Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T14:01:42.569Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

SALIENCE, COGNITION, LANGUAGE COMPLEXITY, AND COMPLEX ADAPTIVE SYSTEMS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 May 2016

Nick C. Ellis*
Affiliation:
University of Michigan

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, J. R. (1990). The adaptive character of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1991). Is human cognition adaptive? Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 14(3), 471517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (2009). Cognitive psychology and its implications (7th ed.). New York: Worth.Google Scholar
Beckett, S. (1954). Waiting for Godot. New York: Grove Press.Google Scholar
Beckner, C., Blythe, R. A., Bybee, J., Christiansen, M. H., Croft, W., Ellis, N. C., & Schoenemann, T. (2009). Language is a complex adaptive system: Position paper. Language Learning, 59(Suppl. 1), 126.Google Scholar
Bentz, C., Verkerk, A., Kiela, D., Hill, F., & Buttery, P. (in press). Adaptive communication: Languages with more non-native speakers tend to have fewer word forms.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bulté, B., & Housen, A. (2012). Defining and operationalising L2 complexity. In Housen, A., Kuiken, F., & Vedder, I. (Eds.), Dimensions of L2 performance and proficiency: Investigating complexity, accuracy and fluency in SLA (pp. 2146). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2003). Mechanisms of change in grammaticalization: The role of frequency. In Janda, R. D. & Joseph, B. D. (Eds.), Handbook of historical linguistics (pp. 602–623). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (2006). From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. Language, 82, 711733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cerezo, L., Caras, A., & Leow, R. P. (2016). Effectiveness of guided induction versus deductive instruction on the development of complex Spanish “gustar” structures: An analysis of learning outcomes and processes. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(2), 265291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 181204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, L., & Ellis, N. C. (Eds.). (2009). Input and second language construction learning: Frequency, form, and function [Special issue]. Modern Language Journal, 93(2).Google Scholar
Collins, L., Trofimovich, P., White, J., Cardoso, W., & Horst, M. (2009). Some input on the easy/difficult grammar question: An empirical study. The Modern Language Journal, 93(3), 336353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Bot, K., Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2007). A dynamic systems theory to second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Della Putta, P. (2016). The effects of Textual Enhancement on the acquisition of two nonparallel grammatical features by Spanish-speaking learners of Italian. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(2), 217238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (1998). Emergentism, connectionism and language learning. Language Learning, 48(4), 631664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 143188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2006). Selective attention and transfer phenomena in SLA: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied Linguistics, 27(2), 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2008). The dynamics of second language emergence: Cycles of language use, language change, and language acquisition. Modern Language Journal, 41(3), 232249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (in press-a). Chunking. In Hundt, M., Pfenninger, S., & Mollin, S. (Eds.), The changing English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (in press-b). Salience. In Hundt, M., Pfenninger, S., & Mollin, S. (Eds.), The changing English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (Eds.). (2006a). Language emergence: Implications for applied linguistics [Special issue]. Applied Linguistics, 27(4).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (2006b). Language emergence: Implications for applied linguistics (Introduction to the special issue). Applied Linguistics, 27(4), 558589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (Eds.). (2009). Language as a complex adaptive system. Mahwah, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., Römer, U., & O’Donnell, M. B. (2016). Language usage, acquisition, and processing: Cognitive and corpus investigations of construction grammar. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science, 14, 179211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K., & Simon, H. A. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (2nd ed.). Boston: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gescheider, G. A. (2013). Psychophysics: The fundamentals. Hove Sussex: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In Shaw, R. & Bransford, J. (Eds.), Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldschneider, J. M., & DeKeyser, R. (2001). Explaining the “natural order of L2 morpheme acquisition” in English: A meta-analysis of multiple determinants. Language Learning, 51(1), 150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holland, J. H. (2014). Complexity: A very short introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890a). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Dover.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890b). The principles of psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Kempe, V., & MacWhinney, B. (1998). The acquisition of case-marking by adult learners of Russian and German. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 20, 543587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D. (1997). Chaos/complexity science and second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 18, 141165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D., & Cameron, L. (2008). Complex systems and applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (2006). Recasts in SLA: The story so far. In Long, M. (Ed.), Problems in SLA (pp. 75–116). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & O’Grady, W. (Eds.). (2015). The handbook of language emergence. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F. (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current theory and research (pp. 6499). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Shanks, D. R. (1995). The psychology of associative learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yalçin, S., & Spada, N. (2016). Language aptitude and grammatical difficulty. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(2), 239263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeats, W. B. (1989). Yeats’s poems. London: Macmillan. (Originally published 1928).Google Scholar
Zipf, G. K. (1935). The psycho-biology of language: An introduction to dynamic philology. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar