Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T13:01:15.585Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Disrupted upregulation of salience network connectivity during acute stress in siblings of schizophrenia patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 January 2020

Judith M. C. van Leeuwen*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Christiaan H. Vinkers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry/GGZ InGeest, Amsterdam UMC (location VUmc), Amsterdam, the Netherlands Department of Anatomy and Neurosciences, Amsterdam UMC (location VUmc), Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Matthijs Vink
Affiliation:
Utrecht University, Experimental Psychology, Utrecht, The Netherlands
René S. Kahn
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York, USA
Marian Joëls
Affiliation:
Department of Translational Neuroscience, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands
Erno J. Hermans
Affiliation:
Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
*
Author for correspondence: Judith M. C. van Leeuwen, E-mail: j.vanleeuwen@donders.ru.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

An adaptive neural stress response is essential to adequately cope with a changing environment. It was previously argued that sympathetic/noradrenergic activity during acute stress increases salience network (SN) connectivity and reduces executive control network (ECN) connectivity in healthy controls, with opposing effects in the late aftermath of stress. Altered temporal dynamics of these networks in response to stress are thought to play a role in the development of psychopathology in vulnerable individuals.

Methods

We exposed male healthy controls (n = 40, mean age = 33.9) and unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients (n = 39, mean age = 33.2) to the stress or control condition of the trier social stress test and subsequently investigated resting state functional connectivity of the SN and ECN directly after and 1.5 h after stress.

Results

Acute stress resulted in increased functional connectivity within the SN in healthy controls, but not in siblings (group × stress interaction pfwe < 0.05). In the late aftermath of stress, stress reduced functional connectivity within the SN in both groups. Moreover, we found increased functional connectivity between the ECN and the cerebellum in the aftermath of stress in both healthy controls and siblings of schizophrenia patients.

Conclusions

The results show profound differences between siblings of schizophrenia patients and controls during acute stress. Siblings lacked the upregulation of neural resources necessary to quickly and adequately cope with a stressor. This points to a reduced dynamic range in the sympathetic response, and may constitute a vulnerability factor for the development of psychopathology in this at-risk group.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2020

Introduction

While a substantial body of research has established an important role for stress in the development of many psychiatric disorders (Butjosa et al., Reference Butjosa, Gómez-Benito, Huerta-Ramos, Del cacho, Barajas, Baños and Ochoa2016; Koenders et al., Reference Koenders, Giltay, Spijker, Hoencamp, Spinhoven and Elzinga2014; Lex, Bäzner, & Meyer, Reference Lex, Bäzner and Meyer2017; McCraw & Parker, Reference McCraw and Parker2017; Shapero et al., Reference Shapero, Weiss, Burke, Boland, Abramson and Alloy2017), not all people who experience stressful life events develop a form of psychopathology (Kalisch et al., Reference Kalisch, Baker, Dewleen, Boks, Bonanno, Brummelman and Kleim2017). The neural mechanisms underlying these interindividual differences are not well understood, but have previously been suggested to arise from impaired temporal dynamics of large-scale brain networks in response to stress in vulnerable individuals (Homberg, Kozicz, & Fernández, Reference Homberg, Kozicz and Fernández2017).

Stress initiates a cascade of temporally distinct neurochemical and physiological changes, which allow individuals to deal with an ongoing stressor and return to homeostasis thereafter. In this context, Hermans and colleagues proposed an integrative model of the acute and delayed effects of stress on large-scale neural networks in the healthy brain (Hermans, Henckens, Joëls, & Fernández, Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014). First, the activation of the sympathetic nervous system and the release of catecholamines such as (nor)epinephrine and dopamine shift the brain to a hyperalerted state. This is accompanied by increased activity within, and connectivity between, nodes of the salience network (SN), including the anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and amygdala (Hermans et al., Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014, Reference Hermans, van Marle, Ossewaarde, Henckens, Qin, van Kesteren and Fernández2011; van Marle, Hermans, Qin, & Fernández, Reference van Marle, Hermans, Qin and Fernández2010). Supraoptimal levels of catecholamines simultaneously reduce functioning of the executive control network (ECN), impairing complex cognitive abilities during a stressful event (Arnsten, Reference Arnsten2009; Qin, Hermans, van Marle, Luo, & Fernández, Reference Qin, Hermans, van Marle, Luo and Fernández2009; Shields, Sazma, & Yonelinas, Reference Shields, Sazma and Yonelinas2016), and promoting habitual behavior as a result of attenuated top-down control over limbic areas (Schwabe, Tegenthoff, Höffken, & Wolf, Reference Schwabe, Tegenthoff, Höffken and Wolf2010).

While these rapid changes are considered to form an adaptive response to acute stressors, their adequate termination is essential for successful recovery from stress (de Kloet, Meijer, de Nicola, de Rijk, & Joëls, Reference de Kloet, Meijer, de Nicola, de Rijk and Joëls2018). Levels of cortisol, the end-product of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis, peak around 30 min after stress (Dickerson & Kemeny, Reference Dickerson and Kemeny2004). While cortisol, through nongenomic pathways, amplifies catecholaminergic effects in the early phase of the stress response (de Kloet, Karst, & Joëls, Reference de Kloet, Karst and Joëls2008), slower genomic effects of cortisol are thought to aid the return to homeostasis by reversing the hyperalerted state and enhancing cognitive processes such as reappraisal of the situation after stress has subsided (Henckens, van Wingen, Joels, & Fernandez, Reference Henckens, van Wingen, Joels and Fernandez2010; Hermans et al., Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014; Joëls, Sarabdjitsingh, & Karst, Reference Joëls, Sarabdjitsingh and Karst2012). The administration of hydrocortisone (a synthetic form of cortisol) slowly reduces activity and connectivity of the amygdala (Henckens et al., Reference Henckens, van Wingen, Joels and Fernandez2010; Henckens, van Wingen, Joëls, & Fernández, Reference Henckens, van Wingen, Joëls and Fernández2012a), improves emotional states (Reuter, Reference Reuter2002), and enhances dlPFC functioning and working memory performance (Henckens et al., Reference Henckens, Pu, Hermans, van Wingen, Joëls and Fernández2012), supporting a role for the late effects of cortisol on SN and ECN functioning.

