Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T15:09:32.678Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Using Hidden Markov Models to Uncover Underlying States in Neuroimaging Data for a Design Ideation Task

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 July 2019

Kosa Goucher-Lambert*
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley;
Christopher McComb
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University
*
Contact: Goucher-Lambert, Kosa, University of California, Berkeley, Mechanical Engineering, United States of America, kosa@berkeley.edu

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Recently, design researchers have begun to use neuroimaging methods (e.g., functional magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI) to understand a variety of cognitive processes relevant to design. However, common neuroimaging analysis techniques require significant assumptions relating temporal and spatial information during model formulation. In this work, we apply hidden Markov Models (HMM) in order to uncover patterns of brain activation in a design-relevant fMRI dataset. The underlying fMRI data comes from a prior research study in which participants generated solutions for twelve open-ended design problems from the literature. HMMs are generative models that are able to automatically infer the internal state characteristics of a process by observing state emissions. In this work, we demonstrate that distinct states can be extracted from the design ideation fMRI dataset, and that designers are likely to transition between a few key states. Additionally, the likelihood of occupancy within these states is different for high and low performing designers. This work opens up the door for future research to investigate the patterns of neural activation within the discovered states.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019

References

Alexiou, K., Zamenopoulos, T., Johnson, J.H. and Gilbert, S.J. (2009), “Exploring the neurological basis of design cognition using brain imaging: some preliminary results”, Design Studies, Elsevier Ltd, Vol. 30 No. 6, pp. 623647.10.1016/j.destud.2009.05.002Google Scholar
Anderson, J.R. (2012), “Tracking problem solving by multivariate pattern analysis and Hidden Markov Model algorithms”, Neuropsychologia, Vol. 50 No. 4, pp. 487498.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.025Google Scholar
Anderson, J.R., Betts, S., Ferris, J.L. and Fincham, J.M. (2010), “Neural imaging to track mental states while using an intelligent tutoring system”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 107 No. 15, pp. 70187023.10.1073/pnas.1000942107Google Scholar
Anderson, J.R., Pyke, A.A. and Fincham, J.M. (2016), “Hidden Stages of Cognition Revealed in Patterns of Brain Activation”, Psychological Science, Vol. 27 No. 9, pp. 12151226.10.1177/0956797616654912Google Scholar
Atman, C.J., Adams, R.S., Cardella, M.E., Turns, J., Mosborg, S. and Saleem, J. (2007), “Engineering Design Processes: A Comparison of Students and Expert Practitioners”, Journal of Engineering Education, Vol. 96 No. 4, pp. 359379.Google Scholar
Baldassano, C., Chen, J., Zadbood, A., Pillow, J.W. and Norman, K.A. (2018), “Discovering event structure in continuous narrative perception and memory”, Neuron, Vol. 95 No. 3, pp. 709721.10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.041Google Scholar
Baum, L.E. (1972), “An equality and associated maximization technique in statistical estimation for probabilistic functions of Markov processes”, Inequalities, Vol. 3, pp. 18.Google Scholar
Baum, L.E. and Eagon, J.A. (1967), “An inequality with applications to statistical estimation for probabilistic functions of Markov processes and to a model for ecology”, Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society, Vol. 73 No. 3, pp. 360364.Google Scholar
Baum, L.E. and Petrie, T. (1966), “Statistical Inference for Probabilistic Functions of Finite State Markov Chains”, The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, Vol. 37 No. 6, pp. 15541563.Google Scholar
Baum, L.E., Petrie, T., Soules, G. and Weiss, N. (1970), “A Maximization Technique Occurring in the Statistical Analysis of Probabilistic Functions of Markov Chains”, The Annals of Mathematical Statistics, Vol. 41 No. 1, pp. 164171.Google Scholar
Baum, L.E. and Sell, G. (1968), “Growth transformations for functions on manifolds”, Pacific Journal of Mathematics, Vol. 27 No. 2, pp. 211227.Google Scholar
Clegg, B.A.B.A., Digirolamo, G.J. and Keele, S.W. (1998), “Sequence learning”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 2 No. 8, pp. 275281.10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01202-9Google Scholar
Cox, R.W. (1996), “AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages”, Computers and Biomedical Research, an International Journal, Vol. 29 No. 3, pp. 162173.10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014Google Scholar
Curran, T. and Keele, S.W. (1993), “Attentional and Nonattentional Forms of Sequence Learning”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.Google Scholar
