An important predictor of children's fruit and vegetable consumption is their enjoyment of these foods( Reference Gallaway, Jago and Baranowski 1 , Reference Resnicow, Davis-Hearn and Smith 2 ). Innate preferences for sweet tastes and dislike of sour or bitter tastes mean that fruit is readily accepted, but that liking for vegetables may be harder to achieve. However, innate preferences can be modified through pre- and post-natal experiences( Reference Beauchamp and Mennella 3 ). Flavours become more acceptable as they grow in familiarity and there is unequivocal evidence in young children that intake and liking for unfamiliar foods can be increased through repeated exposure, i.e. providing repeated opportunities to taste small quantities of the food( Reference Wardle, Cooke and Gibson 4 – Reference Liem and de Graaf 10 ).
Between the ages of 4 and 7 months, infants are highly receptive to new flavours and textures, requiring fewer exposures than older children to increase acceptance( Reference Harris, Thomas and Booth 11 – Reference Birch 14 ). Exposing children to the taste of commonly rejected foods, such as vegetables, may be most effective in early infancy before the onset of food neophobia or pickiness (a normal developmental stage during the second year of life)( Reference Birch 14 ). Since food preferences develop early and have been shown to track through later childhood and into adulthood( Reference Lien, Lytle and Klepp 15 , Reference Skinner, Carruth and Wendy 16 ), early intervention is likely to reap the greatest benefit.
While repeated exposure to a single vegetable flavour increases infants' acceptance, the speed with which they acquire preferences means that a lack of sufficient variety might result in a ‘monotony’ effect – the infant becoming bored with the taste( Reference Gerrish and Mennella 17 ). Daily changes in the vegetables offered to infants during the transition to solid foods have been shown to lead to immediate increases in preference and intake, and a generalisation of the effect to acceptance of novel tastes( Reference Gerrish and Mennella 17 – Reference Mennella, Nicklaus and Jagolino 19 ). In a group of formula-fed infants, the effect of offering a variety of different vegetables v. carrots alone, or potatoes alone over a period of 9 d was evaluated( Reference Gerrish and Mennella 17 ). Infants in both the variety and carrot groups significantly increased their intake of carrots compared with infants fed potatoes, but only the infants exposed to a variety of tastes ate more of a novel food at the end of the exposure period. It has been suggested that the variety of vegetables from meal to meal offered to weaning infants is more important than the overall number of vegetables offered. For example, increased intake of novel foods was observed in weaning-age infants experiencing daily changes in the vegetables offered compared to infants fed three vegetables, each for three consecutive days( Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 18 ), suggesting that the beneficial effect of variety is maximised by daily changes. In addition, a more varied diet during the weaning period has been linked to greater dietary diversity in later childhood( Reference Skinner, Carruth and Bounds 20 , Reference Cooke, Wardle and Gibson 21 ).
The benefits of repeated and varied exposure early in the complementary feeding period have been previously described( Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 18 , Reference Mennella, Nicklaus and Jagolino 19 , Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 22 , Reference Mennella and Trabulsi 23 ), suggesting promising opportunities for increasing children's vegetable intake. However, no studies to date have tested the procedures in the form of an easily disseminable intervention comprising simple, practical guidance to parents for introducing a variety of vegetables as first foods. Furthermore, no previous study of variety exposure in infants has included a no-treatment control group in which mothers receive only the current standard weaning advice offered by their national health service. Finally, although the existing weaning recommendations and practices vary across Europe, no study to date has examined cross-cultural differences in the effectiveness of such an intervention.
The present study is an exploratory trial of an intervention comprising guidance to parents on the introduction of a variety of vegetables at the first stages of weaning. The primary outcome was infants' consumption of a novel vegetable, offered 1 month after the start of complementary feeding. The secondary outcome was infant's ‘liking’ for the novel vegetable, rated by both researchers and mothers. The same procedure was followed by researchers in the UK, Greece and Portugal in order to examine the effect of the intervention compared with usual care in the different countries.