Even in the absence of stress, SN and ECN functional connectivity was found to be affected in severe psychiatric disorders including schizophrenia (Negrón-Oyarzo, Aboitiz, & Fuentealba, Reference Negrón-Oyarzo, Aboitiz and Fuentealba2016; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Ji, Hong, Poh, Krishnan, Lee and Zhou2016). Moreover, increased SN functional connectivity is associated with subclinical symptoms in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis (Pelletier-Baldelli, Bernard, & Mittal, Reference Pelletier-Baldelli, Bernard and Mittal2015) and increased insula activity is associated with auditory and verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia (Sommer et al., Reference Sommer, Diederen, Blom, Willems, Kushan, Slotema and Kahn2008). Given the time-dependent effects of stress on the SN and ECN, altered functional connectivity of these two networks in psychiatric disease could be the result of a maladaptive response to stress in vulnerable individuals (Homberg et al., Reference Homberg, Kozicz and Fernández2017). However, no studies to date have investigated the temporal dynamics of these two large-scale brain networks in response to stress in an at-risk group.

Here, we investigated the acute and delayed effects of an acute stressor, the trier social stress test (TSST), on functional connectivity of the SN and ECN during task-free periods in healthy controls and unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients. Siblings have a higher chance to develop various psychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Chang, Chen, Tsai, Su, Li and Bai2017), and show increased stress sensitivity in daily life (Myin-Germeys, Marcelis, Krabbendam, Delespaul, & van Os, Reference Myin-Germeys, Marcelis, Krabbendam, Delespaul and van Os2005). We hypothesized that stress would immediately result in increased SN functional connectivity and decreased ECN functional connectivity in healthy controls, whereas we expect to find the opposite in the aftermath of stress. Moreover, we examined whether there are differences between siblings and controls, expecting a potential increase in SN functional connectivity during stress and/or attenuated recovery of this network in the aftermath of stress. Finally, we expected an increase in cortisol levels and adrenergic activity after stress and investigated potential differences between the groups.

Methods and materials

Participants

We recruited 40 healthy controls and 40 healthy siblings of schizophrenia patients from the Genetic Risk & Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP) study (Korver, Quee, Boos, Simons, & de Haan, Reference Korver, Quee, Boos, Simons and de Haan2012) and via advertisements. Because of the influence of gender and the menstrual cycle on stress-induced cortisol levels (Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Gaab, Schommer, & Hellhammer, Reference Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Gaab, Schommer and Hellhammer1999) we only included male participants. Participants in the control group were matched to siblings based on demographical data and both groups were subsequently randomly assigned to the validated stress or no-stress condition of the TSST (see below for detailed description). None of the participants suffered from a psychiatric disorder [as assessed with a semi-structured interview by a trained researcher (the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller and Dunbar1998))], used any synthetic corticosteroids, carried ferromagnetic objects in their body, or suffered from claustrophobia. Furthermore, controls did not have first-degree relatives with a psychiatric disorder. The current use of psychoactive substances (amphetamines, cocaine, opiates, methadone, benzodiazepines, and cannabinoids) was determined with a urine multi-drug screening device (Multi-line) and self-report questionnaire. None of the participants reported the use of drugs in the past 3 days. Two participants (1 control and 1 sibling) tested positive for cannabis. Exclusion of these participants did not influence any of the results. One participant was excluded due to technical problems with the magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-scanner (sibling-stress group). This resulted in four experimental groups: control-no-stress (n = 20), control-stress (n = 20), sibling-no-stress (n = 20), and sibling-stress (n = 19). Differences in demographics between the four groups were analyzed using one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) or χ2 tests using SPSS 23.0 (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, Chicago, IL).

Prior to the experiment, all participants gave written informed consent. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

General procedures and stress induction

We told participants that the study investigated the effects of ‘cognitive load’ on the brain. We provided all information regarding the study purpose during debriefing. Participants were instructed to refrain from heavy exercise (2 h prior to participation) and caffeine intake (4 h prior to participation). The experimental scan session started with an anatomical and resting state scan (RS0) to acclimatize all participants to the scanning environment. The first scan session was followed by the stress or control condition of the TSST. The first post-stress resting state (RS1) was acquired 20 min following TSST onset, followed by an emotion processing task [viewing and rating pictures from the international affective picture system (van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, Vink, Fernández, Hermans, Joëls, Kahn and Vinkers2018)] and a reward processing task (van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, Vink, Joëls, Kahn, Hermans and Vinkers2019). The second post-stress resting state scan (RS2) was acquired 90 min following TSST onset. The TSST was carried out as previously published (Kirschbaum, Pirke, & Hellhammer, Reference Kirschbaum, Pirke and Hellhammer1993), between 4:30 and 8:30 PM to minimize variation in diurnal cortisol secretion. In short, participants received instructions 5 min prior to the stress or control condition, which was carried out outside the scanner in a separate room. The stress condition consisted of a 5 min job interview, followed by a 3 min mental arithmetic task in front of a committee (one woman and one man). The control condition consisted of a free speech (5 min) followed by a simple arithmetic task (3 min) (Het, Rohleder, Schoofs, Kirschbaum, & Wolf, Reference Het, Rohleder, Schoofs, Kirschbaum and Wolf2009). The experimenter was in the same room but did not evaluate the participant, nor was there a committee present.

Cortisol and subjective stress

We obtained seven saliva samples throughout the experiment using salivettes (Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany) for the quantification of cortisol and alpha-amylase [an indirect marker of adrenergic activity (van Stegeren, Rohleder, Everaerd, & Wolf, Reference van Stegeren, Rohleder, Everaerd and Wolf2006)] at the following time points: −10, +5, +20, +30, +65, +90, and +120 min relative to TSST onset. Samples were temporarily stored at 4 °C and subsequently stored at −20 °C. Cortisol and alpha-amylase levels were analyzed as described previously (Vinkers et al., Reference Vinkers, Zorn, Cornelisse, Koot, Houtepen, Olivier and Joëls2013). In short, cortisol was measured without extraction using an in-house competitive radio-immunoassay, and alpha-amylase was measured using a Beckman-Coulter AU5811 chemistry analyzer. Three out of 497 cortisol samples were not collected (all non-peak values) and these missing values were calculated based on the mean group decline. The area under the curve (AUCi) for cortisol was calculated as previously described (Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid, & Hellhammer, Reference Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid and Hellhammer2003). The effects of time, stress, group, and their interaction on cortisol and alpha-amylase levels were analyzed using repeated measures ANOVAs using SPSS 23.0.

