Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-xm8r8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-03T13:57:45.933Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dimensions of Familial Allocentrism in Brazilian Mothers from State Capitals and Small Cities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2013

Maria Lucia Seidl-de-Moura*
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro (Brazil)
Cílio Ziviani
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro (Brazil)
Ângela Donato Oliva
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro (Brazil)
Ana Carolina Fioravanti-Bastos
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro (Brazil)
Rodolfo de Castro Ribas Jr.
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro (Brazil)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Maria Lucia Seidl-de-Moura. Rua Fritz Feigl, 465. 22750-600. Rio de Janeiro (Brazil) E-mail: mlseidl@gmail.com

Abstract

This study focuses on 606 Brazilian women’s cultural models regarding their relation with their family, as evaluated by the Family Allocentrism Idiocentrism Scale (FAS). The scale was translated into Portuguese, submitted to back-translation and adapted. Analyses of the scale’s structure indicated that the best fit model involves two independent factors. Univariate GLM (General Linear Model) analyses showed that the place where mothers were raised presented a significant effect on their scores on factor 1 (normative familial allocentrism). Mothers raised on rural areas have higher scores on this factor, than the ones raised on urban areas. The opposite occurred with factor 2 (relational familial allocentrism). The set of evidences indicate that the FAS may be a bi-dimensional measure. One dimension would be part of a more stable and basic model of relation to family, constructed during development. The second dimension would be related to more recent experiences and would be more readily affected by socio-cultural context changes, including in acculturation processes.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The study was conducted with grants from the Brazilian National Research Council/CNPq. The authors are grateful to the research teams that worked in the project and to the participants.

References

Alavi, S. B., & McCormick, J. (2004). Theoretical and measurement issues for studies of collective orientation in team contexts. Small Group Research, 35, 111127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1046496403258499 Google Scholar
Andrich, D. (2010). Sufficiency and conditional estimation of person parameters in the polytomous Rasch model. Psychometrika, 75, 292308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11336-010-9154-8 Google Scholar
Andrich, D. (1988). Rasch models for measurement. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Andrich, D. (1978). Scaling attitude items constructed and scored in the Likert tradition. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 38, 665680. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001316447803800308 Google Scholar
Bond, T. G., & Fox, C. M. (2007). Applying the Rasch model: Fundamental measurement in the human sciences (2nd Ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644663. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.58.4.644 Google Scholar
Ferreira, M. C., Assmar, E. M. L., & Souto, S. (2002). O individualismo e o coletivismo como indicadores de culturas nacionais: Convergências e divergências teórico-metodológicas [Individualism and collectivism as indicators of national cultures: Theoretical and methodological convergences and divergences]. Psicologia em Estudo, 7, 8189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-73722002000100011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgas, J. (2006). Families and family change. In Georgas, J., Berry, J. W., van de Vijver, F. J. R., Kagitcibasi, C., & Poortinga, Y. H. (Eds.), Families across cultures (pp. 350). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Griep, R. H., Chor, D., Faerstein, E., Werneck, G. L., & Lopes, C. S. (2003). Apoio social: Confiabilidade teste-reteste de escala no Estudo Pró-Saúde [Social support: Scale test-retest reliability in the Pro-Health Study]. Cadernos de Saúde Pública, 19, 625634. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-311X2003000200029 Google Scholar
Harwood, R. L. (1992). The influence of culturally derived values on Anglo and Puerto Rican mothers’ perceptions of attachment behavior. Child Development, 63, 822839. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131236 Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: international differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2 nd Ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1986). Individualism-collectivism: A study of cross cultural researches. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 17, 225248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022002186017002006 Google Scholar
Hutz, C. S. (2009). Ética na avaliação psicológica [Ethics in psychological evaluation]. In C. S. Hutz (Ed.), Avanços e polêmicas em avaliação psicológica [Advances and polemics in psychological evaluation] (p. 297–310). São Paulo, Brazil: Casa do Psicólogo.Google Scholar
Kagitçibasi, Ç. (2007). Family, self, and human development across cultures: Theory and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (2007). Cultures of infancy. London, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Keller, H., Abels, M., Borke, J., Lamm, B., Su, W. L. Y., & Wang, Y. (2007). Socialization environments of Chinese and Euro-American middle-class babies: Parenting behaviors, verbal discourses and ethnotheories. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 210217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025407074633 Google Scholar
Keller, H., Lamm, B., Abels, M., Relindis, Y., Borke, J., Henning, , … Chaudhary, N. (2006). Cultural models, socialization goals and parenting ethnotheories: A multicultural analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 155172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022105284494 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirschner, B. E. (2009). The Family Allocentrism Idiocentrism Scale: Further convergent validity exploration. (Published master’s thesis. Pacific University, Oregon, OR. Retrieved from http://commons.pacificu.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1195&context=spp Google Scholar
Lay, C., Fairlie, P., Jackson, S., Ricci, T., Eisenberg, J., Sato, , … Melamud, A. (1998). Domain-specific allocentrism-idiocentrism: A measure of family connectedness. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 434460. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022198293004 Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.128.1.3 Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311 Google Scholar
Raykov, T., & Marcoulides, G. A. (2008). An introduction to applied multivariate analysis. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Sato, T. (2007). The family allocentrism-idiocentrism scale: Convergent validity and construct exploration. Individual Differences Research, 5, 194200.Google Scholar
Seidl-de-Moura, M. L, Lordelo, E., Vieira, M. L., Piccinnini, C. A., Siqueira, J. E O., Magalhães, , ... Rimoli, A. (2008). Brazilian mothers’ socialization goals: Intracultural differences in seven cities. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32, 465472. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025408093666 Google Scholar
Seidl-de-Moura, M. L., Targino, T., Nahum, K., Da Cruz, E. M., Amaral, G., & De Marca, R. (2009). Parenting cultural models of a group of mothers from Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12, 506517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 580591. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205014 Google Scholar
Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D. P. S., & Gelfand, M. J. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research, 29, 240275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/106939719502900302 Google Scholar
Simmel, G. (1973). A metrópole e a vida mental [The metropolis and mental life]. In Velho, O. G. (Ed.), O fenômeno urbano [The urban phenomenon] . Rio de Janeiro, Brazil: Zahar.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. New York, NY: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing social contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.96.3.506 Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (2004). Collectivism: Cultural concerns. In Smelser, N. J. & Baltes, P. B. B. (Eds.), International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (pp. 22272232). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (2002). Odysseus wandered for 10, I wondered for 50 years. In Lonner, W. J., Dinnel, D. L., Hayes, S. A., & Sattler, D. N. (Eds.), Online readings in psychology and culture (Unit 2, Chapter 1) . Bellingham, WA: Center for Cross-Cultural Research, Western Washington University. Retrieved from http://www.wwu.edu/culture/triandis2.htm Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (2009). Ecological determinants of cultural variations. In Wyer, R. W., Chiu, C.y, & Hong, Y.y (Eds.), Understanding culture: Theory, research and applications (pp. 189210). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M. J., Asai, M., & Lucca, N. (1988). Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspectives on self-in-group relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 323338. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.323 Google Scholar
Wright, B. D., & Stone, M. H. (2004). Making measures. Chicago, IL: Phaneron Press.Google Scholar
Vieira, M. L., Seidl-de-Moura, M. L., Mafioletti, S. M., Martins, G. D. F., Tokumaru, R. S., Lordelo, E., & Oliva, A. D. (2010). Autonomy and interdependency: Beliefs of Brazilian mothers from State capitals and small towns. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13, 816824.Google Scholar