Subjects and methods
Trial design
A multi-centre, individually randomised (ratio; 1:1), parallel-group study design was adopted for this exploratory trial conducted in the UK, Greece and Portugal between February 2011 and July 2012.
Sample size
Estimating effect size was difficult because few comparable studies have been published. The closest study in the literature( Reference Gerrish and Mennella 17 ) achieved an extremely large effect on vegetable intake (d= 4·0) from a brief but intensive exposure-based intervention, with outcomes measured in the laboratory. A second parent-led, exposure-based intervention with 2- to 6-year-olds showed a significant, but smaller effect (d= 0·2) on children's vegetable consumption( Reference Wardle, Cooke and Gibson 4 ). Outcomes in previous studies have therefore ranged from small to very large, with larger effects in a younger age group, which is most comparable to the present study. The sample size for the current trial (n 120) was therefore designed to provide 80 % power to detect a medium effect size (d= 0·5) at P =0·05( Reference Cohen 24 ) on intake of a novel vegetable in a taste test.
Recruitment of participants
Women in the final trimester of their pregnancy and mothers of infants aged less than 6 months were recruited from antenatal clinics (n 327), primary care, paediatricians and hospitals in London (UK), Athens (Greece) and Porto (Portugal) to a larger study exploring children's fruit and vegetable acceptance during weaning. Mothers were eligible to participate if they were over 18 years old at recruitment, they were sufficiently proficient in each country's respective native language to understand the study materials and their infant was born after 37 weeks' gestation, without diagnosed feeding problems. Mothers who volunteered to participate were asked to complete a consent form and baseline questionnaire following recruitment. A sub-sample of these participants was randomly selected to take part in the present trial and invited to meet with a researcher or health professional immediately prior to the initiation of complementary feeding. All participants were advised that they were free to withdraw from the study at any point. The flow of participants through the trial in each of the three countries is illustrated in Fig. 1.
Randomisation
An independent statistician at University College London generated a block randomisation matrix that was used in all three countries. Individual participants were randomly assigned to an intervention or control (‘usual care’) condition following an initial interview to establish the feeding method. As research has shown that breast-fed infants accept new foods more readily than their formula-fed counterparts( Reference Mennella, Jagnow and Beauchamp 25 , Reference Mennella 26 ), equal representation of breast-fed and formula-fed infants were ensured across the groups, and within each country, using block randomisation. Allocation was revealed to the researcher. Because of the nature of the intervention, parents in the intervention arm and researchers delivering the intervention were not blind. However, parents were unaware of the randomised controlled design and therefore neither the control nor the intervention group knew of the existence of the other.
Ethical approval
This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the relevant ethical committees in each participating country. In the UK, ethical approval was granted by the NHS Central London Research Ethics Committee (10/H0718/54), research and development approval by NHS University College Hospital and NHS North Central London Research Consortium. In Greece, ethical approval was granted by the Ethical Committee of Harokopio University of Athens (session no. 27/14-07-2010). In Portugal, ethical approval was granted by the local ethical committee (Ethical committee for Health of the São João Hospital/University of Porto Medical School – 29.JUL10-12 951). Written informed consent was obtained from the subjects. In the UK, this trial is registered as ISRCTN85048101.
Intervention
All participants met with a researcher or health professional immediately prior to the initiation of complementary feeding (i.e. introducing solid foods). Visits took place either at the participants' home or at a paediatrician's office and the mothers determined the precise timing of these visits (which in some instances was up to 4 weeks prior to the initiation of complementary feeding).
In the intervention group, a researcher or health professional explained to the participant: (1) the importance of introducing vegetables early in the weaning process, (2) the beneficial effects of offering different single vegetables each day, (3) the techniques of exposure feeding, (4) interpreting infants' facial reactions to food and (5) the need for persistence when an infant initially rejects a food. A leaflet reinforcing these messages (standardised across countries) was given to participants, who were then asked to complete a short questionnaire about their infant's early milk-feeding experiences.
