Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vpsfw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T09:06:34.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Elaborative Detail: Help or Hindrance to the NNS Listener?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2008

Tracey M. Derwing
Affiliation:
University of Alberta

Extract

Three experiments were conducted to examine the effects of three types of elaboration—marked paraphrase, unmarked paraphrase, and unnecessary detail—on NNS listening comprehension. Participants (both NSs and NNSs) were asked to listen to instructions and then draw or alter drawings on a page in front of them. The instructions were heard in one of four conditions, the basic set or one of the three elaboration conditions. Subsequent analyses of the participants' comprehension indicated that all listeners had significantly more difficulty following instructions in the unnecessary detail condition than in any other. Marked paraphrase was found to facilitate comprehension in some cases. It was concluded that the quality of elaboration has implications for listening comprehension, in both the classroom and the workplace.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Belfiore, M. E., & Burnaby, B. (1984). Teaching English in the workplace. Toronto: OISE Press, Hodder & Stoughton.Google Scholar
Blau, E. K. (1990). The effect of syntax, speed, and pauses on listening comprehension. TESOL Quarterly, 24, 746753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaudron, C. (1982). Vocabulary elaboration in Teachers' speech to L2 learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 4, 170180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaudron, C. (1983). Simplification of input: Topic reinstatements and their effects on L2 learners' recognition and recall. TESOL Quarterly, 17, 437458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaudron, C. (1986). The role of simplified input in classroom language. In Kasper, G. (Ed.), Learning, teaching and communication in the foreign language classroom (pp. 99110). Aarhus, Denmark: Aarhus University Press.Google Scholar
Chiang, C. S., & Dunkel, P. (1992). The effect of speech modification, prior knowledge, and listening proficiency on EFL lecture learning. TESOL Quarterly, 26, 345374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derwing, T. M. (1987). Individual differences in foreigner talk. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada.Google Scholar
Derwing, T. M. (1989). Information type and its relation to nonnative speaker comprehension. Language Learning, 39, 157172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derwing, T. M. (1990). Speech rate is no simple matter: Rate adjustment and NS-NNS communicative success. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 303313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derwing, T. M. (1991). The role of NS personality and experience in NS-NNS interaction. TESL Canada Journal, 9, 928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunkel, P. A. (1988). Academic listening and lecture notetaking for L1/L2 students: The need to investigate the utility of the axioms of good notetaking. TESL Canada Journal, 6, 1126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehrlich, S., Avery, P., & Yorio, C. (1989). Discourse structure and the negotiation of comprehensible input. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 11, 397414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaies, S. (1982). Native speaker-nonnative speaker interaction among academic peers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 5, 7481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, R. (1990). Speech rate and NNS comprehension: A preliminary study in time-benefit analysis. Language Learning, 40, 311336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelch, K. (1985). Modified input as an aid to comprehension. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 7, 8190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. H. (1983). Native speaker/nonnative speaker conversation and the negotiation of comprehensible input. Applied Linguistics, 4, 126141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. H. (1985). Input and second language acquisition theory. In Gass, S. M. & Madden, C. G. (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 377393). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (1992, 02). Input, focus on form, and second language acquisition. Plenary address, American Association of Applied Linguistics, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Parker, K., & Chaudron, C. (1987). The effects of linguistic simplification and elaborative modifications on L2 comprehension. University of Hauiai'i Working Papers in ESL, 6, 107133.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. A., & Lee, K. S. (1992). Discourse structure of direction giving: Effects of native/nonnative speaker status and gender. TESOL Quarterly, 26, 113127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pica, T. (1987). Second language acquisition, social interaction, and the classroom. Applied Linguistics, 8, 321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pica, T., & Doughty, C. (1985). Input and interaction in the communicative language classroom: A comparison of teacher-fronted and group activities. In Gass, S. M. & Madden, C. G. (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 115132). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Pica, T., & Doughty, C. (1988). Variations in classroom interaction as a function of participation pattern and task. In Fine, J. (Ed.), Second language discourse A textbook of current research (pp. 4155). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Pica, T., Holliday, L., Lewis, N., Berducci, D., & Newman, J. (1991). Language learning through interaction. What role does gender play? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 343376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pica, T., Young, R., & Doughty, C. (1987). The impact of interaction on comprehension. TESOL Quarterly, 21, 735758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plough, I., & Gass, S. (1993). Interlocutor and task familiarity: Effects on interactional structure. In Crookes, G. & Gass, S. (Eds.), Tasks and language learning: Integrating theory and practice (pp. 3556). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual matters.Google Scholar
Reder, L. M. (1982). Elaborations: When do they help and when do they hurt? Text, 2, 211224.Google Scholar
Reder, L. M., Charney, D. H., & Morgan, K. I. (1986). The role of elaborations in learning a skill from an instructional text. Memory and Cognition, 14, 6478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sauvé, V. (1982). English in the workplace: A study of six projects in Alberta and Ontario. Canada Employment and Immigration Commission. Edmonton: Queen's Printer.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrell, T. D. (1990). Foreigner talk as comprehensible input. In Alatis, J. E. (Ed.), Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics (pp. 193206). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Verplaetse, L. (1993, 04). Modifications in native speaker input: A hindrance to interaction. Paper presented at TESOL '93, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Woken, M. D. & Swales, J. (1989). Expertise and authority in native-non-native conversations: The need for a variable account. In Gass, S., Madden, C., Preston, D., & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Variation in second language acquisition (Vol. 1, pp. 211227). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual matters.Google Scholar
Yano, Y., Long, M. H., & Ross, S. (1994). The effects of simplified and elaborated texts on foreign language reading comprehension. Language Learning, 44, 189219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yule, G., & MacDonald, D. (1990). Resolving referential conflicts in L2 interaction: The effect of proficiency and interactive role. Language Learning, 40, 539556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuengler, J., & Bent, B. (1991). Relative knowledge of content domain: An influence on native-nonnative conversations. Applied Linguistics, 12, 397415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar