Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-24hb2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T15:55:49.718Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Uncovering the complexity of Tourette syndrome, little by little

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Daniel A. Gorman*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto and The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto
Elia Abi-Jaoude
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto and University Health Network (Toronto Western Hospital), Toronto, Canada
*
Daniel A. Gorman, MD, FRCPC, The Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Avenue, Toronto M5G 1X8, Canada. Email: daniel.gorman@utoronto.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

The aetiology of Tourette syndrome is highly complex and still poorly understood. In this issue, using data from a large, prospective, population-based cohort of children, Mathews et al examine associations of pre- and perinatal exposures with Tourette syndrome and other chronic tic disorders. Their work illustrates the importance of environmental factors in the aetiology of neuropsychiatric conditions and the value of replication in science.

Type
Editorials
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2014

‘Science, my boy, is made up of mistakes, but they are mistakes which it is useful to make, because they lead little by little to the truth.’

Jules Verne, Journey to the Centre of the Earth

Tourette syndrome is considered a prototypical developmental neuropsychiatric disorder.Reference Steinberg, King and Apter1 Its defining symptoms, motor and vocal tics, are neurological but it is usually associated with one or more psychiatric conditions, especially attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. In addition, the tics often follow a well-described developmental course: onset in early childhood, increasing severity until approximately age 11 and for most patients a gradual improvement during adolescence.Reference Leckman, Zhang, Vitale, Lahnin, Lynch and Bondi2 Despite these general patterns related to comorbidity and natural history, however, the variability of Tourette syndrome is remarkable. Patients differ enormously with respect to the nature, severity and course of their tics, the range of their comorbid conditions, the degree of their functional impairment and their response to treatment. It is hardly surprising, therefore, that the aetiology of Tourette syndrome is highly complex and still poorly understood, with genetic, epigenetic, neurobiological, environmental and psychosocial factors all contributing.

The multistrike model of aetiology

To account for the clinical complexity and heterogeneity that characterise Tourette syndrome, a ‘multistrike model’ of its aetiology has been proposed.Reference Spessot, Plessen and Peterson3 In this model, Strike 1 represents a genetic vulnerability that results in structural and functional neurological changes. These neurological changes, in turn, predispose the individual to develop tics and related symptoms. Strike 2 represents disturbances in brain regions that support self-regulatory functions. When these brain regions function normally, they mount a compensatory response that serves to inhibit tic expression; consequently, a disturbance in their functioning compromises the compensatory response, resulting in increased tic severity. Strike 3 comprises a host of environmental factors that increase the risk of tics being expressed in genetically and neurologically vulnerable individuals. These factors include neuroendocrine, infectious, autoimmune, toxic and psychosocial influences. Moreover, they can be divided into (a) pre- and perinatal events that exert ‘organisational’ effects on brain networks that generate tics, and (b) postnatal events that exert ‘activational’ effects on these networks, thereby increasing the expression of tics.Reference Hoekstra, Dietrich, Edwards, Elamin and Martino4

Although the multistrike model is a useful framework for guiding research, it is not particularly specific. In fact, it can probably be applied to all complex developmental neuropsychiatric disorders.Reference Spessot, Plessen and Peterson3 Unfortunately, a more specific model of the aetiology of Tourette syndrome is not possible at this point, because at the level of each strike of the general model, research findings are disappointing or conflicting, require replication or have significant limitations. For example, even though twin and family studies have demonstrated that Tourette syndrome is highly heritable, no candidate genes have been replicated consistently, and recently the first genome-wide association study detected no genome-wide significant loci.Reference Scharf, Yu, Mathews, Neale, Stewart and Fagerness5

Neuroimaging studies have yielded more promising results, suggesting that reduced caudate volume and thinning of motor and sensorimotor cortices reflect the neurological predisposition to tics. In addition, hypertrophy of cortical (dorsolateral prefrontal, orbitofrontal and parietal) and limbic (amygdala and hippocampus) regions and a smaller corpus callosum are thought to reflect compensatory neuroplastic changes.Reference Plessen, Bansal and Peterson6 Almost all these studies have been cross-sectional, however, making it difficult to tease out underlying cause from compensatory response. Conflicting findings have also been reported, likely as a result of differences in sample size, demographic characteristics, tic severity, comorbid conditions, medication use, techniques for the acquisition and processing of images, and methods for data analysis. Nevertheless, the neuroimaging literature does suggest that tic formation results from a disordered striatum and related dysfunction in cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical circuits. This notion is further supported by neuropathological studies showing reduced number and altered distribution of inhibitory GABAergic neurons in the sensorimotor areas of the striatum.Reference Ganos, Roessner and Munchau7

