Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T16:08:48.047Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Connecting quantitatively derived personality–psychopathology models and neuroscience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 October 2021

Robert D. Latzman*
Affiliation:
Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, USA
Robert F. Krueger
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Colin G. DeYoung
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Giorgia Michelini
Affiliation:
UCLA Semel Institute for Neuroscience & Human Behavior, Los Angeles, CA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Robert D. Latzman, Email: rlatzman@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Traditionally, personality has been conceptualized in terms of dimensions of human experience – habitual ways of thinking, feeling, and behaving. By contrast, psychopathology has traditionally been conceptualized in terms of categories of disorder – disordered thinking, feeling, and behaving. The empirical literature, however, routinely shows that psychopathology does not coalesce into readily distinguishable categories. Indeed, psychopathology tends to delineate dimensions that are relatively similar to dimensions of personality. In this special issue of Personality Neuroscience, authors took up the challenge of reconceptualizing personality and psychopathology in terms of connected and interrelated dimensions, and they considered the utility of pursuing neuroscientific inquiry from this more integrative perspective. In this editorial article, we provide the relevant background to the interface between personality, psychopathology, and neuroscience; summarize contributions to the special issue; and point toward directions for continued research and refinement. All told, it is evident that quantitatively derived, integrative models of personality–psychopathology represent a particularly promising conduit for advancing our understanding of the neurobiological foundation of human experience, both functional and dysfunctional.

Type
Review Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Come gather 'round people

Wherever you roam

And admit that the waters

Around you have grown

And accept it that soon

You’ll be drenched to the bone

If your time to you is worth savin'

And you better start swimmin'

Or you’ll sink like a stone

For the times they are a-changin'

– Bob Dylan, The Times They Are A-Changin’

Clinical neuroscience aims to elucidate neural correlates of mental illness and translate that knowledge into effective biologically informed interventions. There has been a growing consensus, however, that the lack of progress to date may be a result of the well-documented shortcomings of the categorical diagnostic system (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Cicero, Conway, DeYoung and Wright2021; Latzman et al., Reference Latzman and DeYoung2020) – the categorical system is scientifically untenable. A large and reliable empirical literature has demonstrated the superiority of quantitatively derived dimensional models of classification, including, for example, the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Kotov, Watson, Forbes, Eaton, Ruggero and Zimmermann2018).

Importantly, it has become clear that these quantitatively derived models of psychopathology converge with quantitative models of personality, typically reflecting similar latent dimensions in both the general and maladaptive ranges (Markon, Krueger, & Watson, Reference Markon, Krueger and Watson2005; Widiger & Trull, Reference Widiger and Trull2007; Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov and Wright2019). The field has come to realize that the convergence of dimensional models of personality and psychopathology represents a promising phenotypic target for neurobiological investigations of psychopathology and related processes. The series of articles included in this special issue of Personality Neuroscience provide a set of exemplars of sophisticated novel research aimed at elucidating neurobiological correlates of broad, transdiagnostic processes within an integrated personality–psychopathology framework.

After outlining the state of the science with regard to dimensional models, integration of personality and psychopathology into quantitative hierarchical models, and links between personality–psychopathology models and large-scale research initiatives, we highlight the contribution to the field of the articles in this special issue. Finally, we conclude with recommendations for researchers interested in carrying out integrative dimensionally based neuroscience research spanning the personality–psychopathology continuum. We hope that the articles in this special issue, and our introductory remarks, will spark excitement for this integrative research approach and highlight its value for elucidating the neurobiological underpinnings of personality and psychopathology and for informing future applied applications.

