Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T10:57:17.293Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mentalising impairment as a trait marker of schizophrenia?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Esther Pousa
Affiliation:
Department Psychiatry and Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Parc Taulí Hospital, Barcelona, Spain. Email: epousa@tauli.cat
Ada I. Ruiz
Affiliation:
Hospital del Mar, Institut Municipal d'Assistència Sanitària, Barcelona, Spain
Anthony S. David
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2008 

One of the most controversial issues in ‘theory of mind’ research in schizophrenia in recent years has been whether theory of mind impairment may be seen as a trait marker or rather linked to particular symptoms. Sprong et al Reference Sprong, Schothorst, Vos, Hox and van Engeland1 conclude that evidence to date seems to favour the notion that mentalising impairment represents a possible trait marker. We believe that their meta-analysis is an excellent piece of scientific work but that this conclusion should remain tentative.

First, the existing evidence on theory of mind abilities in remitted patients is limited and difficult to interpret because of methodological shortcomings, such as non-explicit criteria for remission and poor control of cognitive abilities in the experimental design. A recent study by our group revealed that as a whole, stable patients did not show theory of mind impairment compared with carefully matched non-psychiatric controls. When standard consensus criteria for remission were applied to the sample, half failed to meet criteria for remission and showed a significantly worse theory of mind performance than remitted patients and controls. Specific theory of mind deficits in this group were associated with delusions. Thus, specific theory of mind impairment could go hand-in-hand with the presence of symptoms. Reference Pousa, Duñó, Brébion, David, Ruiz and Obiols2

Second, findings of theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia high-risk groups seem to support the assumption that theory of mind deficits represent a trait marker of the disorder. However, since these studies are mostly correlational, it is possible that the continuity of theory of mind deficits among ‘at risk’ groups may in fact derive from an intrinsic relationship between a psychotic symptoms continuum and theory of mind impairment. A review of the literature of theory of mind and schizotypal personality traits reveals that studies finding a positive significant relationship do so mainly with respect to schizotypal positive traits such as the cognitive-perceptual and unusual experiences dimensions of the schizotypy instruments. Reference Pickup3 Regarding investigations of first-degree relatives, evidence is controversial, Reference Sprong, Schothorst, Vos, Hox and van Engeland1 with findings of impaired performance on the more common types of theory of mind tasks but not on the ‘eyes’ test. However, it should be noted from these studies that those controlling for subclinical symptoms or schizotypal traits conclude that the association may be linked exclusively to the presence of subclinical positive symptoms. Reference Irani, Platek, Panyavin, Calkins, Kohler, Siegel, Schachter, Gur and Gur4,Reference Marjoram, Miller, McIntosh, Cunningham Owens, Johnstone and Lawrie5

In our opinion, the existing evidence in theory of mind research is still limited but the possibility of a state-like association should not be ruled out. The most methodologically sound means to explore this would be to carry out longitudinal studies comparing theory of mind abilities in different phases of the illness, defined by explicit criteria. Future studies also need to differentiate between the affective and cognitive aspects of theory of mind, since it is possible that these show a different pattern of relationship with symptom clusters or schizophrenia profiles. Furthermore, it is possible that future research reveals that state–trait interactions may be occurring.

Footnotes

Edited by Kiriakos Xenitidis and Colin Campbell

References

1 Sprong, M, Schothorst, P, Vos, E, Hox, J, van Engeland, H. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 2007; 191: 513.Google Scholar
2 Pousa, E, Duñó, R, Brébion, G, David, AS, Ruiz, AI, Obiols, JE. Theory of mind deficits in chronic schizophrenia: evidence for state dependence. Psychiatry Res 2007; in press.Google Scholar
3 Pickup, GJ. Theory of mind and its relation to schizotypy. Cognit Neuropsychiatry 2006; 11: 177–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Irani, F, Platek, SM, Panyavin, IS, Calkins, ME, Kohler, C, Siegel, SJ, Schachter, M, Gur, RE, Gur, RC. Self-face recognition and theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia and first-degree relatives. Schizophr Res 2006; 88: 151–60.Google Scholar
5 Marjoram, D, Miller, P, McIntosh, AM, Cunningham Owens, DG, Johnstone, EC, Lawrie, S. A neuropsychological investigation into ‘Theory of Mind’ and enhanced risk of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2006; 144: 2937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.