In consultation with mothers (and paediatricians in the Portuguese sample), five vegetables were selected as the first foods to be introduced. Mothers were provided with a small number of commercially available vegetable purées to use, but were told that they could prepare their own foods if they preferred. They were asked to offer the five vegetables in a sequence over 15 d as follows: A,B,C,D,E, A,B,C,D,E, A,B,C,D,E and to record progress on a chart provided. For a further 5 d, participants were told to continue to offer vegetables, but in addition, to start to introduce additional age-appropriate foods.
Participants in the control group completed the same questionnaire as intervention mothers. However, control mothers were not offered any specific guidance, instructions or information on weaning with vegetables. Instead, the control group received ‘usual care’, which varies between European countries.
In the UK, the recommendations are to introduce fruits, vegetables and baby rice or cereal as first foods, but the information provided to mothers is inconsistent and the advice available may vary by local health authority. In Greece, paediatricians provide parents with guidance on appropriate first foods, commonly baby rice, cereals or fruits. In Portugal, the guidelines for weaning are not prescriptive, and health professionals are advised to adapt international and national recommendations (e.g. from WHO, ESPGHAN and Portuguese Paediatric Society), to the needs and circumstances of individual infants( Reference Guerra, Rêgo and Silva 27 ). In Portugal there has been a move towards advice to introduce vegetable soups or purées as first foods.
Outcome measures
Mothers in both control and intervention groups completed questionnaires about themselves and their infant prior to the intervention and at follow-up (1 month after the introduction of solid foods), which included items on demographics and feeding practices. Mothers reported their date of birth, parity, marital status and educational qualifications. Mothers reported separately on the frequency of fruit and vegetable servings they had consumed in the past week and the data were recoded to provide an estimation of the total number each of fruit and vegetable portions consumed daily. Self-reported height and weight was used to calculate maternal BMI (kg/m2) and maternal age was calculated at the time of child's birth. Maternal age and BMI were treated as continuous variables, while education was dichotomised as ‘university level’ v. ‘below university level’. Mothers were also asked to record their child's date of birth, sex, birth weight (in kg) and the number of weeks' gestation at birth, to provide an estimate of gestational age. Feeding method was assessed with the question, ‘Which feeding methods did you use in the first three months’, with response options: ‘entirely breastfeeding’; ‘mostly breastfeeding with some bottle-feeding’; ‘equally breastfeeding and bottle-feeding’; ‘mostly bottle-feeding and some breastfeeding’; ‘almost entirely bottle-feeding (only tried breastfeeding a few times)’; ‘entirely bottle-feeding (never tried breastfeeding)’; and ‘other’. Infant age at the time of introduction to solids was calculated in weeks by using the child's date of birth and the date on which mothers reported that they had offered solid food for the first time.
At follow-up, taste tests were administered in which an unfamiliar vegetable (artichoke purée) was offered to participating infants. The primary outcome was intake (g) of the novel vegetable. Infant liking for the vegetable (independently rated by mothers and researchers) was also recorded. The procedure was then repeated with a novel fruit (peach purée), which acted as a control food and to provide an indication of whether the intervention had the unintended side effect of reducing acceptance of foods other than vegetables.
Taste tests took place in the infant's home or paediatrician's office and test foods were fed to infants by mothers in the presence of the researcher. The researcher present at the taste test was the same individual who delivered the intervention and was not therefore able to be blinded to condition. Taste tests were conducted at the child's mealtime in order to ensure they were hungry. Mothers were provided with two 130 g jars of artichoke purée, the contents of which were weighed prior to the start of the taste test. Artichoke purée was selected on the basis that it is an unfamiliar and rarely consumed vegetable among young children across Europe( Reference Caton, Ahern and Remy 8 , Reference Hausner, Olsen and Moller 28 ) and not available as a commercial baby food in any of the three participating countries. Mothers were instructed to feed their infant as normal and at their usual pace until the infant refused the food on three or more occasions, or had finished two full jars. Refusal was defined as keeping the mouth closed, turning the head away, pushing the spoon away, crying, or playing( Reference Mennella, Nicklaus and Jagolino 19 ). Conditions were kept as naturalistic as possible; with mothers using any techniques (i.e. facial expressions and verbal encouragements) they would normally employ to encourage their infant to eat in a mealtime situation. On completion of the test, all spilled food was returned to the bowl/jar, which was weighed again to calculate the weight of food consumed( Reference Mennella, Nicklaus and Jagolino 19 ). Immediately after each feeding session, the mother and researcher separately and independently rated the infant's apparent liking for the food on a 9-point scale, ranging from 1 = ‘dislikes very much’, to 9 = ‘likes very much’ with a central point of 5 = ‘neither likes nor dislikes’( Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 22 , Reference Forestell and Mennella 29 ). The entire process was then repeated with the unfamiliar test fruit (peach purée). The vegetable was always offered first, followed by the fruit approximately 10 min later.