Structural and functional MRI acquisition

All imaging was performed on a Philips 3.0-T whole-body MRI scanner (Philips Medical Systems). First, a whole-brain three-dimensional T1 weighted structural image was acquired with the following scan parameters: voxel size 1 mm isotropic; repetition time (TR) = 10 ms; echo time (TE) = 4.6 ms; FOV = 240 × 240 × 160 mm; flip angle = 8°. Functional images were obtained using a two-dimensional echo planar imaging-sensitivity encoding sequence with the following parameters: voxel size 3 mm, slice thickness = 3 mm, TR = 2000 ms; TE = 35 ms; 35 slices; gap = 0.43 mm; flip angle = 70°. Two hundred and two functional scans were acquired during the resting state scan (acquisition time: 7 min). A small white cross on a black background was shown for the entire scan duration. Participants were instructed to lay still, keep their eyes open and stay awake.

Preprocessing

We performed preprocessing using the tools from the Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain (FMRIB) Software Library [FSL; http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl; RRID:SCR_002823; (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Jenkinson, Woolrich, Beckmann, Behrens, Johansen-Berg and Matthews2004)]. First, correction for head movement was done using fMRI Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT) and Independent Component Analysis-based Automatic Removal of Motion Artifacts [ICA-AROMA; (Pruim et al., Reference Pruim, Mennes, van Rooij, Llera, Buitelaar and Beckmann2015)] after which a high pass filter of 128 s was applied. Then, the functional images were coregistered to the anatomical image and normalized in SPM12 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) using unified segmentation (Ashburner & Friston, Reference Ashburner and Friston2005). All images were spatially smoothed using an 8-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel.

Given (1) the between-subject design, allowing a direct comparisons between functional connectivity during stress and no-stress conditions and (2) the portion of participants that had never been in an MRI-scanner before the experiment [which is stressful in itself (Muehlhan, Lueken, Wittchen, & Kirschbaum, Reference Muehlhan, Lueken, Wittchen and Kirschbaum2011)], we focused on the two post-stress resting state scans in this paper and disregarded RS0. Indeed, cortisol levels were higher directly after RS0 for scanner-naïve participants (online Supplementary Fig. S1B), and scanner-related subjective stress was highest before the first MRI-scan (online Supplementary Fig. S1A), suggesting that in general the first functional scan session of an experiment should not be used as baseline resting-state scan but rather to acclimatize participants to the scanner environment.

Functional connectivity

To analyze network connectivity on the subject-level, we extracted and averaged the time series from all voxels within pre-defined core nodes of the salience and ECNs (network signal) (Shirer, Ryali, Rykhlevskaia, Menon, & Greicius, Reference Shirer, Ryali, Rykhlevskaia, Menon and Greicius2012). The SN seeds consisted of the dACC and bilateral anterior insula. The ECN consisted of the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal and bilateral posterior parietal cortices. For all seeds, we selected only voxels within gray matter. Timeseries extraction was performed on unsmoothed data. The averaged timeseries of each of the two networks and each of the two sessions was fed into a separate first-level ANOVA, resulting in four connectivity maps per participant (two sessions and two networks). We opted not to include additional regressors since nuisance signals (e.g. motion-related signal fluctuations) were already removed from the data using the ICA-AROMA preprocessing step explained above.

To investigate the acute and delayed effects of stress on SN and ECN connectivity in both groups, we applied two second level full-factorial ANOVAs with stress (stress/no-stress) and group (control/sibling) as between-subject factors: one with the connectivity maps from RS1 and one from RS2. Statistical parametric maps were thresholded at a cluster-level whole-brain family-wise error (FWE) corrected threshold of p < 0.05 (cluster-forming threshold at the voxel-level: p < 0.001, uncorrected). For hypothesis-driven investigations of network changes under acute stress in regions of interest (ROIs) for which we had a-priori hypotheses, we applied small-volume corrections with search regions. We identified the left and right insula and the dACC as ROIs for the SN analyses, and the left and right middle frontal gyrus and the left and right parietal cortex (a combination of the inferior parietal and the angular gyrus) for the ECN analyses. All ROIs were based on the automatic anatomical labeling atlas (AAL, Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., Reference Tzourio-Mazoyer, Landeau, Papathanassiou, Crivello, Etard, Delcroix and Joliot2002).

Results

Demographics

The four groups did not differ in age, BMI, education, and self-reported childhood trauma (Table 1).

Table 1. Group characteristics

Con, control participant; Sib, sibling of schizophrenia patient.

Mean values (standard error of the mean) are denoted for age, education, body mass index (BMI), and childhood trauma. Handedness is reported in total right/left/both hand preference.

a Comparisons were made between the four groups.

Cortisol, alpha-amylase, and subjective stress

First, we validated successful stress induction by the TSST. Indeed, the TSST affected salivary cortisol levels over time (time × stress interaction F = 12.401, p = 1.833 × 10−9, η p2 = 0.515) and the area under the curve was larger for stress than control sessions (F = 21.621, p = 1.4 × 10−5, η p2 = 0.224). Time-dependent changes appeared to be different between controls and siblings (time × group × stress interaction F = 2.310, p = 0.043, η p2 = 0.165) but follow-up analyses did not reveal significant differences on individual samples (stress × group interaction on all assessments p > 0.05). Moreover, the AUCi for stress v. no-stress sessions, a robust measurement of stress reactivity, did not differ between controls and siblings (stress × group interaction F = 2.240, p = 0.139, η p2 = 0.029), indicating that indeed there was no systematic difference between healthy controls and siblings of schizophrenia patients in stress-induced cortisol concentrations (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol responses to stress. Con = healthy control, Sib = sibling of schizophrenia patient. * p < 0.05. Error bars represent standard error of the mean (s.e.m.).

We then investigated the effect of the TSST on alpha-amylase levels, an indirect marker for adrenergic activity (van Stegeren et al., Reference van Stegeren, Rohleder, Everaerd and Wolf2006). Overall, alpha-amylase concentrations were higher in the stress compared to the no-stress condition (main effect of stress F = 5.757, p = 0.019, η p2 = 0.074, main effect of group or group × stress interaction p > 0.05). We found a trend towards a similar effect of the TSST on alpha-amylase AUCi (F = 3.736, p = 0.057, η p2 = 0.047, main effect of stress and group × stress interaction p > 0.05). Focusing on the no-stress condition only, we found that siblings of schizophrenia patients had higher alpha-amylase levels throughout almost the entire experiment (sibling v. control in the no-stress condition p < 0.05 at t = −10, 15, 60, 85, and 115 min relative to TSST onset), while there was no difference between siblings and controls in the stress condition, or between siblings in the no-stress condition and the stress condition (all comparisons p > 0.1). These results demonstrate increased alpha-amylase concentrations in response to the TSST across both participant groups and higher alpha-amylase levels in the absence of stress in siblings (Fig. 1).

