Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-fv566 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-21T06:55:19.876Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developmental and Gender Differences in the Production of the Past Tense in Spanish Children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 May 2018

Alberto Domínguez*
Affiliation:
Universidad de La Laguna (Spain)
Cristina Izura
Affiliation:
Swansea University (UK)
Ana Cecilia Medina
Affiliation:
Universidad de La Laguna (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alberto Domínguez. Facultad de Psicología de la Universidad de La Laguna. Campus de Guajara, La Laguna. 38200 Santa Cruz de Tenerife (Spain). E-mail: adomin@ull.es

Abstract

This study investigates gender differences in children’s linguistic development of Spanish past tense verbs. Two groups of 30 children, each consisting of 15 girls and 15 boys, were studied: Preschool children (5 years old) and 1st grade (7 years old). Participants carried out an elicitation task where a verbal change from present to past tense was required. Frequency and regularity variables were introduced as experimental factors to study differences between gender and age. The results showed that morphological rules were more successfully applied in 1st grade than in preschool children. Youngest participants showed a greatest influence of lexical variables, such as verb frequency and produced more omission and repetition errors. Gender differences only appeared in the oldest children, with girls being more affected by verb frequency and regularity. Girls also demonstrated a broader vocabulary than boys as shown by the qualitative analyses.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

We would like sincerely to thank all those who took part in the collection of data. We also thank Clive Tyrell for editing and reviewing this manuscript for English language. This study was supported by PSI2013-47959-P grant from the Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad of Spanish government.

How to cite this article:

Domínguez, A., Izura, C., & Medina, A. C. (2018). Developmental and gender differences in the production of the past tense in Spanish children. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 21. e13. Doi:10.1017/sjp.2018.15