1. Dimensional models

Problems with categorical approaches to psychopathology have been well documented and are generally well known in the field (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2015; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Kotov, Watson, Forbes, Eaton, Ruggero and Zimmermann2018). Critical limitations with regard to categorical diagnoses include arbitrary thresholds for meeting criteria for a diagnosis, excessive comorbidity among putatively separate diagnoses, extensive within-category heterogeneity, relatively low-diagnostic stability, and limited treatment utility (for comprehensive reviews, see Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017, Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Cicero, Conway, DeYoung and Wright2021). All told, it has become abundantly clear that conceptualizing phenotypic psychopathology categorically is untenable. Extensive quantitative evidence supports continuous models of psychopathology, when categorical and continuous models are directly compared (Haslam et al., Reference Haslam, McGrath, Viechtbauer and Kuppens2020; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Kotov, Watson, Forbes, Eaton, Ruggero and Zimmermann2018). Categorical psychopathology diagnoses are thus problematic targets for neuroscientific investigations.

Dimensional models of psychopathology offer a promising strategy for overcoming the limitations posed by categorical diagnoses and represent evidence-based targets for neuroscientific research. This is recognized via a number of recent initiatives broadly relevant to personality neuroscience. For example, the HiTOP consortium seeks to organize the signs and symptoms of psychopathology using empirically based approaches (https://renaissance.stonybrookmedicine.edu/HITOP). The basic thrust of the consortium is to work with data from traditional assessment modalities (e.g., interviews and questionnaires) to articulate a model of psychopathology that reflects the empirical structure of these data. Thus, for example, HiTOP recognizes the empirical organization of psychopathology into broad groupings such as the emotional dysfunction, psychosis, and externalizing super-spectra (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Jonas, Carpenter, Dretsch, Eaton, Forbes and Watson2020; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Hobbs, Conway, Dick, Dretsch and Eaton2020; Watson et al., Reference Watson, Levin-Aspenson, Waszczuk, Conway, Dalgleish and Dretschin press). In addition, the U.S. National Institute of Mental Health has developed the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2015; Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010), which seeks to encourage research directed at six-dimensional domains of human functioning organized around putative systems underlying human capacities: negative valence systems, positive valence systems, cognitive systems, systems for social processes, arousal and regulatory systems, and sensorimotor systems.

HiTOP and RDoC have specific similarities and differences in strategy and approach to psychopathology research, as described in more detail elsewhere (Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman, & Kotov, Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021) and below. An important point from our perspective, though, is that these major efforts stand in stark contrast to the idea of organizing research by traditional psychiatric categories but are well aligned with a long tradition of dimensional models in personality research. For example, the domains of normative personality variation are generally aligned with the domains emerging from the HiTOP and RDoC approaches (see Figure 1). In traditional personality research, a consensus taxonomy promotes coherent communication among diverse coworkers in the field, facilitating intellectual commerce and a cumulative literature. Efforts in the psychopathology literature such as HiTOP and RDoC have a similar basic thrust – to create a consensus taxonomy of fundamental organizing constructs that is better tethered to the empirical literature, by contrast to traditional psychiatric categories. These are changing times in psychopathology research, and there is a good deal about which to be optimistic. Indeed, it is a good time to take stock of empirical interrelations linking both psychopathology and personality variation.

Figure 1. Proposed research landscape connecting normal range personality traits, maladaptive personality/psychopathology dimensions in the Hierarchical Taxonomy or Psychopathology (HiTOP) model, and constructs included in Research Domain Criteria (RDoC), Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment (ANA), and the National Institute on Drug Addiction’s Phenotyping Battery (NIDA PhAB). Normal personality dimensions reflect the Big Five model. Negative emotionality, incentive salience, and executive function are included in both ANA and NIDA PhAB, whereas the sleep domain is specific to NIDA PhAB. The links between HiTOP and RDoC are the strongest with the most empirical support, to date (see Michelini et al., 2021, for a comprehensive review). Less prominent or supported links are not shown. Due to paucity of relevant studies, it was not possible to link the recently introduced RDoC sensorimotor domain to any HiTOP spectra, nor the HiTOP somatoform spectrum to any RDoC domains. Negative associations between HiTOP and RDoC are presented in red and positive associations in blue. Double arrows indicate that, within an RDoC domain, some constructs show positive links, whereas others show negative links to the HiTOP spectrum (see Michelini et al., 2021, for details). Associations between normal personality and HiTOP dimensions, as well as between RDoC and ANA or NIDA PhAB domains, are shown with black symbols for approximate equality (≈). The gray approximate equality symbol depicts the fact that not all components of openness are roughly equivalent to HiTOP thought disorder (those related to intellectual engagement are largely unrelated). The links between RDoC and NIDA PhAB domains of metacognition and interoceptive processes are unclear and are not shown here.