Subjective stress increased in response to the TSST (time × stress interaction F = 7.911, p = 0.00078, η p2 = 0.178). These ratings did not differ between healthy controls and siblings (time × group × stress interaction F = 0.841, p = 0.435, η p2 = 0.023).

Salience network functional connectivity

We first investigated the acute response to stress (RS1), expecting increased SN functional connectivity in healthy controls. We found a significant difference between siblings and controls in stress-induced functional connectivity between the right insula and the rest of the SN (group × stress interaction T = 4.10, cluster size = 1120 mm3, p svc = 0.008) which survived whole brain correction as well (cluster size = 2808 mm3, p whole−brain = 0.046) (Fig. 2a). Post-hoc comparisons between the groups indeed revealed a significant increase in functional connectivity between the insula and the rest of the SN during stress relative to no-stress in controls (con-stress > con-no-stress T = 3.88, cluster size = 896 mm3, p svc = 0.012) but not in siblings (sib-stress > sib-no-stress; no suprathreshold clusters). Moreover, controls in the stress condition had higher connectivity levels than siblings in the stress condition at the trend level [con-stress > sib-stress (T = 3.43, cluster size = 24 mm3, p svc = 0.093; T = 3.30, cluster size = 32 mm3, p svc = 0.089)], supporting increased SN functional connectivity in stressed controls relative to stressed siblings, rather than a difference between the two groups during no-stress conditions. These findings indicate an increase in SN functional connectivity during stress in healthy controls, and a lack of SN reactivity in siblings.

Fig. 2. Effects of stress on SN functional connectivity in siblings of schizophrenia patients and controls. (a) Group (control/sibling) × stress (stress/no-stress) interaction in SN connectivity during acute stress. Controls and siblings differed in SN connectivity directly after stress (group × stress interaction (p svc < 0.05)) in the right insula. Follow up comparisons revealed that this was driven by a stress-induced increase in healthy controls but not in siblings. (b) Reduced SN connectivity in the aftermath of stress (blue) and a group (control/sibling) × stress (stress/no-stress) interaction (green) in the aftermath of stress. Functional connectivity between the SN and the right anterior insula decreased in the aftermath of stress in both healthy controls and siblings of schizophrenia patients (blue, no-stress > stress p svc = 0.048). Additionally, there was a decrease in functional connectivity between the SN and the posterior insula in the aftermath of stress in siblings (green, group × stress interaction p svc = 0.032 and p svc = 0.057). T-Maps are thresholded at p < 0.001 uncorrected and overlaid onto a normalized anatomical scan for visualization purposes. For a color version, see this figure online. See Table 2 for cluster level inferences.

We expected to find decreased SN functional connectivity in the late aftermath of stress (RS2, 90 min after TSST onset) in healthy controls, which would be affected in siblings. We found a group × stress interaction in the left insula, indicating a difference between the groups in functional connectivity between this area and the rest of the SN (T = 3.82, cluster size = 400 mm3, p svc = 0.032; T = 3.54, cluster size = 168 mm3, p svc = 0.057) (Fig. 2b). Follow up comparisons revealed a decrease in this area in siblings in the stress condition compared to the no-stress condition (sib-no-stress > sib-stress T = 3.85, cluster size 288 mm3, p svc = 0.041; T = 3.45, cluster size = 368, p svc = 0.034) and no difference between controls in the stress and no-stress condition (no suprathreshold clusters). Additionally, we found a main effect of stress on functional connectivity with the left anterior insula, with decreased levels in the stress groups (no-stress > stress T = 3.97, cluster size = 232 mm3, p svc = 0.048), which did not differ between siblings and controls (no suprathreshold clusters). These results support a general SN downregulation in the aftermath of stress in both groups, and an additional disconnect with the posterior insula in siblings only.

Executive control network functional connectivity

We first investigated ECN functional connectivity during the acute phase of stress (RS1), expecting decreased connectivity in healthy controls in the stress condition. However, we found no significant differences between the groups on ECN functional connectivity during this phase (stress > no-stress, no-stress > stress, group × stress interaction all p > 0.05).

During the late aftermath of stress (RS2), we expected to find increased ECN functional connectivity in healthy controls. We found a significant increase in functional connectivity between the ECN and the left and right cerebellar lobule VI in all participants (stress > no-stress left T = 4.69; cluster size = 5656 mm3, p whole−brain = 0.005, right T = 3.93, cluster size = 3392 mm3, p whole−brain = 0.037) (Fig. 3). This effect did not differ between healthy controls and siblings (group × stress interaction p > 0.05). Table 2 lists the group results for these analyses.

Fig. 3. Executive control functional connectivity in the aftermath of stress. Functional connectivity between the ECN and the left and right cerebellum lobule VI in the aftermath of stress was higher in stressed participants (stress > no-stress left: p fwe = 0.005, right, p fwe = 0.037). There were no differences between controls and siblings. The contrast map is displayed at p < 0.001 uncorrected and overlaid onto a normalized anatomical scan for visualization purposes. For a color version, see this figure online. See Table 2 for cluster level inferences.

Table 2. SN and ECN functional connectivity during acute stress (RS1) and the aftermath of stress (RS2)

MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute; R, right; L, left; Sib, sibling of schizophrenia patient; Con, control participant.

All effects are analyzed using cluster-level statistics, using a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected at the voxel level. For pairwise comparisons, only contrasts yielding significant clusters are displayed. ** p < 0.01 (whole brain corrected), * p < 0.05 (whole brain corrected); †† p < 0.01 (small-volume corrected for region of interest); † p < 0.05 (small-volume corrected for region of interest); # p < 0.1 (small volume corrected for region of interest).

Reported cluster sizes represented the size of a significant cluster after corrected for a small volume unless the cluster was significant at the whole brain level only.

Together, these results show no effect on ECN connectivity during acute stress, and a comparable increase in ECN-cerebellar connectivity in siblings and controls during the aftermath of stress.

Discussion

This is the first study that compared the rapid and delayed effects of stress on resting-state functional connectivity in healthy controls and siblings of schizophrenia patients, a group that is at increased risk for a wide range of stress-related psychopathology. We aimed to (1) investigate the acute and delayed effects of stress on functional connectivity of the SN and the ECN as proposed by Hermans and colleagues (Hermans et al., Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014) and (2) investigate whether and when differences existed between controls and siblings. We found that stress causes a rapid increase in functional connectivity within the SN in healthy controls, while siblings of schizophrenia patients failed to upregulate SN functional connectivity in response to stress. Furthermore, we found reduced SN functional connectivity and increased connectivity between the ECN and the cerebellum in the aftermath of stress in both controls and siblings, with an additional decrease in SN-posterior insula connectivity in siblings. These findings demonstrate time-dependent opposing effects of stress on SN functional connectivity in healthy controls, and reveal for the first time that the neural dynamics in siblings of schizophrenia patients are primarily affected during the acute phase of stress.