Statistical analyses
Participants with complete data on the primary outcome (intake at the taste test) were included in the analyses. ANCOVA were conducted to compare intervention and control groups by weight of vegetable consumed, and researcher's and mother's rating of vegetable liking at the taste test while controlling for country. These analyses were repeated for fruit intake and the researcher's and mother's rating of fruit liking at the taste test. As typical weaning practices in the three participating nations varied, secondary analyses were then performed comparing intake and liking ratings between intervention and control groups for each country separately.
Results
The flow of participants through the trial is illustrated in Fig. 1. In total, 139 families completed the trial, including the taste tests 1 month post-intervention (fifty-three, thirty-one and fifty-five families in the UK, Greece and Portugal, respectively). Sample demographics are presented in Table 1. The first solid foods consumed by the infants in the three countries, provided by experimental condition, are shown in Table 2.
* The variables were categorised as follows – below university: no qualifications, secondary school certificate, technical school, high school certificate, private faculty diploma; undergraduate and above: undergraduate degree, postgraduate qualification.
† The variables were categorised as follows – entirely breastfed: breastfeeding exclusively; mixed: mostly breastfeeding with some bottle-feeding, equally breastfeeding and bottle-feeding, mostly bottle-feeding and some breastfeeding; entirely bottle fed: almost all bottle-feeding (only tried breastfeeding a few times), Bottle-feeding only (never tried breastfeeding).
* Vegetable soups are common weaning foods in Portugal and typically include potato, olive oil and at least two different vegetables (e.g. carrot, pumpkin, onion, garlic and leek).
Completed intervention charts were returned by 86 % of intervention families (UK; 100 % (28/28), Greece; 100 % (16/16), Portugal; 63 % (17/27)). Completed charts revealed that over the 15-d intervention period, parents recorded their infants consuming vegetables on 89 % (mean 13·3 (sd 3·0)) of the fifteen possible eating occasions (UK; (86 %) mean 12·8 (sd 3·4), Greece; (95 %), mean 14·2 (sd 1·8), Portugal (88 %), mean 13·1 (sd3·0)). Infants were recorded as eating nothing on 7 % (mean 1·0 (sd 1·8) of the fifteen intervention days (UK; (6 %) mean 1·1 (sd 2·4), Greece; (5 %), mean 0·8 (sd 1·8), Portugal; (8 %), mean 1·2 (sd 1·7)). Data on infants' willingness to eat during the intervention period were missing for 5 % of the total eating occasions (UK; 8 %, Greece; 0 %, Portugal; 5 %).
The results of the taste tests by experimental condition are shown in Table 3. The mean intake of the unfamiliar vegetable purée was almost 10 g higher among the intervention group infants (38·91 g) compared to the control group (29·84 g). However, the primary analyses examining the effect of the intervention in the three countries combined (n 139) revealed no significant main effect of the intervention on vegetable intake, controlling for the effect of country (F (1,135)= 3·49, P= 0·064). Infants in the intervention group were rated by researchers as liking the unfamiliar vegetable significantly more than control infants (F (1,135)= 4·70, P< 0·032), but a similar trend observed for maternal ratings of infants' vegetable liking did not reach significance (F (1,135)= 3·84, P= 0·052), while controlling for the effect of country in the pooled sample. No main effect of the intervention was found for either intake or liking ratings for fruit.
* Value was significantly different (P< 0·05).
† Effect of condition controlling for country
Separate analyses by country revealed a significant effect of the intervention on intake of the novel vegetable in the UK, with intervention infants eating on average 16 g more artichoke purée than control infants (32·8 v. 16·5 g; t(51) = 3·10; P= 0·003) (see Table 4). This group difference in vegetable intake represented a large effect size (Cohen's d= 0·8)( Reference Coulthard, Harris and Emmett 13 ). UK intervention infants were also rated as liking the purée significantly more than control infants by mothers (6·7 v. 4·3; t(51) = 4·51; P< 0·001) and researchers (6·7 v. 4·6; t 51) = 4·37; P< 0·001) separately (see Table 4). A large intervention effect size was observed for both maternal and researcher ratings of liking (Cohen's d= 1·2 for both). No group differences were found between UK intervention and control infants for intake (27·9 v. 40·7 g) or liking ratings of the unfamiliar fruit.
* Values were significantly different (P< 0·05).
In the Greek sample, mean intake in the intervention group was on average 13 g higher than in the control group (36·3 v. 23·6 g), although this difference was not statistically significant. Intervention infants were also given slightly higher vegetable liking ratings in the taste test by both mothers (4·3 v. 3·3) and researchers (4·6 v. 3·4) in Greece but again these differences were not significant.
In Portugal, no significant intervention effect on infants' intake of the artichoke purée was observed at follow-up, with intervention infants consuming only an average of 2 g more than control infants (46·9 v. 45·1 g). Similarly, there was no effect of the intervention on mothers' or researchers' vegetable liking ratings for the Portuguese infants (4·6 v. 5·2 and 4·5 v. 5·0, respectively).
There were no significant group differences in intake or either mother's or researcher's ratings of the infants' liking of the unfamiliar fruit purée in any of the three countries (see Table 4).
Discussion
No significant main effect of the intervention on children's intake of a novel vegetable was found in the full sample. Children in the intervention group were rated by researchers as liking the unfamiliar vegetable more than the control group, but this was not the case for maternal ratings. However, UK intervention infants ate significantly more of an unfamiliar vegetable and were rated by both mothers and researchers as liking the vegetable more than infants in the control group 1 month after the introduction of solid foods. In the Greek and Portuguese samples, there was no significant effect of the intervention on either intake or liking of an unfamiliar vegetable. However, observations of the raw data in Greece did suggest a positive trend towards higher consumption among the intervention infants.
The UK findings provide support for previous research showing repeated exposure to vegetables during complementary feeding can impact positively on infants' vegetable acceptance( Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 22 , Reference Barends, de Vries and Mojet 30 , Reference Sullivan and Birch 31 ) and daily changes in the variety of vegetables consumed increase acceptance of a novel food( Reference Gerrish and Mennella 17 , Reference Maier, Chabanet and Schaal 18 ). The differences in the outcomes observed across the three study sites may be partly explained by cultural variations in typical weaning practices in these countries. Recent research has revealed that Portuguese school children have among the highest levels of vegetable intake in Europe( Reference Yngve, Wolf and Poortvliet 32 ). In addition, the Euro-Growth study, examining infant feeding practices and the introduction of complementary foods across Europe, found that fruit was the most common first food offered to infants in both Greece and the UK, while in Portugal it was cereal or vegetables( Reference Freeman, van't Hof and Haschke 33 ). This suggests that normal weaning practices in Portugal more closely resemble those advocated in the present study, potentially minimising differences between intervention and control groups. The observation that over 70 % of the Portuguese control infants in the present study were given vegetable-based soups as their first food supports this assertion. In contrast, only 32 % of the UK control infants and just 7 % of the Greek control infants received vegetables as their first foods. Instead, baby rice or cereals were the most common first foods consumed by control infants in the UK (56 %) and Greece (73 %). This is also reflected in the raw intake data from the taste test in which Portuguese control infants ate more of the unfamiliar vegetable purée than UK or Greek intervention infants.