1.1. Integration of personality and psychopathology into common quantitative hierarchical models

It is becoming increasingly clear not only that features of psychopathology are best described dimensionally, as noted above, but also that they are not categorically distinct from features of normal personality (Markon et al., Reference Markon, Krueger and Watson2005; Widiger & Trull, Reference Widiger and Trull2007; Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov and Wright2019). Persistent symptoms of psychopathology appear to be equivalent to extreme or maladaptive variants of normal features of personality. This has been particularly well established in relation to what have traditionally been designated “personality disorders” (e.g., Latzman & Kumari, Reference Latzman and Kumari2020). Symptoms of personality disorder are supposed to be long lasting in much the same way that personality traits are (indeed, they are often described as “maladaptive traits”), so it is not surprising that considerable attention has been given to comparing them to normal dimensions of personality. Empirical comparisons have indicated for some time that major dimensions of covariation among maladaptive traits are equivalent to four of the so-called Big Five dimensions of normal personality, namely neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (Markon et al., Reference Markon, Krueger and Watson2005; Widiger & Trull, Reference Widiger and Trull2007; Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov and Wright2019). With the exception of neuroticism, the maladaptive labels for these dimensions focus on the opposite pole (i.e., low extraversion, low agreeableness, and low conscientiousness): negative affect, detachment, antagonism, and disinhibition, respectively. Nonetheless, despite in most cases emphasizing the opposite pole of the trait, analyses using item response theory (IRT) have repeatedly shown that measures of these maladaptive traits are measuring the same underlying latent dimensions as measures of the Big Five (Stepp et al., Reference Stepp, Yu, Miller, Hallquist, Trull and Pilkonis2012; van Dijk et al., Reference van Dijk, Krueger and Laceulle2021).

Links between the fifth of the Big Five, labeled openness to experience or intellect, and the fifth factor of personality disorder symptoms, psychoticism, are more complicated, with two of the three IRT studies just cited not finding evidence that measures of these traits assess the same latent dimension. Nonetheless, various studies have found that measures of openness/intellect group together with measures of psychoticism (or positive schizotypy) in factor analysis (De Fruyt et al., Reference De Fruyt, De Clercq, De Bolle, Wille, Markon and Krueger2013; Gore & Widiger, Reference Gore and Widiger2013; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Yalch, Krueger, Wright, Markon and Hopwood2013), and large molecular genetic studies have shown genetic correlations and overlapping genetic variants for openness/intellect and risk of schizophrenia (Lo et al., Reference Lo, Hinds, Tung, Franz, Fan, Wang and Chen2017; Smeland et al., Reference Smeland, Frei, Kauppi, Hill, Li and Wang2017). It appears that some facets of the broader openness/intellect dimension (those related to aesthetic interest and fantasy proneness) confer risk of psychosis, whereas others (those related to intellectual engagement) do not, or are even protective (Allen et al., Reference Allen, Schreiber, Hall and Hallquist2020; Chmielewski et al., Reference Chmielewski, Bagby, Markon, Ring and Ryder2014; DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Grazioplene and Peterson2012; DeYoung, Carey, Krueger, & Ross, Reference DeYoung, Carey, Krueger and Ross2016). At any rate, it is clear that dimensions of variation in risk of and manifestations of personality disorder are largely equivalent to dimensions of normal personality.

