Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-7drxs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-22T23:59:54.478Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN COGNITIVE-LINGUISTIC TASK DIFFICULTY AND L1-L2 INTERACTION FOR ACADEMIC LISTENING COMPREHENSION IN TURKISH–DUTCH EMERGENT BILINGUALS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2021

Sven Sierens*
Affiliation:
Ghent University, Belgium
Koen Van Gorp
Affiliation:
Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA and KU Leuven, Belgium
Stef Slembrouck
Affiliation:
Ghent University, Belgium
Piet Van Avermaet
Affiliation:
Ghent University, Belgium
*
*Corresponding author: Sven Sierens. E-mail: sven.sierens@ugent.be

Abstract

This study investigated the relationship between the level of cognitive-linguistic difficulty of task input and the size of the cross-linguistic relationship for academic listening comprehension in emergent bilinguals. It was theoretically motivated by task-dependent cross-linguistic interaction frameworks. We hypothesized that task item sets that involve a higher level of cognitive-linguistic difficulty, drawing on a number of sources of item difficulty, would show a smaller strength of interaction than sets involving a lower level. Using a task-based assessment instrument, listening comprehension was measured in 75 Turkish–Dutch bilingual children at first-grade entry (Mage = 6;7). Partial L1-L2 correlations indicated that cognitively more demanding item sets tended to coincide with smaller L1-L2 correlations. This finding was, in part, consistent for cognitive difficulty, yet inconclusive for linguistic difficulty. An explanation is discussed that, in line with information-processing theory, highlights a trade-off between cognitive-linguistic task demands and cross-linguistic influence.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This investigation was supported financially by the Education Department of the City of Ghent, Belgium. We wish to thank handling editor Andrea Révész, reviewer Jan Hulstijn, and two anonymous reviewers for their insightful and valuable suggestions. We are grateful to all children, parents, teachers, headmasters, and school advisers who have made this investigation possible. Special thanks go to Elke Flament, Ayşe İşçi, Tuğba Kalaz, Günnur Karataş, Liesbet Philippaert, Magda Seynaeve, Lieve Stalpaert, and Çiğdem Vandegehuchte for their participation in the test administration. We also owe thanks to Orhan Ağırdağ, Katrien Bultynck, Griet Ramaut, and Machteld Verhelst for their vital contributions. Finally, we thank Mustafa Sağlam for the translation of SALTO into Turkish and Fauve De Backer for her comments on an earlier version of this article.