Role of environmental risk factors

Except for the controversy regarding the potential role of group A beta-haemolytic streptococcal (GABHS) infections in a subset of patients with Tourette syndrome or obsessive-compulsive disorder,Reference Hoekstra, Dietrich, Edwards, Elamin and Martino4 less attention has been given to environmental factors that influence the expression of tics. The number of studies in this area is still relatively small, and limitations often include retrospective design, absence of a control group and use of clinical rather than population-based samples. Furthermore, although the findings have generated interesting hypotheses, most have not been replicated. Generation of hypotheses is of course critical for scientific research, but a literature made up of many unreplicated findings risks the adoption of premature conclusions or may be dismissed altogether. Researchers on both sides of the Atlantic have argued recently for greater emphasis on studies that aim to replicate previous findings,Reference Munafò and Flint8,Reference Murad and Montori9 and it has been said that ‘reproducibility… is at the core of “the spirit of science’”.Reference Santer, Wigley and Taylor10 In this issue of the Journal, the study by Mathews and colleaguesReference Mathews, Scharf, Miller, Macdonald-Wallis, Lawlor and Ben-Shlomo11 exemplifies this spirit.

Using data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) - a prospective, prebirth cohort of all children born to close to 15 000 women in Avon, UK - Mathews et al examine associations of pre- and perinatal exposures with Tourette syndrome and other chronic tic disorders. A primary objective of the study was to determine whether associations detected in previous studies could be replicated: maternal prenatal smoking and alcohol use, excessive vomiting during pregnancy, complications of delivery, low birth weight and young gestational age at birth. Perhaps surprisingly, an association with only one of these previously identified risk factors - alcohol use during pregnancy - was replicated. The authors also identified three potential risk factors that have not been previously reported: cannabis use during pregnancy, parity (being the first-born child) and inadequate weight gain during pregnancy. Based on the odds ratios and significance levels in both the Tourette syndrome and Tourette syndrome/chronic tic disorder groups, the most robust risk factor appears to be inadequate weight gain during pregnancy. All three new findings, however, now await replication.

As the authors themselves point out, the pattern of associations they identified does not suggest an obvious overall mechanism by which environmental risk factors exert their effects. For example, why should inadequate weight gain during pregnancy be such a strong risk factor for Tourette syndrome, but not low birth weight? Why should being the first-born child make a difference? It is tempting to apply Occam’s razor in search of a unifying, parsimonious explanation for these findings, but doing so would probably be misguided. The aetiology of Tourette syndrome is a complex problem, and as H. L. Mencken said, ‘For every complex problem there is an answer that is clear, simple, and wrong’. Bearing in mind the multistrike model of Tourette syndrome and other complex developmental neuropsychiatric disorders, it is likely that environmental risk factors have their effects at different points in the cascade of genetic, epigenetic and neurobiological events that ultimately lead to the expression of tics and related symptoms.

Among the risk factors for Tourette syndrome/chronic tic disorder identified by Mathews et al, the one that is probably most easily explained is prenatal alcohol exposure, since it is associated with poor inhibitory control and has been shown to impair cortical development and plasticity.Reference Lebel, Mattson, Riley, Jones, Adnams and May12 Thus, prenatal alcohol exposure could increase the risk of Tourette syndrome in predisposed individuals by disrupting the compensatory mechanisms that serve to inhibit tic expression. Caution is still warranted, however, regarding the role of prenatal alcohol exposure in the aetiology of Tourette syndrome, as epidemiological study findings have a high risk of confounding bias even with the best efforts to account for potential confounders. For instance, it could be that mothers with a genetic predisposition to Tourette syndrome are more likely to use alcohol during pregnancy, perhaps as a result of related problems with behavioural inhibition.

Despite the inherent risk of confounding bias and the relatively small sample size (50 participants with Tourette syndrome, 122 with Tourette syndrome/chronic tic disorder), the study by Mathews et al has many strengths that support the validity of its findings. These include the prospective, longitudinal design; ascertainment of participants from the general population; use of a conservative algorithm for data analysis; and careful procedures to control for potential confounders. Regarding this last issue, it is notable that controlling for socioeconomic status in univariate analyses tended to reduce the strength of associations of potential risk factors, rendering certain ones non-significant (maternal age and assisted delivery). This pattern highlights the contribution of social factors in the aetiology of Tourette syndrome, even though the disorder has a strong neurobiological basis and is even defined by neurological symptoms.

It is well recognised that stress and social context can influence tic expression in the moment, but evidence is also emerging regarding the influence of social factors on long-term tic severity. For example, psychosocial stress was found to modestly predict increased future tic severity in children and adolescents with chronic tics,Reference Lin, Katsovich, Ghebremichael, Findley, Grantz and Lombroso13 and the effect was three times stronger in the context of a new GABHS infection.Reference Lin, Williams, Katsovich, Findley, Grantz and Lombroso14 In addition, Mathews et al cite a previous (unpublished) finding that lower socioeconomic status was associated with an increased risk of developing Tourette syndrome/chronic tic disorder. These results are in keeping with evidence regarding the contribution of social factors in the aetiology of other neuropsychiatric conditions, such as schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s disease and obsessive-compulsive disorder.Reference Eisenberg15 Indeed, the important role of socioeconomic statusReference Spencer, Thanh and Louise16 and stressReference Cohen, Janicki-Deverts and Miller17 in disease has long been recognised.

Conclusion

The contemporary view of cutting-edge psychiatric research tends to prioritise new discoveries that reveal the secrets of the genome or the brain, whereas studies aiming to replicate previous findings or examine environmental factors are decidedly less glamorous. Rigorous replication studies, however, are an essential step in science, and environmental factors are no less important than biological ones with respect to the aetiology of mental illness as well as the development of effective therapies. Mathews et al should therefore be commended for their efforts to replicate previously reported associations of pre- and perinatal exposures with Tourette syndrome. By suggesting that a number of those associations may be mistaken, their results lead us a little closer to the truth.

Footnotes

See pp. 40–45, this issue.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Steinberg, T King, R Apter, A Tourette's syndrome: a review from a developmental perspective. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 2010; 47: 105109.Google ScholarPubMed
2 Leckman, JF Zhang, H Vitale, A Lahnin, F Lynch, K Bondi, C et al. Course of tic severity in Tourette's syndrome: the first two decades. Pediatrics 1998; 102: 1419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Spessot, AL Plessen, KJ Peterson, BS Neuroimaging of developmental psychopathologies: the importance of self-regulatory and neuroplastic processes in adolescence. Ann NY Acad Sci 2004; 1021: 86104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Hoekstra, PJ Dietrich, A Edwards, MJ Elamin, I Martino, D Environmental factors in Tourette syndrome. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37: 10401049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Scharf, JM Yu, D Mathews, CA Neale, BM Stewart, SE Fagerness, JA et al. Genome-wide association study of Tourette's syndrome. Mol Psychiatry 2013; 18: 721728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Plessen, KJ Bansal, R Peterson, BS Imaging evidence for anatomical disturbances and neuroplastic compensation in persons with Tourette syndrome. J Psychosom Res 2009; 67: 559573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Ganos, C Roessner, V Munchau, A The functional anatomy of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37: 10501062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Munafò, MR Flint, J How reliable are scientific studies? Br J Psychiatry 2010; 197: 257258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Murad, MH Montori, VM. Synthesizing evidence: shifting the focus from individual studies to the body of evidence. JAMA 2013; 309: 22172218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
10 Santer, BD Wigley, TM Taylor, KE The reproducibility of observational estimates of surface and atmospheric temperature change. Science 2011; 334: 12321233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Mathews, CA Scharf, JM Miller, LL Macdonald-Wallis, C Lawlor, DA Ben-Shlomo, Y Association between pre- and perinatal exposures and Tourette syndrome or chronic tic disorder in the ALSPAC cohort. Br J Psychiatry 2014; 204: 4045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12 Lebel, C Mattson, SN Riley, EP Jones, KL Adnams, CM May, PA et al. A longitudinal study of the long-term consequences of drinking during pregnancy: heavy in utero alcohol exposure disrupts the normal processes of brain development. J Neurosci 2012; 32: 15243–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13 Lin, H Katsovich, L Ghebremichael, M Findley, DB Grantz, H Lombroso, PJ et al. Psychosocial stress predicts future symptom severities in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome and/or obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007; 48: 157166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14 Lin, H Williams, KA Katsovich, L Findley, DB Grantz, H Lombroso, PJ et al. Streptococcal upper respiratory tract infections and psychosocial stress predict future tic and obsessive-compulsive symptom severity in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67: 684691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Eisenberg, L. The social construction of the human brain. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152: 15631575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16 Spencer, N Thanh, TM Louise, S Low income/socio-economic status in early childhood and physical health in later childhood/adolescence: a systematic review. Matern Child Health J 2013; 17: 424431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17 Cohen, S Janicki-Deverts, D Miller, GE Psychological stress and disease. JAMA 2007; 298: 16851687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.