Goldschmidt, G. and Rodgers, P.A. (2013), “The design thinking approaches of three different groups of designers based on self-reports”, Design Studies, Elsevier Ltd, Vol. 34 No. 4, pp. 454471.Google Scholar
Goucher-Lambert, K. and Cagan, J. (2019), “Crowdsourcing Inspiration: Using crowd generated inspirational stimuli to support designer ideation”, Design Studies, http://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2019.01.001Google Scholar
Goucher-Lambert, K., Moss, J. and Cagan, J. (2017), “Inside the Mind: Using Neuroimaging to Understand Moral Product Preference Judgments Involving Sustainability (IDETC2016-59406)”, ASME Journal of Mechanical Design, Vol. 139 No. 4, pp. 112.Google Scholar
Goucher-Lambert, K., Moss, J. and Cagan, J. (2018), “A neuroimaging investigation of design ideation with and without inspirational stimuli—understanding the meaning of near and far stimuli”, Design Studies, available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2018.07.001Google Scholar
Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E.J., Woolrich, M.W. and Smith, S.M. (2012), “FSL”, NeuroImage.Google Scholar
McComb, C., Cagan, J. and Kotovsky, K. (2016), “Utilizing Markov Chains to Understand Operation Sequencing in Design Tasks”, in Gero, J.S. (Ed.), Design Computing and Cognition ‘16, Springer International Publishing, Cham, Switzerland, pp. 401418.Google Scholar
McComb, C., Cagan, J. and Kotovsky, K. (2017a), “Mining Process Heuristics From Designer Action Data via Hidden Markov Models”, Journal of Mechanical Design, Vol. 139 No. 11, p. 111412.10.1115/1.4037308Google Scholar
McComb, C., Cagan, J. and Kotovsky, K. (2017b), “Data on the configuration design of internet-connected home cooling systems by engineering students”, Data in Brief, Vol. 14, pp. 773776.Google Scholar
McComb, C., Cagan, J. and Kotovsky, K. (2017c), “Capturing Human Sequence-Learning Abilities in Configuration Design Tasks Through Markov Chains”, Journal of Mechanical Design, Vol. 139 No. 9, p. 091101.10.1115/1.4037185Google Scholar
McComb, C., Cagan, J. and Kotovsky, K. (2018), “Data on the design of truss structures by teams of engineering students”, Data in Brief, Vol. 18, available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dib.2018.02.078.Google Scholar
Meier, C., Yassine, A.A. and Browning, T.R. (2007), “Design Process Sequencing With Competent Genetic Algorithms”, Journal of Mechanical Design, Vol. 129 No. 6, pp. 566585.Google Scholar
Nissen, M.J. and Bullemer, P. (1987), “Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures”, Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 132.10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8Google Scholar
Perruchet, P. and Amorim, M.-A. (1992), “Conscious Knowledge and Changes in Perfomance in Sequence Learning: Evidence Against Dissociation”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Vol. 18 No. 4, pp. 785800.Google Scholar
Radcliffe, D.F. and Lee, T.Y. (1989), “Design methods used by undergraduate engineering students”, Design Studies, Vol. 10 No. 4, pp. 199207.Google Scholar
Raina, A., McComb, C. and Cagan, J. (2018), “Design Strategy Transfer in Cognitively-Inspired Agents”, ASME IDETC - Design Automation Conference, pp. 110.Google Scholar
Reed, J. and Johnson, P. (1994), “Assessing implicit learning with indirect tests: Determining what is learned about sequence structure”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Vol. 20 No. 3, pp. 585594.Google Scholar
Rogers, J. (1996), “DeMAID/GA - An enhanced design manager's aid for intelligent decomposition”, 6th Symposium on Multidisciplinary Analysis and Optimization, American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics, available at: http://doi.org/10.2514/6.1996-4157Google Scholar
Sen, C., Ameri, F. and Summers, J.D. (2010), “An Entropic Method for Sequencing Discrete Design Decisions”, Journal of Mechanical Design, Vol. 132 No. 10, p. 101004.Google Scholar
Stempfle, J. and Badke-schaub, P. (2002), “Thinking in design teams - an analysis of team communication”, Design Studies, Vol. 23 No. 5, pp. 473496.Google Scholar
Sylcott, B., Cagan, J. and Tabibnia, G. (2013), “Understanding Consumer Tradeoffs Between Form and Function Through Metaconjoint and Cognitive Neuroscience Analyses”, Journal of Mechanical Design, Vol. 135 No. 10, p. 101002.Google Scholar
Todd, D. (1997), “Multiple Criteria Genetic Algorithms in Engineering Design and Operation”, University of Newcastle.Google Scholar
Vidaurre, D., Abeysuriya, R., Becker, R., Quinn, A.J., Alfaro-Almagro, F., Smith, S.M. and Woolrich, M.W. (2018), “Discovering dynamic brain networks from big data in rest and task”, NeuroImage, Vol. 180, pp. 646656.Google Scholar
Vidaurre, D., Smith, S.M. and Woolrich, M.W. (2017), “Brain network dynamics are hierarchically organized in time”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 114 No. 48, pp. 1282712832.Google Scholar
Waldron, M.B. and Waldron, K.J. (1988), “A time sequence study of a complex mechanical system design”, Design Studies, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 95106.Google Scholar
Willingham, D.B., Nissen, M.J.J. and Bullemer, P. (1989), “On the development of procedural knowledge”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Vol. 15 No. 6, pp. 10471060.Google Scholar