Differential effects of stress on salience network connectivity in siblings of schizophrenia patients

In line with the model proposed above, acute stress resulted in a rapid increase in functional connectivity between the right insula and the rest of the SN in healthy controls. Several studies have revealed immediate increases in the SN during or directly after stress (see Hermans et al. Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014; van Oort et al. Reference van Oort, Tendolkar, Hermans, Mulder, Beckmann, Schene and van Eijndhoven2017, for reviews). For example, immediately after stress, functional connectivity between the amygdala and other SN regions such as the insula and dACC was shown to be increased (van Marle et al., Reference van Marle, Hermans, Qin and Fernández2010). However, not all studies found increases in connectivity with the insula. A recent resting-state study found increased functional connectivity between the amygdala and the medial prefrontal cortex during resting state immediately after a stress induction paradigm, specifically in participants with strong cortisol responses (Quaedflieg et al., Reference Quaedflieg, van de Ven, Meyer, Siep, Merckelbach and Smeets2015). The insula is at the intersection of the cognitive, homeostatic, and affective systems, facilitating the bottom-up filtering of potential salient stimuli and, once a stimulus is detected, engaging other brain areas to initiate an appropriate response while disengaging areas that are not immediately relevant (Menon & Uddin, Reference Menon and Uddin2010). Therefore, our finding of increased SN-insula connectivity indicates an elevation in vigilance which may serve to quickly respond to changes in the environment.

In contrast to the SN effect in healthy controls, we found no increase in SN functional connectivity during acute stress in siblings of schizophrenia patients. There are at least two possible explanations for this finding. First, siblings of schizophrenia patients may be unable to upregulate SN functional connectivity in response to stress, while SN functional connectivity during baseline conditions is unaffected. Alternatively, SN functional connectivity may be chronically elevated in siblings and, as a consequence, a further increase in SN connectivity in stressful situations is compromised. The first explanation is supported by follow-up fMRI analyses between the four groups: SN connectivity during acute stress was lower in siblings compared to healthy controls, indicating impairments in salience processing and adequate response selection during stress. However, our finding of increased alpha-amylase levels in siblings during no-stress conditions appears more consistent with the second explanation. Alpha-amylase is an indirect marker of adrenergic activity (van Stegeren et al., Reference van Stegeren, Rohleder, Everaerd and Wolf2006), and higher baseline concentrations, which did not further increase in response to stress, point towards higher basal sympathetic/noradrenergic tone. Supporting hypervigilance in mental illness, early work revealed an increased central noradrenergic output in schizophrenia patients that predict relapse (Maas et al., Reference Maas, Contreras, Miller, Berman, Bowden, Javors and Weintrub1993; van Kammen et al., Reference van Kammen, Ågren, Yao, O'Conner, Gurklis and Peters1994; van Kammen & Kelley, Reference van Kammen and Kelley1991), and the effects of atypical antipsychotics on symptom reduction partly act through alpha-adrenergic receptors (Svensson, Reference Svensson2003). Possibly, a combination of the two explanations underlies the reduced responsiveness of the SN to stress in siblings. Elevated tonic norepinephrine levels in siblings may reduce detectable changes in phasic firing of noradrenergic neurons in the locus coeruleus (Aston-Jones & Cohen, Reference Aston-Jones and Cohen2005; Vazey, Moorman, & Aston-Jones, Reference Vazey, Moorman and Aston-Jones2018), and lead to an attenuation of SN responses to stressors. We therefore hypothesize that a dynamic co-activation of the insula with the rest of the SN is necessary to respond adequately to acute stressors, and that in siblings, compromised SN upregulation due to an attenuated dynamic range in the sympathetic/noradrenergic response may constitute a vulnerability factor for the development of psychopathology.

In contrast to these acute effects, the late aftermath of stress is important for the normalization of emotional reactivity and higher order cognitive processes of the event. We found reduced functional connectivity between the right insula and the rest of the SN during the aftermath of stress in both controls and siblings, confirming the hypothesis of opposing effects of stress on SN connectivity during the acute and aftermath phases of stress (Hermans et al., Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014). The late phase did not differ between the two groups, indicating that network connectivity during the aftermath of stress is not affected in siblings. However, siblings showed an additional reduction in functional connectivity between a more posterior part of the insula and the rest of the SN during the late aftermath of stress. Interestingly, this area overlapped with the area that increased during the acute stress phase in healthy controls only. The anterior and posterior parts of the insula are in general both activated during salient stimuli processing, but they have different input and output connections and play different roles in saliency. Through connections between the anterior insula and the motor cortex, activating the SN initiates rapid response selection, while the integration of homeostatic sensory signals (such as thermoregulation) is regulated through connections with the mid and posterior insula (Craig, Reference Craig2009; Menon, Reference Menon2011). During the acute phase of stress, siblings did not show an increase in connectivity with the posterior insula, but apparently they do reduce this connection during the aftermath of stress, implicating a similar downregulation of this area after stress in controls and siblings and a lack of increased connectivity with this area during stress in siblings.

Time-dependent effects of stress on executive control network functional connectivity

In contrast to the proposed model by Hermans et al. (Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014), we found no reduced ECN functional connectivity during acute stress. A possible explanation is that the ECN might have been insufficiently activated to be affected by stress, given that we measured functional connectivity during rest, unlike previous studies that investigated ECN functional activity during cognitive tasks (Cousijn, Rijpkema, Qin, van Wingen, & Fernández, Reference Cousijn, Rijpkema, Qin, van Wingen and Fernández2012; Oei, Tollenaar, Spinhoven, & Elzinga, Reference Oei, Tollenaar, Spinhoven and Elzinga2009; Qin et al., Reference Qin, Hermans, van Marle, Luo and Fernández2009). Another explanation is that intrinsic communication of the ECN is not affected, but that the time-locking of activity to external events in accordance with task demands is altered during acute stress, as proposed by a recent study that found effects of aversive movie viewing on SN, but not ECN functional connectivity (Young et al., Reference Young, Raz, Everaerd, Beckmann, Tendolkar, Hendler and Hermans2017).