References

Aguado-Orea, J., & Pine, J. M. (2015). Comparing different models of the development of verb inflection in early child Spanish. PLoS ONE 10(3), e0119613. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alameda, J. R., & Cuetos, F. (1995). Diccionario de frecuencias de las unidades lingüísticas del español [Frequency dictionary of linguistic units of Spanish]. Oviedo, Spain: Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Oviedo.Google Scholar
Aveledo, F. (2006). El procesamiento de verbos regulares e irregulares en el español infantil: Mecanismo dual vs. conexionismo [The processing of regular and irregular verbs children’ Spanish: dual mechanism vs. connectionism]. Boletín de Lingüística, 18(26), 532.Google Scholar
Balota, D. A., & Ferraro, F. R. (1993). A dissociation of frequency and regularity effects in pronunciation performance across young-adults, older adults, and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer-type. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(5), 573592. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1993.1029CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbarotto, R., Laiacona, M., & Capitani, E. (2008). Does sex influence the age of acquisition of common names? A contrast of different semantic categories. Cortex, 44 (9), 11611170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.016Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C. S., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). Specific and general language performance across early childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First Language, 24(3), 267304. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723704045681Google Scholar
Bouchard, C., Trudeau, N., Sutton, A., Boudreault, M., & Deneault, J. (2009). Gender differences in language development in French Canadian children between 8 and 30 months of age. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(4), 685707. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716409990075Google Scholar
Burman, D. D., Bitan, T., & Booth, J. R. (2008). Sex differences in neural processing of language among children. Neuropsychologia, 46, 13491362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.021Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Aveledo, F., & Roca, I. (2002): The development of regular and irregular verb inflection in Spanish child language”. Journal of Child Language, 29, 591622. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000902005172Google Scholar
Coates, J. (1993) Women, men and language. A sociolinguistic account of gender differences in language. Routledge, NY: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Cuetos-Vega, F., Domínguez, A., Baauw, S., & Berthier, M. L. (2007). Disociación entre pacientes agramáticos y anómicos en la producción de formas verbales [Dissociation between agrammatic and anomic patients in their production of verbal forms]. Revista de Neurología, 44(4), 203208.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Schmidt, R. (1998). Rules or associations in the acquisition of morphology? The frequency by regularity interaction in human and PDP learning of morphosyntax. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13(2–3), 307336. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909698386546CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Perez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., … Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 326343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.xGoogle Scholar
Feldman, H. M., Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Kurs-Lasky, M., Janosky, J. E., & Paradise, J. L. (2000). Measurement properties of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories at ages one and two years. Child Development, 71(2), 310322.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, S. J., … Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1185. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166093CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P., Reznick, S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Fiestas, C. E., & Peña, E. D. (2004). Narrative discourse in bilingual children: Language and task effects. Journal of Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 35(2), 155168. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2004/016)Google Scholar
Galsworthy, M. J., Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science. 3, 206215. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazella, J., & Stockman, I. J. (2003). Children’s story retelling under different modality and task conditions: Implications for standardizing language sampling procedures. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(1), 6172. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2003/053)Google Scholar
Halpern, D. F., & LaMay, M. L. (2000). The smarter sex: A critical review of sex differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 12(2), 229246. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009027516424Google Scholar
Hartshorne, J. K., & Ullman, M. T. (2006). Why girls say ‘holded’ more than boys. Developmental Science, 9, 2132. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00459.xGoogle Scholar
Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104(1), 5369. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.104.1.53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., & Maldonado, R. (2017). Grammaticality differences between Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment and their typically developing peers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 52(6), 669869. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12312Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Yoo, J. (2011). Rehearsal effects in adult word learning. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 121148. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.486579Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., Gross, M., & Buac, M. (2013). Gender differences in child word learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 8289. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.lindif.2013.07.002Google Scholar
Kidd, E., & Lum, J. A. G. (2008). Sex differences in past tense overregularization. Developmental Science, 11(6), 882889. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00744.xGoogle Scholar
Kimura, D., & Clarke, P. G. (2002). Women’s advantage on verbal memory is not restricted to concrete words. Psychological Reports, 91(3), 11371142. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2002.91.3f.1137Google Scholar
Kramer, J. H., Delis, D. C., & Daniel, M. (1988). Sex differences in verbal learning. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44: 907-915. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198811)44:6<907::AID-JCLP2270440610>3.0.CO;2–8Google Scholar
López-Ornat, S., Fernández, A., Gallo, P., & Mariscal, S. (1994). La adquisición de la lengua española [The acquisition of Spanish]. Madrid, Spain: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., Xu, F., & Clahsen, H. (1992). Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57(4), I-178. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166115Google Scholar
Martin, N. (2016). Disorders of word production. In Papathanasiou, I. & Coppens, P. (Eds.) Aphasia and related neurogenic communication disorders (pp. 169195). Burlington, MA: Jones & Bartlett Learning.Google Scholar
Maslen, R. J. C., Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). A dense corpus study of past tense and plural overregularization in English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 13191333. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/099)Google Scholar
Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1993). From rote learning to system building: Acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition, 48, 2169. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90057-3Google Scholar
Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (1997). How complex simplex words can be. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(1), 118139. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2510Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., & Bruck, M. (1990). Consistency effects in the generation of past tense morphology. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.01.001Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 156163. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01462-5Google Scholar
Tombaugha, T. N., Kozakb, J., & Reesc, L. (1999). Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(2), 167177. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00095-4Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2001). A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 717726. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094573Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T., Estabrooke, I. V., Steinhauer, K., Brovetto, C., Pancheva, R., & Ozawa, K., (2002). Sex differences in the neurocognition of language. Brain and Language, 83, 141143.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T., Miranda, R. A., & Travers, M. L. (2008). Sex differences in the neurocognition of language (pp. 291309). In Becker, J. B., Berkley, K. J., Geary, N., Hampson, E., Herman, J., & Young, E. (Eds.) Sex on the brain: From genes to behavior. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Weiss, E. M., Kemmler, G., Deisenhammer, E. A., Fleischhacker, W. W., & Delazer, M. (2003). Sex differences in cognitive functions. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(4), 863875. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00288-XGoogle Scholar