No group differences in fruit intake or liking were observed in the taste test in any of the three countries. This suggests that introducing single vegetables as first foods, and not offering fruit for the first 15 d does not reduce fruit preference in young infants. This is reassuring, but unsurprising given infants' innate preferences for sweet tastes( Reference Beauchamp and Moran 34 , Reference Ventura and Mennella 35 ). The finding that vegetable exposure does not affect fruit acceptance also supports a recent study that found infants who had been exposed to 5 d of rice flour porridge, followed by 19 d of vegetables, ate on average the same amount of a novel fruit immediately post-intervention as infants who had no exposure yet to either fruits or vegetables and had only received 5 d of rice flour porridge( Reference Barends, de Vries and Mojet 30 ).
Alternative explanations for the group differences in novel vegetable acceptance observed for UK infants should be acknowledged. It is possible that the intervention infants received less energy as a result of consuming vegetables only for the first 15 d of weaning and were therefore hungrier than control infants during this period. However, there is no clear reason why this phenomenon should be unique to UK infants. In addition, first solid foods are ‘complementary’ to the continuation of milk feeding and most infants continue to consume a large proportion of their energy via breast or formula milk during this early weaning period, particularly if solids are introduced prior to 6 months as was common in the present study. As the intervention feeding plan lasted 15 d and the taste tests were conducted 1 month after the introduction of solids, it is particularly unlikely that intervention infants were systematically hungrier than control infants at the time of the taste tests.
Necessary variations in the study procedures of the participating countries are likely to have impacted on results. In Portugal, researchers had to acquire permission from the infants' paediatricians or general practitioner in order to conduct the study and in a proportion of cases the advice was delivered by health professionals rather than researchers. Although acceptance was generally high, all health professionals did not comply fully with the intervention even after agreeing to participate. Consequently, fidelity of, and adherence to, the intervention may have been undermined. There is some evidence of lower compliance among Portuguese participants – only 67 % of the intervention mothers gave their infants an isolated vegetable as their first food as requested, compared to 93 % in the UK and 100 % in Greece. In addition, fewer Portuguese intervention parents (63 %, compared to 100 % in the UK and Greece) returned completed study charts, suggesting lower compliance with the intervention procedure. There is a need to repeat this study in a larger sample within countries where vegetables are not already common first foods, and further research would benefit from exploring differences in outcome when advice is delivered by health professionals compared to researchers. A further limitation of this study is that while mothers were unaware of the study hypotheses, neither they nor researchers could be ‘blinded’ to treatment, which may have influenced preference ratings in the taste tests. However, the ecological validity of the experiment; the fact it was implemented by mothers themselves in the home is a strength, as is the randomised study design and inclusion of a no-treatment control.
The intervention was received positively by parents who particularly welcomed the simple, prescriptive and unambiguous nature of the instructions at an often anxiety-provoking stage of infant development. UK intervention infants showed increased intake and liking of an unfamiliar vegetable in the short term but this was not true in Portugal where vegetables are commonly given as first foods. It appears that repeated exposure to a variety of vegetables at weaning may work to increase vegetable acceptance in the short term in countries where vegetables are not typically provided as first foods. However, the longer-term impact of the intervention remains to be explored. This intervention is straightforward and would be easy to disseminate to mothers during an infants' first months when parents are in frequent contact with health professionals and actively seeking advice about weaning.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank the families who participated in the study and the healthcare providers and staff who assisted with their recruitment.
This research is supported by European Community's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under the grant agreement no. 245012-HabEat. The purées offered to participants in this study and the artichoke and peach purées used as a test food were donated by Danone Nutricia Research. The funders had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.
None of the authors reported a conflict of interest.
The authors' contributions are as follows: L. C., C. L., P. M., Y. M. and J. W. designed the research; A. F., G. M., C. L., A. O. and C. M. conducted the research; A. F., A. O., C. M. and R. B. analysed the data; A. F., L. C., G. M., C. L., R. B., A. O., C. M., G. M. and J. W. wrote the manuscript; A. F. had primary responsibility for the final content of the manuscript; and all authors read and approved the final manuscript.