Previous studies found that ECN functioning is increased in the late aftermath of stress, presumably through genomic actions of cortisol (Henckens, van Wingen, Joëls, & Fernández, Reference Henckens, van Wingen, Joëls and Fernández2011, Reference Henckens, van Wingen, Joëls and Fernández2012b; Hermans et al., Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014). Here, we found increased functional connectivity between the ECN and left and right lobule VI of the cerebellum, an area which co-activates with prefrontal and parietal regions during cognitive tasks such as language processing, working memory and executive function, even if a motor response is not required (Stoodley, Reference Stoodley2012). Damage to the cerebellum further leads to deficits in working memory, indicating that cognitive processes partly depend on the engagement of cortico-cerebellar circuits. These results suggest that both healthy controls and siblings increase executive functioning during stress recovery, but it should be noted that we did not find an effect on core ECN regions and therefore the role of the cerebellum in stress must be further investigated in future studies. Concluding, we found that stress only affected ECN functional connectivity with the cerebellum in the aftermath of stress, with no differences between healthy controls and siblings of schizophrenia patients.

Strengths and limitations

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the early and late effects of stress on SN and ECN functional connectivity in healthy controls and at-risk individuals. An important strength is that we used resting-state functional connectivity analyses which allow for repeated measurements without possible learning effects. Another strength of the study is that we induced stress using the TSST, a stress task that evokes the strongest cortisol response of all laboratory stressors (Skoluda et al., Reference Skoluda, Strahler, Schlotz, Niederberger, Marques, Fischer and Nater2015). Reduced SN connectivity in the aftermath of stress is presumably driven by the genomic effects of cortisol, and therefore this finding might not have been detected if another stressor was used. On the other hand, moving the participants from the TSST room to the MRI scanner induces a delay, resulting in lower adrenergic activity during scanning. Therefore, stronger effects on network connectivity during acute stress might have been observed if we had chosen a different stressor, such as the presentation of aversive movie clips (Young et al., Reference Young, Raz, Everaerd, Beckmann, Tendolkar, Hendler and Hermans2017), the cold pressor task (Blandini, Martignoni, Sances, Bono, & Nappi, Reference Blandini, Martignoni, Sances, Bono and Nappi1995), or the montreal imaging stress task (Dedovic et al., Reference Dedovic, Renwick, Mahani, Engert, Lupien and Pruessner2005). Furthermore, we obtained the mean time course of each entire network, which was used as a regressor of interest in the first-level analyses. It is therefore possible that changes during and after stress are driven by one of the seed regions of the network. Moreover, it should be noted that we found effects in SN functional connectivity in opposing hemispheres during the acute and late phases of stress. However, since the conventional statistical parametric mapping approach does not allow for a direct comparison between the left and right hemispheres, this does not warrant conclusions about lateralization regarding the different stress phases. Furthermore, while the use of resting-state analyses has it benefits, it is difficult to make inferences about the effects of stress on responses to the environment. A previous study, however, found a strong overlap between task-related brain networks and networks during rest (Mennes et al., Reference Mennes, Kelly, Zuo, Di Martino, Biswal, Castellanos and Milham2010), and interindividual differences during rest also correspond with interindividual differences during tasks (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Fox, Miller, Glahn, Fox, Mackay and Beckmann2009). This implies that the observed responses to stress during rest might also be there during tasks. An important limitation is that our sample consisted of males only, and our results are therefore not generalizable to females. Given that there are gender-related differences in behavioral and neural responses to stress (Bangasser, Eck, & Ordoñes Sanchez, Reference Bangasser, Eck and Ordoñes Sanchez2019; Kelly, Tyrka, Anderson, Price, & Carpenter, Reference Kelly, Tyrka, Anderson, Price and Carpenter2008; Lighthall et al., Reference Lighthall, Sakaki, Vasunilashorn, Nga, Somayajula, Chen and Mather2012), future studies should compare males to females directly.

Conclusion

We aimed to investigate whether at-risk individuals differ from healthy controls in the temporal dynamics of networks involved in the central stress response. Our results show similarities during the aftermath of stress: both groups showed the expected changes in brain functioning during stress recovery. However, the groups differed during the acute phase of the stress response: at-risk individuals lacked the increase in neural resources necessary to quickly and adequately cope with a stressor, possibly as a consequence of chronically elevated catecholaminergic activity. Our results therefore offer a new explanation for the increased susceptibility for the development of stress-related psychopathology in this at-risk group.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719004033.

Financial support

This work was supported by a Brain Center Rudolf Magnus Research Fellowship (2015) and a European Research Council Grant No. ERC-2015-CoG 682591.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no competing financial interests.

References

Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10(6), 410422. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2648CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. NeuroImage, 26(3), 839851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 403450. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bangasser, D. A., Eck, S. R., & Ordoñes Sanchez, E. (2019). Sex differences in stress reactivity in arousal and attention systems. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44, 129139. 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0137-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blandini, F., Martignoni, E., Sances, E., Bono, G., & Nappi, G. (1995). Combined response of plasma and platelet catecholamines to different types of short-term stress. Life Sciences, 56(13), 11131120. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3205(95)00048-BCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butjosa, A., Gómez-Benito, J., Huerta-Ramos, E., Del cacho, N., Barajas, A., Baños, I., … Ochoa, S. (2016). Incidence of stressful life events and influence of sociodemographic and clinical variables on the onset of first-episode psychosis. Psychiatry Research, 245, 108115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.08.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, C.-M., Chang, W.-H., Chen, M.-H., Tsai, C.-F., Su, T.-P., Li, C.-T., … Bai, Y.-M. (2017). Co-aggregation of major psychiatric disorders in individuals with first-degree relatives with schizophrenia: A nationwide population-based study. Molecular Psychiatry, 23, 17561763. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.217CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cousijn, H., Rijpkema, M., Qin, S., van Wingen, G. A., & Fernández, G. (2012). Phasic deactivation of the medial temporal lobe enables working memory processing under stress. NeuroImage, 59(2), 11611167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 5970. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Kloet, E. R., Karst, H., & Joëls, M. (2008). Corticosteroid hormones in the central stress response: Quick-and-slow. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 29(2), 268272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2007.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Kloet, E. R., Meijer, O. C., de Nicola, A. F., de Rijk, R. H., & Joëls, M. (2018). Importance of the brain corticosteroid receptor balance in metaplasticity, cognitive performance and neuro-inflammation. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 49, 124145. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.YFRNE.2018.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dedovic, K., Renwick, R., Mahani, N. K., Engert, V., Lupien, S. J., & Pruessner, J. C. (2005). The montreal imaging stress task: Using functional imaging to investigate the effects of perceiving and processing psychosocial stress in the human brain. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN, 30(5), 319325. Retrieved from http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1197276&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstractGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 355391. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henckens, M. J. A. G., Pu, Z., Hermans, E. J., van Wingen, G. A., Joëls, M., & Fernández, G. (2012). Dynamically changing effects of corticosteroids on human hippocampal and prefrontal processing. Human Brain Mapping, 33(12), 28852897. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henckens, M. J. A. G., van Wingen, G. A., Joels, M., & Fernandez, G. (2010). Time-dependent effects of corticosteroids on human amygdala processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(38), 1272512732. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3112-10.2010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henckens, M. J. A. G., van Wingen, G. A., Joëls, M., & Fernández, G. (2011). Time-dependent corticosteroid modulation of prefrontal working memory processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(14), 58015806. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1019128108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henckens, M. J. A. G., van Wingen, G. A., Joëls, M., & Fernández, G. (2012a). Corticosteroid induced decoupling of the amygdala in men. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 22(10), 23362345. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henckens, M. J. A. G., van Wingen, G. A., Joëls, M., & Fernández, G. (2012b). Time-dependent effects of cortisol on selective attention and emotional interference: A functional MRI study. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 66. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2012.00066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, E. J., Henckens, M. J. A. G., Joëls, M., & Fernández, G. (2014). Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(6), 304314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J. F., Ossewaarde, L., Henckens, M. J. A. G., Qin, S., van Kesteren, M. T. R., … Fernández, G. (2011). Stress-related noradrenergic activity prompts large-scale neural network reconfiguration. Science (New York, N.Y.), 334(6059), 11511153. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1209603CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Het, S., Rohleder, N., Schoofs, D., Kirschbaum, C., & Wolf, O. T. (2009). Neuroendocrine and psychometric evaluation of a placebo version of the ‘Trier Social Stress Test’. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(7), 10751086. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Homberg, J. R., Kozicz, T., & Fernández, G. (2017). Large-scale network balances in the transition from adaptive to maladaptive stress responses. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 14, 2732. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joëls, M., Sarabdjitsingh, R. A., & Karst, H. (2012). Unraveling the time domains of corticosteroid hormone influences on brain activity: Rapid, slow, and chronic modes. Pharmacological Reviews, 64(4), 901938. https://doi.org/10.1124/pr.112.005892CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalisch, R., Baker, U., Dewleen, G. B., Boks, M. P., Bonanno, G. A., Brummelman, E., … Kleim, B. (2017). The resilience framework as a strategy to combat stress-related disorders. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 784790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, M. M., Tyrka, A. R., Anderson, G. M., Price, L. H., & Carpenter, L. L. (2008). Sex differences in emotional and physiological responses to the Trier Social Stress Test. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39(1), 8798. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JBTEP.2007.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Gaab, J., Schommer, N. C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1999). Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61(2), 154162. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10204967CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’ – a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28(1–2), 7681. https://doi.org/119004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenders, M. A., Giltay, E. J., Spijker, A. T., Hoencamp, E., Spinhoven, P., & Elzinga, B. M. (2014). Stressful life events in bipolar I and II disorder: Cause or consequence of mood symptoms? Journal of Affective Disorders, 161, 5564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.02.036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korver, N., Quee, P. J., Boos, H. B. M., Simons, C. J. P., de Haan, L., & GROUP investigators. (2012). Genetic risk and outcome of psychosis (GROUP), a multi site longitudinal cohort study focused on gene-environment interaction: Objectives, sample characteristics, recruitment and assessment methods. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21(3), 205221. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lex, C., Bäzner, E., & Meyer, T. D. (2017). Does stress play a significant role in bipolar disorder? A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 208, 298308. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JAD.2016.08.057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lighthall, N. R., Sakaki, M., Vasunilashorn, S., Nga, L., Somayajula, S., Chen, E. Y., … Mather, M. (2012). Gender differences in reward-related decision processing under stress. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(4), 476484. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maas, J. W., Contreras, S. A., Miller, A. L., Berman, N., Bowden, C. L., Javors, M. A., … Weintrub, S. (1993). Studies of catecholamine metabolism in schizophrenia/psychosis-I. Neuropsychopharmacology, 8(2), 97109. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.1993.11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCraw, S., & Parker, G. (2017). The prevalence and outcomes of exposure to potentially traumatic stressful life events compared across patients with bipolar disorder and unipolar depression. Psychiatry Research, 255, 399404. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.06.070CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mennes, M., Kelly, C., Zuo, X.-N., Di Martino, A., Biswal, B. B., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2010). Inter-individual differences in resting-state functional connectivity predict task-induced BOLD activity. NeuroImage, 50(4), 16901701. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2010.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 483506. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2011.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure & Function, 214(5–6), 655667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muehlhan, M., Lueken, U., Wittchen, H.-U., & Kirschbaum, C. (2011). The scanner as a stressor: Evidence from subjective and neuroendocrine stress parameters in the time course of a functional magnetic resonance imaging session. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79(2), 118126. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.IJPSYCHO.2010.09.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., Marcelis, M., Krabbendam, L., Delespaul, P., & van Os, J. (2005). Subtle fluctuations in psychotic phenomena as functional states of abnormal dopamine reactivity in individuals at risk. Biological Psychiatry, 58(2), 105110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Negrón-Oyarzo, I., Aboitiz, F., & Fuentealba, P. (2016). Impaired functional connectivity in the prefrontal cortex: A mechanism for chronic stress-induced neuropsychiatric disorders. Neural Plasticity, 2016, 7539065. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/7539065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oei, N. Y. L., Tollenaar, M. S., Spinhoven, P., & Elzinga, B. M. (2009). Hydrocortisone reduces emotional distracter interference in working memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(9), 12841293. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2009.03.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelletier-Baldelli, A., Bernard, J. A., & Mittal, V. A. (2015). Intrinsic functional connectivity in salience and default mode networks and aberrant social processes in youth at ultra-high risk for psychosis. PloS One, 10(8), e0134936. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134936CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28(7), 916931. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00108-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruim, R. H. R., Mennes, M., van Rooij, D., Llera, A., Buitelaar, J. K., & Beckmann, C. F. (2015). ICA-AROMA: A robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. NeuroImage, 112, 267277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qin, S., Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J. F., Luo, J., & Fernández, G. (2009). Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biological Psychiatry, 66(1), 2532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quaedflieg, C. W. E. M., van de Ven, V., Meyer, T., Siep, N., Merckelbach, H., & Smeets, T. (2015). Temporal dynamics of stress-induced alternations of intrinsic amygdala connectivity and neuroendocrine levels. PloS One, 10(5), e0124141. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter, M. (2002). Impact of cortisol on emotions under stress and nonstress conditions: A pharmacopsychological approach. Neuropsychobiology, 46(1), 4148. https://doi.org/10.1159/000063575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwabe, L., Tegenthoff, M., Höffken, O., & Wolf, O. T. (2010). Concurrent glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity shifts instrumental behavior from goal-directed to habitual control. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 30(24), 81908196. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0734-10.2010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapero, B. G., Weiss, R. B., Burke, T. A., Boland, E. M., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2017). Kindling of life stress in bipolar disorder: Effects of early adversity. Behavior Therapy, 48(3), 322334. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BETH.2016.12.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D. V, Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., … Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 2), 2233, quiz 34-57. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9881538Google ScholarPubMed
Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2016). The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 651668. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2016.06.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shirer, W. R., Ryali, S., Rykhlevskaia, E., Menon, V., & Greicius, M. D. (2012). Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 22(1), 158165. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skoluda, N., Strahler, J., Schlotz, W., Niederberger, L., Marques, S., Fischer, S., … Nater, U. M. (2015). Intra-individual psychological and physiological responses to acute laboratory stressors of different intensity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 51, 227236. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2014.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., Mackay, C. E., … Beckmann, C. F. (2009). Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(31), 1304013045. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905267106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Johansen-Berg, H., … Matthews, P. M. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage, 23, S208S219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommer, I. E. C., Diederen, K. M. J., Blom, J.-D., Willems, A., Kushan, L., Slotema, K., … Kahn, R. S. (2008). Auditory verbal hallucinations predominantly activate the right inferior frontal area. Brain, 131(12), 31693177. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoodley, C. (2012). The Cerebellum and Cognition: Evidence from Functional Imaging Studies. The Cerebellum, 11, 352365. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Svensson, T. H. (2003). α-Adrenoceptor modulation hypothesis of antipsychotic atypicality. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 27(7), 11451158. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PNPBP.2003.09.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., … Joliot, M. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage, 15(1), 273289. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Kammen, D. P., Ågren, H., Yao, J. K., O'Conner, D. T., Gurklis, J., & Peters, J. L. (1994). Noradrenergic activity and prediction of psychotic relapse following haloperidol withdrawal in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 379384. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.151.3.379Google Scholar
van Kammen, D. P., & Kelley, M. (1991). Dopamine and norepinephrine activity in schizophrenia: An intergrative perspective. Schizophrenia Research, 4(2), 173191. https://doi.org/10.1016/0920-9964(91)90032-MCrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Leeuwen, J. M. C., Vink, M., Fernández, G., Hermans, E. J., Joëls, M., Kahn, R. S., & Vinkers, C. H. (2018). At-risk individuals display altered brain activity following stress. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43, 19541960. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0026-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Leeuwen, J. M. C., Vink, M., Joëls, M., Kahn, R. S., Hermans, E. J., & Vinkers, C. H. (2019). Increased responses of the reward circuitry to positive task feedback following acute stress in healthy controls but not in siblings of schizophrenia patients. NeuroImage, 184, 547554. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2018.09.051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Marle, H. J. F., Hermans, E. J., Qin, S., & Fernández, G. (2010). Enhanced resting-state connectivity of amygdala in the immediate aftermath of acute psychological stress. NeuroImage, 53(1), 348354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.070CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Oort, J., Tendolkar, I., Hermans, E., Mulder, P., Beckmann, C., Schene, A., … van Eijndhoven, P. (2017). How the brain connects in response to acute stress: A review at the human brain systems level. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 281297. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2017.10.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Stegeren, A. H., Rohleder, N., Everaerd, W., & Wolf, O. (2006). Salivary alpha amylase as marker for adrenergic activity during stress: Effect of betablockade. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(1), 137141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.05.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vazey, E. M., Moorman, D. E., & Aston-Jones, G. (2018). Phasic locus coeruleus activity regulates cortical encoding of salience information. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(40), E9439E9448. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1803716115Google ScholarPubMed
Vinkers, C. H., Zorn, J. V., Cornelisse, S., Koot, S., Houtepen, L. C., Olivier, B., … Joëls, M. (2013). Time-dependent changes in altruistic punishment following stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(9), 14671475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, C., Ji, F., Hong, Z., Poh, J. S., Krishnan, R., Lee, J., … Zhou, J. (2016). Disrupted salience network functional connectivity and white-matter microstructure in persons at risk for psychosis: Findings from the LYRIKS study. Psychological Medicine, 46(13), 27712783. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716001410CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, C. B., Raz, G., Everaerd, D., Beckmann, C. F., Tendolkar, I., Hendler, T., … Hermans, E. J. (2017). Dynamic shifts in large-scale brain network balance as a function of arousal. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(2), 281290. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1759-16.2016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Group characteristics