In addition, this equivalence appears to extend not just to forms of psychopathology traditionally identified as personality disorders, but also to those formerly identified as “Axis I” clinical disorders, such as mood and anxiety disorders and schizophrenia. When symptoms of these other disorders are analyzed together with symptoms of personality disorders, they vary together in a structure similar to that of personality disorders and normal personality (Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov and Wright2019; Wright & Simms, Reference Wright and Simms2015). One striking example is that the general tendency toward internalizing problems (e.g., anxiety, depression, and phobia) is nearly indistinguishable from neuroticism (Griffith et al., Reference Griffith, Zinbarg, Craske, Mineka, Rose, Waters and Sutton2010). In a similar vein, a large literature suggests that disinhibition represents liability to externalizing problems (e.g., substance use and conduct problems; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning and Kramer2007; Patrick et al., Reference Patrick, Kramer, Krueger and Markon2013). Thus, the major dimensions of covariation among features of psychopathology closely parallel major dimensions of normal personality variation. This suggests that when people become dysfunctional, their dysfunction manifests in ways that reflects their personality traits. In addition, it suggests that extreme personality traits confer risk of psychopathology.

Because dimensions of personality and psychopathology largely overlap, their mechanisms are also likely to overlap, and studying the neurobiology of risk of psychopathology in relation to the neurobiology of personality can be highly advantageous. Extremes in the functioning of neural systems associated with variation in personality traits are likely to be risk factors for corresponding forms of psychopathology (DeYoung & Krueger, Reference DeYoung and Krueger2020). To understand the neural basis of psychopathology, therefore, we need to understand the neural basis of personality. Clinical neuroscience should attend to the fact that brain systems that vary from person to person in their normal functioning probably also confer risk of psychopathology when they are particularly extreme or unusual in their functioning.

1.2. Linking integrated models of personality–psychopathology to research initiatives

Integrated dimensional personality–psychopathology models are consistent with, and have informed, a variety of influential dimensional models of psychopathology, including HiTOP. Given the advantages of conceptualizing personality and psychopathology dimensionally and on the same continuum, it is important to identify linkages between personality–psychopathology models and the three-dimensional research frameworks launched by NIH, RDoC (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010), the Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment (ANA; Kwako et al., 2016), and the National Institute on Drug Abuse’s Phenotyping Assessment Battery (NIDA PhAB; Keyser-Marcus et al., Reference Keyser-Marcus, Ramey, Bjork, Adams and Moeller2021). These frameworks share with personality–psychopathology models, as well as HiTOP, the aim to characterize behavior on the basis of individual differences through dimensional constructs of relevance to psychopathology. Yet, whereas HiTOP is based on an explicit quantitative model of manifest psychopathology, NIH models are more strongly grounded in neuroscience and encompass both behavioral and biological units of analysis (Latzman et al., Reference Latzman, Palumbo, Krueger, Drislane and Patrick2020). For example, with regard to neuroscientific research strategies, HiTOP starts with empirically based dimensions of manifest psychopathology, seeking to understand how neuroscientific concepts interweave with these dimensions (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Cicero, Conway, DeYoung and Wright2021; Latzman et al., Reference Latzman, Palumbo, Krueger, Drislane and Patrick2020). By contrast, RDoC does not emphasize the need to understand the phenotypic structure of psychopathology, but rather encourages research on major underlying systems thought to be relevant to understanding why some people experience psychopathology (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2015).

The rich behavioral characterization of general and maladaptive features offered by integrated dimensional models of personality–psychopathology and HiTOP can complement NIH approaches and facilitate clinical translation of the knowledge gained through research informed by NIH frameworks. At the same time, mechanistic studies guided by NIH frameworks can be useful for establishing the etiological and neurobiological validity of HiTOP and personality–psychopathology models. It is becoming widely accepted that a given behavioral manifestation (e.g., antisocial behavior and social withdrawal) may arise from multiple etiological pathways and neural systems (Viding & McCrory, Reference Viding and McCrory2020). Thus, clarifying the multiple possible underpinnings of personality–psychopathology constructs may help refine phenotypic models of these constructs, identify objective biomarkers, and develop new neuroscience-informed treatments.

