 229240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puetz, V. B., Zweerings, J., Dahmen, B., Ruf, C., Scharke, W., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., & Konrad, K. (2016). Multidimensional assessment of neuroendocrine and psychopathological profiles in maltreated youth. Journal of Neural Transmission, 123(9), 10951106. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-016-1509-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quevedo, K., Doty, J., Roos, L., & Anker, J. J. (2017). The cortisol awakening response and anterior cingulate cortex function in maltreated depressed versus non-maltreated depressed youth. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 86, 8795. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.09.001Get CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quevedo, K., Johnson, A. E., Loman, M. L., LaFavor, T. L., & Gunnar, M. (2012). The confluence of adverse early experience and puberty on the cortisol awakening response. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 36(1), 1928. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025411406860 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raffington, L., Prindle, J., Keresztes, A., Binder, J., Heim, C., & Shing, Y. L. (2018). Blunted cortisol stress reactivity in low-income children relates to lower memory function. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 90, 110121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.02.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raffington, L., Schmiedek, F., Heim, C., & Shing, Y. L. (2018). Cognitive control moderates parenting stress effects on children’s diurnal cortisol. PLoS One, 13(1), e0191215. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0191215 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raymond, C., Marin, M. F., Wolosianski, V., Journault, A. A., Longpré, C., Leclaire, S., & Lupien, S. J. (2021). Early childhood adversity and HPA axis activity in adulthood: The importance of considering minimal age at exposure. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 124, 105042. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.105042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichl, C., Heyer, A., Brunner, R., Parzer, P., Völker, J. M., Resch, F., & Kaess, M. (2016). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, childhood adversity and adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 74, 203211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.09.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogosch, F. A., Dackis, M. N., & Cicchetti, D. (2011). Child maltreatment and allostatic load: Consequences for physical and mental health in children from low-income families. Development and Psychopathology, 23(4), 11071124. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000587 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roisman, G. I., Susman, E., Barnett-Walker, K., Booth-LaForce, C., Owen, M. T., Belsky, J., Bradley, R. H., Houts, R., Steinberg, L., The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2009). Early family and child-care antecedents of awakening cortisol levels in adolescence. Child Development, 80(3), 907920, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01305.x,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86(3), 638641. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russ, S. J., Herbert, J., Cooper, P., Gunnar, M. R., Goodyer, I., Croudace, T., & Murray, L. (2012). Cortisol levels in response to starting school in children at increased risk for social phobia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(4), 462474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, R., Booth, S., Spathis, A., Mollart, S., & Clow, A. (2016). Use of salivary diurnal cortisol as an outcome measure in randomised controlled trials: A systematic review. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 50(2), 210236. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-015-9753-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M., & Munck, A. U. (2000). How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Reviews, 21(1), 5589. https://doi.org/10.1210/edrv.21.1.0389 Google ScholarPubMed
Seidenfaden, D., Knorr, U., Soendergaard, M. G., Poulsen, H. E., Fink-Jensen, A., Jorgensen, M. B., & Jorgensen, A. (2017). The relationship between self-reported childhood adversities, adulthood psychopathology and psychological stress markers in patients with schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 72, 4855. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.09.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selye, H. (1946). The general adaptation syndrome and the diseases of adaptation. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology, 6(2), 117230. https://doi.org/10.1210/jcem-6-2-117 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shirtcliff, E. A., & Essex, M. J. (2008). Concurrent and longitudinal associations of basal and diurnal cortisol with mental health symptoms in early adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 50(7), 690703. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20336 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simmons, J. G., Byrne, M. L., Schwartz, O. S., Whittle, S. L., Sheeber, L., Kaess, M., & Allen, N. B. (2015). Dual-axis hormonal covariation in adolescence and the moderating influence of prior trauma and aversive maternal parenting. Developmental Psychobiology, 57(6), 670687. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21275 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, S. S., Beijers, R., Cillessen, A. H., & de Weerth, C. (2015). Development of the cortisol circadian rhythm in the light of stress early in life. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 292300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeets, T., Geraerts, E., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2007). Delayed recall of childhood sexual abuse memories and the awakening rise and diurnal pattern of cortisol. Psychiatry Research, 152(2-3), 197204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.07.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St. John, A. M., Kao, K., Liederman, J., Grieve, P. G., & Tarullo, A. R. (2017). Maternal cortisol slope at 6 months predicts infant cortisol slope and EEG power at 12 months. Developmental Psychobiology, 59(6), 787801. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21540 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stalder, T., Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Adam, E. K., Pruessner, J. C., Wüst, S., Dockray, S., Smyth, N., Evans, P., Hellhammer, D. H., Miller, R., Wetherell, M. A., Lupien, S. J., Clow, A. (2016). Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: Expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 63, 414432, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.10.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stalder, T., Lupien, S. J., Kudielka, B. M., Adam, E. K., Pruessner, J. C., Wüst, S., Dockray, S., Smyth, N., Evans, P., Kirschbaum, C., Miller, R., Wetherell, M. A., Finke, J. B., Klucken, T., & Clow, A. (2022). Evaluation and update of the expert consensus guidelines for the assessment of the cortisol awakening response (CAR). Psychoneuroendocrinology, 146, 105946, https://doi.org/doi:1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105946 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starr, L. R., Dienes, K., Stroud, C. B., Shaw, Z. A., Li, Y. I., Mlawer, F., & Huang, M. (2017). Childhood adversity moderates the influence of proximal episodic stress on the cortisol awakening response and depressive symptoms in adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 29(5), 18771893. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417001468 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th edn. Pearson.Google Scholar
Tarullo, A. R., St. John, A. M., & Meyer, J. S. (2017). Chronic stress in the mother-infant dyad: Maternal hair cortisol, infant salivary cortisol and interactional synchrony. Infant Behavior and Development, 47, 92102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.03.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, L. K., Weems, C. F., Costa, N. M., & Carrión, V. G. (2009). Loss and the experience of emotional distress in childhood. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 14(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1080/15325020802173843 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theall, K. P., Shirtcliff, E. A., Dismukes, A. R., Wallace, M., & Drury, S. S. (2017). Association between neighborhood violence and biological stress in children. JAMA Pediatrics, 171(1), 5360. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.2321 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, S. F., Zalewski, M., Kiff, C. J., & Lengua, L. J. (2018). A state-trait model of cortisol in early childhood: Contextual and parental predictors of stable and time-varying effects. Hormones and Behavior, 98, 198209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.12.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valentino, K., Hibel, L. C., Cummings, E. M., Nuttall, A. K., Comas, M., & McDonnell, C. G. (2015). Maternal elaborative reminiscing mediates the effect of child maltreatment on behavioral and physiological functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4 Pt 2), 15151526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000917 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Cauter, E. (1990). Diurnal and ultradian rhythms in human endocrine function: A minireview. Hormone Research in Paediatrics, 34(2), 4553. https://doi.org/10.1159/000181794 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Bergh, B. R., Van Calster, B., Smits, T., Van Huffel, S., & Lagae, L. (2008). Antenatal maternal anxiety is related to HPA-axis dysregulation and self-reported depressive symptoms in adolescence: A prospective study on the fetal origins of depressed mood. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(3), 536545. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301450 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Vegt, E. J., van der Ende, J., Kirschbaum, C., Verhulst, F. C., & Tiemeier, H. (2009). Early neglect and abuse predict diurnal cortisol patterns in adults: A study of international adoptees. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(5), 660669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.11.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vänskä, M., Punamäki, R. L., Lindblom, J., Tolvanen, A., Flykt, M., Unkila-Kallio, L., & Tiitinen, A. (2016). Timing of early maternal mental health and child cortisol regulation. Infant and Child Development, 25(6), 461483. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1948 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VanTieghem, M., Korom, M., Flannery, J., Choy, T., Caldera, C., Humphreys, K. L., Gabard-Durnam, L., Goff, B., Gee, D. G., Telzer, E. H., Shapiro, M., Louie, J. Y., Fareri, D. S., Bolger, N., Tottenham, N. (2021). Longitudinal changes in amygdala, hippocampus and cortisol development following early caregiving adversity. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 48, 100916, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100916 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vreeburg, S. A., Hartman, C. A., Hoogendijk, W. J., van Dyck, R., Zitman, F. G., Ormel, J., & Penninx, B. W. (2010). Parental history of depression or anxiety and the cortisol awakening response. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(3), 180185. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.109.076869 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissbecker, I., Floyd, A., Dedert, E., Salmon, P., & Sephton, S. (2006). Childhood trauma and diurnal cortisol disruption in fibromyalgia syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(3), 312324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.08.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wielaard, I., Schaakxs, R., Comijs, H. C., Stek, M. L., & Rhebergen, D. (2018). The influence of childhood abuse on cortisol levels and the cortisol awakening response in depressed and nondepressed older adults. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 19(6), 440449. https://doi.org/10.1080/15622975.2016.1274829 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, S. L., & Mann, A. K. (2017). Sexual and gender minority health disparities as a social issue: How stigma and intergroup relations can explain and reduce health disparities. Journal of Social Issues, 73(3), 450461. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12225 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, D. B., Practical meta-analysis effect size calculator, n.d., Campbell Collaboration. https://www.campbellcollaboration.org/escalc/html/EffectSizeCalculator-R-main.php Google Scholar
Wright, J. L., Jarvis, J. N., Pachter, L. M., & Walker-Harding, L. R. (2020). Racism as a public health issue, APS racism series: At the intersection of equity, science, and social justice. Pediatric Research, 88(5), 696698. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-020-01141-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, R., Engel, S. M., Brand, S. R., Seckl, J., Marcus, S. M., & Berkowitz, G. S. (2005). Transgenerational effects of posttraumatic stress disorder in babies of mothers exposed to the World Trade Center attacks during pregnancy. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 90(7), 41154118. https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2005-0550 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zalewski, M., Lengua, L. J., Fisher, P. A., Trancik, A., Bush, N. R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2012). Poverty and single parenting: Relations with preschoolers' cortisol and effortful control. Infant and Child Development, 21(5), 537554. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1759 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zalewski, M., Lengua, L. J., Kiff, C. J., & Fisher, P. A. (2012). Understanding the relation of low income to HPA-axis functioning in preschool children: Cumulative family risk and parenting as pathways to disruptions in cortisol. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 43(6), 924942. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-012-0304-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zalewski, M., Lengua, L. J., Thompson, S. F., & Kiff, C. J. (2016). Income, cumulative risk, and longitudinal profiles of hypothalamic-pituitary–adrenal axis activity in preschool-age children. Development and Psychopathology, 28(2), 341353. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000474 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zandstra, A. R. E., Hartman, C. A., Nederhof, E., van den Heuvel, E. R., Dietrich, A., Hoekstra, P. J., & Ormel, J. (2015). Chronic stress and adolescents’ mental health: Modifying effects of basal cortisol and parental psychiatric history. The TRAILS study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(6), 11191130. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9970-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeiders, K. H., Doane, L. D., & Roosa, M. W. (2012). Perceived discrimination and diurnal cortisol: Examining relations among Mexican American adolescents. Hormones and Behavior, 61(4), 541548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeiders, K. H., Hoyt, L. T., & Adam, E. K. (2014). Associations between self-reported discrimination and diurnal cortisol rhythms among young adults: The moderating role of racial-ethnic minority status. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50, 280288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, Y., Chen, X., Zhao, H., Chen, M., Tian, Y., Liu, C., & Qin, S. (2019). Socioeconomic status disparities affect children’s anxiety and stress-sensitive cortisol awakening response through parental anxiety. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 103, 96103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.01.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) Flowchart. Note. The broad and inclusive search terms likely led to an inflated number of initial records for screening, resulting in the large number of records excluded at the screening level.

Figure 1

Table 1. Study characteristics

Figure 2

Table 2. Number of effect sizes in each category of childhood adversity by diurnal cortisol measure

Figure 3

Table 3. Additional information on moderator variables by study

Figure 4

Table 4. Moderator variable descriptive statistics

Figure 5

Figure 2. Funnel Plots with Trim and Fill. Note. In each figure, the shaded area represents significant effect sizes (p < .05). Black circles represent coded effect sizes. White circles represent effect sizes added by trim and fill analysis.

Supplementary material: File

Perrone et al. supplementary material

Perrone et al. supplementary material

Download Perrone et al. supplementary material(File)
File 191.3 KB