References

Anderson, J. R. (2010). Cognitive psychology and its implications (7th ed.). Freeman.Google Scholar
Andringa, S., Olsthoorn, N., van Beuningen, C., Schoonden, R., & Hulstijn, J. (2012). Determinants of success in native and non-native listening comprehension: An individual differences approach. Language Learning, 62, 4978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57, 289300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomfield, A, Wayland, S. C., Rhoades, E., Blodgett, A., Linck, J., & Ross, S. (2011). What makes listening difficult? Factors affecting second language listening comprehension. University of Maryland Center for Advanced Study of Language.Google Scholar
Branum-Martin, L., Mehta, P. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Cirino, P. T., Miller, J. F., & Iglesias, A. (2009). Pictures and words: Spanish and English vocabulary in classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 897911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunfaut, T., & Révész, A. (2015). The role of task and listener characteristics in second language listening. TESOL Quarterly, 49, 141168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chung, S. C., Chen, X., & Geva, E. (2019). Deconstructing and reconstructing cross-language transfer in bilingual reading development: An interactive framework. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 50, 149161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Colpin, M., & Gysen, S. (2006). Developing and introducing task-based language tests. In Van den Branden, K. (Ed.), Task-based language education: From theory to practice (pp. 151174). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49, 222251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. In California State Department of Education, Schooling and language minority students: A theoretical framework (pp. 349). National Dissemination and Assessment Center.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (2000). Language, power and pedagogy: Bilingual children in the crossfire. Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., Oahan, D., & van Donselaar, W. (1997). Prosody in the comprehension of spoken language: A literature review. Language and Speech, 40, 141201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duran, G., & Ramaut, G. (2006). Tasks for absolute beginners and beyond: Developing and sequencing tasks at basic proficiency levels. In Van den Branden, K. (Ed.), Task-based language education: From theory to practice (pp. 4775). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durgunoğlu, A. Y. (2017). An updated review of cross-language transfer and its educational implications. In Segers, E. & van den Broek, P. (Eds.), Developmental perspectives in written language and literacy (pp. 167182). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Edele, A., & Stanat, P. (2016). The role of first-language listening comprehension in second-language reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 163180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, J. (2013). Cognitive validity. In Geranpayeh, A. & Taylor, L. (Eds.), Examining listening: Research and practice in assessing second language listening (pp. 76151). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Florit, E., Roch, M., & Levorato, C. (2013). The relationship between listening comprehension of text and sentences in preschoolers: Specific or mediated by lower and higher level components? Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 395415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F., Geva, E., Dressler, C., & Kamil, M. L. (2006). Synthesis: Cross-linguistic relationships. In August, D. & Shanahan, T. (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth (pp. 153174). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Geva, E., & Ryan, E. B. (1993). Linguistic and cognitive correlates of academic skills in first and second language. Language Learning, 43, 542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geva, E., & Siegel, S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing, 12, 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilman, R. A., & Moody, L. M. (1984). What practitioners say about listening: Research implications for the classroom. Foreign Language Annuals, 17, 331333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goh, C. C. (2000). A cognitive perspective on language learners’ listening comprehension problems. System, 28, 5575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goh, C. C., & Aryadoust, V. (2016). Learner listening: New insights and directions from empirical studies. International Journal of Listening, 30, 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodrich, J. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2017). Language-independent and language-specific aspects of early literacy: An evaluation of the common underlying proficiency model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109, 782793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gu, P. Y. (2018). Types of listening. In Hu, G. (Ed.), The TESOL encyclopedia of English language teaching: Theorizing listening. Wiley.Google Scholar
Hagen, Å. M., Knoph, R., Hjetland, H. N., Rogde, K., Lawrence, J. F., Lervåg, A., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2021). Measuring listening comprehension and predicting language development in at-risk preschoolers. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313831.2021.1939136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49, 414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, T. P., Adlof, S. M., & Alonzo, C. (2014). On the importance of listening comprehension. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16, 199207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Housen, A., & Simoens, H. (2016). Introduction: Cognitive perspectives on difficulty and complexity in L2 acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 163175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hui, B., & Godfroid, A. (2021). Testing the role of processing speed and automaticity in second language listening. Applied Psycholinguistics, 42, 10891115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhof, M. (2010). What is going on in the mind of a listener? The cognitive psychology of listening. In Wolvin, A. D. (Ed.), Listening and human communication in the 21st century (pp. 97126). Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhof, M., & Janusik, L. A. (2006). Development and validation of the Imhof-Janusik listening concepts inventory to measure listening conceptualization differences between cultures. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 35, 7998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K. M., & Godfroid, A. (2019). Should we listen or read? Modality effects in implicit and explicit knowledge. The Modern Language Journal, 103, 648664.Google Scholar
Kim, Y.-S. G., & Pilcher, H. (2016). What is listening comprehension and what does it take to improve listening comprehension? In Schiff, R. & Joshi, M. (Eds.), Handbook of interventions in learning disabilities (pp. 159174). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohnert, K. (2004). Processing skills in early sequential bilinguals. In Goldstein, B. A. (Ed.), Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish-English speakers (pp. 5376). Brookes.Google Scholar
Kuczynski, B., & Kolakowsky-Hayner, S. A. (2011). Auditory discrimination. In Kreutzer, J. S., DeLuca, J., & Caplan, B. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology. Springer.Google Scholar
Lynch, T., & Mendelsohn, D. (2020). Listening. In Schmitt, N. & Rodgers, M. P. H. (Eds.), An introduction to applied linguistics (3rd ed., pp. 223239). Routledge.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Fernald, A., & Hurtado, N. (2010). How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish-English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 37, 817840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marx, A., Heppt, B., & Henschel, S. (2017). Listening comprehension of academic and everyday language in first language and second language students. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38, 571600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Lervåg, A. (2011). Cross-linguistic transfer of oral language, decoding, phonological awareness and reading comprehension: A meta-analysis of the correlational evidence. Journal of Research in Reading, 34, 114135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ministry of the Flemish Community. (1997). Gewoon basisonderwijs: Ontwikkelingsdoelen en eindtermen. Decree 27 May 1997, Ordinance 15 July 1997.Google Scholar
National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy. National Institute for Literacy.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.). (2002). Language and literacy in bilingual children. Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., Pearson, B. Z., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (2007). Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 191230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pae, T.-I. (2018). Effects of task type and L2 proficiency on the relationship between L1 and L2 in reading and writing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40, 6390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pallotti, G. (2019). An approach to assessing the linguistic difficulty of tasks. Journal of the European Second Language Association, 3, 5870.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plonsky, L, & Oswald, F. L. (2014). How big is “big”? Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research. Language Learning, 64, 878912.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prevoo, M. J. L., Malda, M., Emmen, R. A. G., Yeniad, N., & Mesman, J. (2015). A context-dependent view on the linguistic interdependence hypothesis: Language use and SES as potential moderators. Language Learning, 65, 449469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prevoo, M. J. L., Malda, M., Mesman, J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2016). Within- and cross-language relations between oral language proficiency and school outcomes in bilingual children with an immigrant background: A meta-analytical study. Review of Educational Research, 86, 237276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, C. P., August, D., Snow, C., & Barr, C. D. (2010). The interdependence continuum: A perspective on the nature of Spanish-English bilingual reading comprehension. Bilingual Research Journal, 33, 520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, C. P., Harring, J. R., & Silverman, R. D. (2017). Linguistic interdependence between Spanish language and English language and reading: A longitudinal exploration from second through fifth grade. Bilingual Research Journal, 40, 372391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radvansky, G. A., & Ashcraft, M. H. (2016). Cognition (7th ed.). Pearson.Google Scholar
Ramaut, G., Gysen, S., & Sterckx, M. (2003). TASAN: Taalvaardigheidstoets Aanvang Secundair Onderwijs Anderstalige Nieuwkomers. Steunpunt NT2.Google Scholar
Ramaut, G., Roppe, S., Verhelst, M., & Heymans, R. (2007). SALTO screeningsinstrument aanvang lager onderwijs taalvaardigheid: Achtergronden. Centrum voor Taal en Onderwijs.Google Scholar
Révész, A., & Brunfaut, T. (2013). Text characteristic of task input and difficulty in second language listening comprehension. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 31–-65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, J. C. (2005). Second thoughts on teaching listening. RELC Journal, 36, 8592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rivera, C. (Ed.). (1984). Language proficiency and academic achievement. Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2001). Task complexity, task difficulty, and task production: Exploring interactions in a componential framework. Applied Linguistics, 22, 2757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rost, M. (2016). Teaching and researching listening (3rd ed.). Routledge.Google Scholar
Rubin, J. (1994). A review of second language listening comprehension research. The Modern Language Journal, 78, 199221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rukthong, A., & Brunfaut, T. (2020). Is anybody listening? The nature of second language listening in integrated listening-to-summarize tasks. Language Testing, 37, 3153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheele, A. F., Leseman, P. P. M., & Mayo, A. Y. (2010). The home language environment of monolingual and bilingual children and their language proficiency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 117140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N. (2010). Cognitive bases of second language fluency. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sierens, S., Slembrouck, S., Van Gorp, K., Agirdag, O., & Van Avermaet, P. (2019). Linguistic interdependence of receptive vocabulary skills in emergent bilingual preschool children: Exploring a factor-dependent approach. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40, 12691297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sierens, S., Van Gorp, K., Slembrouck, S., & Van Avermaet, P. (2021). The strength of cross-language interdependence for listening comprehension proficiency in Turkish-Dutch Emergent Bilinguals: Testing three hypotheses. Language Learning, 71, 453486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., & Kim, Y.-S. (2007). Large problem spaces: The challenge of vocabulary for English language learners. In Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannenbaum, K. R. (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 123139). Guilford.Google Scholar
Van den Branden, K., Van den Nulft, D., Verhallen, M., & Verhelst, M. (2001). Referentiekader vroege tweede taalverwerving. Nederlandse Taalunie.Google Scholar
Vandergrift, L. (2006). Second language listening: Listening ability or language proficiency? The Modern Language Journal, 90, 618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandergrift, L. (2007). Recent developments in second and foreign language listening comprehension research. Language Teaching, 40, 191210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandergrift, L., & Baker, S. (2015). Learner variables in second language listening comprehension: An exploratory path analysis. Language Learning, 65, 390416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandergrift, L., & Goh, C. C. M. (2012). Teaching and learning second language listening: Metacognition in action. Routledge.Google Scholar
van Kuyck, J. J. (2003). Taal voor kleuters voor Vlaanderen. Citogroep.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L. (2007). Early bilingualism, language transfer, and phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 425439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., Narain, G., Extra, G., Konak, Ö. A., & Zerrouk, R. (1995). Toets tweetaligheid. Cito.Google Scholar
Wallace, M. P. (2020). Individual differences in second language listening: Examining the role of knowledge, metacognitive awareness, memory, and attention. Language Learning. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Sierens et al. supplementary material

Appendix S1

Download Sierens et al. supplementary material(File)
File 574.7 KB