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol responses to stress. Con = healthy control, Sib = sibling of schizophrenia patient. * p < 0.05. Error bars represent standard error of the mean (s.e.m.).

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Effects of stress on SN functional connectivity in siblings of schizophrenia patients and controls. (a) Group (control/sibling) × stress (stress/no-stress) interaction in SN connectivity during acute stress. Controls and siblings differed in SN connectivity directly after stress (group × stress interaction (psvc < 0.05)) in the right insula. Follow up comparisons revealed that this was driven by a stress-induced increase in healthy controls but not in siblings. (b) Reduced SN connectivity in the aftermath of stress (blue) and a group (control/sibling) × stress (stress/no-stress) interaction (green) in the aftermath of stress. Functional connectivity between the SN and the right anterior insula decreased in the aftermath of stress in both healthy controls and siblings of schizophrenia patients (blue, no-stress > stress psvc = 0.048). Additionally, there was a decrease in functional connectivity between the SN and the posterior insula in the aftermath of stress in siblings (green, group × stress interaction psvc = 0.032 and psvc = 0.057). T-Maps are thresholded at p < 0.001 uncorrected and overlaid onto a normalized anatomical scan for visualization purposes. For a color version, see this figure online. See Table 2 for cluster level inferences.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Executive control functional connectivity in the aftermath of stress. Functional connectivity between the ECN and the left and right cerebellum lobule VI in the aftermath of stress was higher in stressed participants (stress > no-stress left: pfwe = 0.005, right, pfwe = 0.037). There were no differences between controls and siblings. The contrast map is displayed at p < 0.001 uncorrected and overlaid onto a normalized anatomical scan for visualization purposes. For a color version, see this figure online. See Table 2 for cluster level inferences.

Figure 4

Table 2. SN and ECN functional connectivity during acute stress (RS1) and the aftermath of stress (RS2)

Supplementary material: File

van Leeuwen et al. supplementary material

Figure S1

Download van Leeuwen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 35.1 KB