Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-24hb2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T11:28:28.378Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Model for the Division of Semiotic Labor in Scientific Argument: The Interaction of Words and Images

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 November 2011

Alan G. Gross*
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota

Argument

A growing cross-disciplinary literature has acknowledged the importance of verbal-visual interaction in the creation and communication of scientific texts. I contend that the proper understanding of these texts must flow from a hermeneutic model that takes verbal-visual interaction seriously, one that is firmly grounded in cognitive constraints and affordances. The model I propose has two modules, one for perception, derived from Gestalt psychology, the other for cognition, derived from Peirce's semiotics. I apply this model to an important but largely neglected text in the history of nineteenth-century science, Charles Lyell's The Geological Evidences of the Antiquity of Man, and to two accounts by well-respected historians of science, both of the same key discovery in quantum physics. In doing so, I hope to demonstrate the advisability of incorporating the exegetical practices my model entails into the everyday practices of historians of science.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aubert, J. J. et al. 1974. “Experimental Observation of a Heavy Particle J.” Physical Review Letters 33: 1404–06.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Austin, John L. 1975. How to Do Things with Words, 2nd ed. Edited by Ormson, J. O. and Sbisà, Marina. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bahain, J.-J, Falguères, C., Laurent, M., Voinchet, P., Dolo, J-M, Antoine, P., and Tuffreau, A.. 2005. “ESR Chronology of the Somme River Terrace System and First Human Settlements in Northern France.” Quaternary Geology 2:356–62.Google Scholar
Baigrie, Brian S., ed. 1996. Picturing Knowledge: Historical and Philosophical Problems Concerning the Use of Art in Science. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boylan, Patrick J. 1998. “Lyell and the Dilemma of Quarterary Glaciations.” In Lyell: The Past is Key to the Present. Geological Society of London, Special Publications, edited by Dereck John Bundell and Andrew C. Scott 143:145–59.Google Scholar
Bultmann, Rudolph. 1965. Glauben und Verstehen: Gesamelte Aufsätze, vol. 4. Tübingen: J.C.B. Mohr.Google Scholar
Bynum, William F. 1984. “Charles Lyell's Antiquity of Man and its Critics.” Journal of the History of Biology 17:153–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandrasekaran, B. and Narayanan, N. H.. 1993. “Perceptual Representation and Reasoning.” In Intelligent Systems: Concepts and Applications, edited by Sterling, Leon S., 6776. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, James M. and Paivio, A.. 1991. “Dual Coding and Education.” Educational Psychology Review 3:149210.Google Scholar
Cohen, Claudine. 1998. “Charles Lyell and the Evidences of the Antiquity of Man.” In Lyell: The Past is Key to the Present. Geological Society of London, Special Publications 143:8393.Google Scholar
Cohen, Morris R. and Nagel, Ernest. 1934. An Introduction to Logic and Scientific Method. New York: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
Darwin Correspondence Project. <www.Darwinproject.ac.uk.> (Last accessed November 1, 2009).+(Last+accessed+November+1,+2009).>Google Scholar
Daston, Lorraine and Galison, Peter. 2007. Objectivity. New York: Zone Books.Google Scholar
Dawkins, William B. 1874. Cave Hunting, Researches on the Evidence of Caves Respecting the Early Inhabitants of Europe. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Dawkins, William B. 1880. Early Man in Britain and His Place in the Tertiary Period. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Deledalle, Gérard. 2002. Charles S. Peirce's Philosophy of Signs: Essays in Comparative Literature. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Deutsch, Martin. 1981. “Imagery and Inference in Physical Research.” In On Scientific Thinking, edited by Tweney, Ryan D., Doherty, Michael, and Mynatt, Clifford R., 354–60. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, John. 1872. The Ancient Stone Implements, Weapons, and Ornaments of Great Britain. New York: D. Appleton.Google Scholar
Galison, Peter. 1997. Image and Logic: A Material Culture of Microphysics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gamble, Clive. 2008. “Breaking the Time Barrier.” Geoscientist 18:2225.Google Scholar
Gamble, Clive. 2009. Personal communication, January 11, 2009.Google Scholar
Geikie, James. 1914. The Antiquity of Man in Europe, Being the Munro lectures, 1913. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.Google Scholar
Geikie, James. 1877. The Great Ice Age and Its Relation to the Antiquity of Man. New York: D. Appleton.Google Scholar
Geikie, James. 1881. Prehistoric Europe: A Geological Sketch. London: E. Stanford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giere, Ronald N. 1996. “Visual Models and Scientific Judgment.” In Picturing Knowledge: Historical and Philosophical Problems Concerning the Use of Art in Science, edited by Baigre, Brian S.. 269302. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grayson, Donald K. 1983. The Establishment of Human Antiquity. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hochberg, Julian. 1998. “Gestalt Theory and its Legacy: Organization in Eye and Brain, in Attention and Mental Representation.” In Perception and Cognition at Century's End, edited by Hochberg, Julian, 253306. San Diego: Academy Press.Google Scholar
Hüppauf, Bernd-Rüdiger. and Peter, Weingart, eds. 2008. Science Images and Popular Images of the Sciences. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ivins, William M. Jr. 1969. Prints and Visual Communication. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kindfield, A. C. H. 1992. “Expert Diagrammatic Reasoning in Biology.” In Reasoning with Diagrammatic Representations, edited by Chandrasekaran, B. and Simon, Herbert, 4146. Menlo Park: AAAI Press.Google Scholar
Kirby, K. N. and Kosslyn, S. M.. 1992. “Thinking Visually.” In Understanding Vision: An Interdisciplinary Perspective, edited by Humphreys, G. W., 7186. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Köhler, Wolfgang. 1947. Gestalt Psychology: An Introduction to New Concepts in Modern Psychology. New York: New American Library.Google Scholar
Larkin, Jill H. and Simon, Herbert A.. 1987. “Why a Diagram Is (Sometimes) Worth Ten Thousand Words.” Cognitive Science 11:6599.Google Scholar
Lubbock, John. 1875. Pre-historic Times, As Illustrated by Ancient Remains, and the Manner and Customs of Modern Savages. 2nd ed. New York: D. Appleton.Google Scholar
Lyell, Charles. 1863. The Geological Evidences of the Antiquity of Man with Remarks on Theories of the Origin of Species by Variation. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Lyell, Charles. 1873. The Geological Evidences of the Antiquity of Man with an Outline of Glacial and Post-Tertiary Geology and Remarks on the Origin of Species with Special Reference to Man's First Appearance on the Earth. 4th ed. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Lyell, Katherine. 1881. Life, Letters, and Journals of Sir Charles Lyell. 2 vols. London: Murray.Google Scholar
Lynch, Martin and Woolgar, Steve, eds. 1990. Representation in Scientific Practice. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
O'Connor, Ralph. 2007. The Earth on Show: Fossils and the Poetics of Popular Science, 1802–1856. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Oestermeier, U., and Hesse, F. W.. 2000. “Verbal and Visual Causal Arguments.” Cognition 75:65104.Google Scholar
Paivio, Allan. 2007. Mind and its Evolution: A Dual Coding Theoretical Approach. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pauwels, L.uc. ed. 2006. Visual Cultures of Science: Rethinking Representational Practices in Knowledge Building and Scientific Communication. Hanover NH: Dartmouth College Press.Google Scholar
Perini, Laura. 2005a. “The Truth in Pictures.” Philosophy of Science 72:262–85.Google Scholar
Perini, Laura. 2005b. “Visual Representations and Confirmation.” Philosophy of Science 72:913–26.Google Scholar
Pickering, Andrew. 1984. Constructing Quarks: A Sociological History of Particle Physics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1983. Pattern Perception and the Comprehension of Graphs. (Eric Document Service No. ED 1.310/2.237339).Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1990. “A Theory of Graph Comprehension.” In Artificial Intelligence and the Future of Testing, edited by Freedle, R., 73126. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, Paul. 1965. De l'interprétation: Essai sur Freud. Paris: Éditions de Seuil.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, Paul. 1970. Freud and Philosophy: An Essay in Interpretation. Translated by Savage, D.. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, Paul. 1981. “Hermeneutics and the Critique of Ideology.” In Hermeneutics and the Human Sciences, edited and translated by Thompson, John B., 63100. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, Paul. 1973. “Herméneutique et critique des ideologies.” In Démythisation et Idéologie, edited by Castelli, Enrico, 2564. Paris: Aubier Éditions Montaigne.Google Scholar
Rudwick, Martin. 1976. “The Emergence of a Visual Language for Geological Science 1760–1840.” History of Science 14:149–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudwick, Martin. J. S. 2005. Bursting the Limits of Time: The Reconstructiion of Geohistory in the Age of Revolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryle, Gilbert. 1949. The Concept of Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Saussure, Ferdinand de. [1986] 1916. Course in General Linguistics, edited by Bally, C., Sechehasye, A. and Riedlinger, A.; translated by Harris, R.. La Salle IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
Schleiermacher, Friedrich. 1977. Hermeneutics: The Handwritten Manuscripts. Edited by Kimmerle, H., translated by Duke, J. and Forstman, J.. Missoula: Scholars Press.Google Scholar
Scheiermacher, Friedrich. 1959. Hermeneutik: Nach den Handschriften neu herausgeben und eingeleitet. Edited by Kimmerle, H.. Heidelberg: Carl Winter Universitätsverlag.Google Scholar
Sebeok, Thomas A. 1976. “Iconicity.” Modern Language Notes 9:1427–56.Google Scholar
Sellars, Wilfrid. 1991. Science, Perception and Reality. Atascadero CA: Ridgeview Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Sommer, Marianne. 2007. Bones and Ochre: The Curious Afterlife of the Red Lady of Paviland. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ting, Samuel C. C. 1976. “The Discovery of the J Particle: A Personal Recollection.” Nobel Lecture. Accessed 2 February 2011.Google Scholar
Ting, Samuel, Goldhaber, Gerson, and Richter, Burton. 1976. “Discovery of Neutral Massive Vector Mesons.” Adventures in Experimental Physics 5:114–49.Google Scholar
Van Riper, A. Bowdoin. 1993. Men Among the Mammoths: Victorian Science and the Discovery of Human Prehistory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Voss, Julia. 2010. Darwin's Pictures: Views of Evolutionary Theory: 1837–1874. Translated by Lantz, LoriNew Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Wallace, Alfred R. 1873. “Review of Lyell's Antiquity of Man.” Nature (2 October):462–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westropp, Hodder M. 1872. Pre-Historic Phases, or Introductory Essay on Pre-Historic Archaeology. London: Bell and Daldy.Google Scholar