A recent comprehensive literature review illustrates the value of integrating dimensional personality–psychopathology and neuroscience research efforts (Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021). Specifically, Michelini and colleagues (Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021) sought to map an interface (or “cross-walk”) between HiTOP and RDoC based on review of studies that investigated associations between constructs consistent with these frameworks. RDoC was operationalized by measures consistent with RDoC constructs across units of analysis (e.g., neural circuits or behavior) and dimensions included in the HiTOP model were operationalized through latent variable modeling or questionnaires assessing such dimensions. The resulting RDoC–HiTOP interface delineates a conceptual mapping with robust links between each RDoC construct and HiTOP dimensions (e.g., between RDoC cognitive control and HiTOP disinhibited externalizing spectrum). It also highlights a number of less established linkages that can guide future research (e.g., between RDoC positive valence systems and HiTOP detachment). For example, the integration of personality and psychopathology would allow for an even more comprehensive HiTOP–RDoC interface. The extensive literature supporting the association between extraversion, the opposite pole of HiTOP detachment, and RDoC positive valence system could be leveraged to clearly demonstrate this link. This also underscores the critical importance of personality neuroscience for the advancement of clinical neuroscience. Indeed, clinical neuroscience scholars would be wise to familiarize themselves with neuroscience research on traits related to their dimensions of interest (DeYoung & Blain, Reference DeYoung, Blain, Corr and Matthews2020).

Considering the well-established parallels between normal personality and dimensions of psychopathology included in HiTOP, as well as between RDoC constructs and constructs included in ANA and NIDA PhAB, it may be possible to draw an integrative research landscape linking personality, psychopathology, and biobehavioral dimensions. We represent this landscape in Figure 1, which builds on the major connections between HiTOP and RDoC identified by Michelini et al. (Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021) by showing conceptual links to normal personality (on the left side) and ANA and NIDA PhAB frameworks (on the right side). Ultimately, integrative research efforts focused on personality, psychopathology, and their neural/etiological bases have the potential to promote parallel progress in understanding the underpinnings of behavioral and clinical phenomena and in informing clinical practice with regard to assessment and treatment.

1.3. Novel investigations of the connection between quantitative personality–psychopathology models and neuroscience: Overview of the current special issue

The articles included in this special issue offer excellent examples of research on the interrelations between neural systems and quantitative, dimensional models of personality–psychopathology. Empirical studies in this issue used a variety of neuroscientific methodologies, spanning functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI; Hyatt et al., Reference Hyatt, Hallowell, Owens, Weiss, Sweet and Miller2020; Neumann, Reference Neumann2020; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Marquardt, Disner, Burton, Davenport, Lissek and Sponheim2020; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Jahn, Hyatt, Owens, Carter, Sweet and Haas2021), electrophysiology (event-related potentials [ERPs]; Palumbo et al., Reference Palumbo, Perkins, Yancey, Brislin, Patrick and Latzman2020; Suzuki, Novak, Ait Oumeziane, Foti, & Samuel, Reference Suzuki, Novak, Ait Oumeziane, Foti and Samuel2020), positron emission tomography (PET; Gerritsen et al., Reference Gerritsen, Iyengar, DaSilva, Koppel, Rusjan, Bagby and Mizrahi2020), and structural neuroimaging (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Hinton, Meyer, Villalta-Gil, Hulle, Applegate and Zald2020). A number of quantitative methodologies were also employed, including structural equation modeling (SEM; e.g., Neumann, Reference Neumann2020) and “psychoneurometric” operationalizations (e.g., Palumbo et al., Reference Palumbo, Perkins, Yancey, Brislin, Patrick and Latzman2020). Given the different strengths and weaknesses of various individual analytic approaches used in the literature, these articles highlight the power of triangulating evidence from multiple neuroscience and quantitative methods for uncovering the neurobiological bases of the personality–psychopathology continuum. Two review articles complement the breaths of the topics and methods covered by the empirical studies and provide useful recommendations for future research in this field (McNaughton, Reference McNaughton2020; Shane et al., 2021).

Starting from functional neuroimaging studies, four articles in this special issue examined socioaffective processes in relation to personality–psychopathology dimensions using fMRI (Hyatt et al., Reference Hyatt, Hallowell, Owens, Weiss, Sweet and Miller2020; Neumann, Reference Neumann2020; Sun et al., 2020; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Jahn, Hyatt, Owens, Carter, Sweet and Haas2021). Neumann (Reference Neumann2020) investigated the association of amygdala activation in response to facial expressions with personality traits and symptoms of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology in a large sample of young adults (n = 1330). Cross-sectional SEM revealed interesting direct associations of amygdala activation with personality traits, but also indirect associations with psychopathology via personality, thereby advancing our knowledge of neurobiological–personality–psychopathology relationships.

Sun et al. (2020) investigated the associations among maladaptive personality, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms, and activation of brain regions implicated in emotion regulation during an emotional n-back task in US military veterans (n = 93). Greater PTSD symptoms were associated with weaker dorsolateral prefrontal cortex responses and blunted deactivation in amygdala and anterior cingulate. Furthermore, low positive emotionality statistically accounted for the cross-sectional relationship between PTSD symptoms and amygdala deactivation under high cognitive load conditions, pointing to a potential role of positive emotionality in the link between emotion dysregulation and PTSD symptoms.

Two articles included in this special issue used data from the Human Connectome Project (n ~ 1000) to further examine fMRI correlates of personality–psychopathology dimensions. Hyatt et al. (Reference Hyatt, Hallowell, Owens, Weiss, Sweet and Miller2020) examined neural responses to rewards as a candidate mechanism shared between five-factor model personality traits (with a focus on extraversion) and internalizing psychopathology. Weiss et al. (Reference Weiss, Jahn, Hyatt, Owens, Carter, Sweet and Haas2021) investigated neural activity and synchrony during a theory of mind task in relation to antagonistic personality and psychopathology. Neither study found associations surviving multiple testing corrections between neural and personality–psychopathology measures, consistent with growing evidence that effects in large samples tend to be small (Paulus & Thompson, Reference Paulus and Thompson2019). These studies offer rigorous examples of functional neuroimaging investigations of personality–psychopathology constructs and serve as a sobering reminder of how challenging it is to identify meaningful brain–personality–psychopathology relationships.

Complementary evidence on the relationship between socioaffective neural responses and personality–psychopathology comes from two ERP studies. In a community sample of adult twins (n = 507), Palumbo et al. (Reference Palumbo, Perkins, Yancey, Brislin, Patrick and Latzman2020) integrated self-reported affiliative tendencies and ERP responses to emotional faces into a multimodal measure of affiliative capacity (AFF), a biobehavioral construct reflecting ability and desire for sociointerpersonal bonds. The multimodal AFF index obtained through this psychoneurometric approach showed expected associations with antagonistic externalizing, distress, and theoretically relevant ERPs, confirming the validity, both convergent and discriminant, of this index. Focusing on error-related negativity (ERN) elicited by affective and social stimuli, Suzuki et al. (Reference Suzuki, Novak, Ait Oumeziane, Foti and Samuel2020) created a latent ERN measure from multiple tasks and examined its relationship with maladaptive personality traits in a sample of undergraduate students with a history of mental health treatment (n = 93). Despite not identifying significant associations in this small sample, this study illustrates an interesting example of individual difference research that incorporates neural and personality indicators.

In an in-vivo neuroimaging study, Gerritsen et al. (2020) used PET to quantify neuroimmune activation and personality traits across the psychosis spectrum in individuals with clinical high risk of psychosis, first-episode psychosis, and controls (total n = 61). Expression of a marker of central inflammation (translocator protein 18 kDa) was specifically associated with neuroticism, but with different signs across the three groups. If replicated in larger samples, these findings would suggest a link between neuroticism and neuroimmune activation that may vary across severity of the psychosis spectrum.

Structural MRI also emerged as a powerful tool for studying connections between brain and personality–psychopathology indicators. Lahey et al. (Reference Lahey, Hinton, Meyer, Villalta-Gil, Hulle, Applegate and Zald2020) investigated the association between three dispositional traits in adolescence and white matter microstructure in young adulthood in a twin sample enriched for psychopathology (n = 410). Dispositional traits showed sex-specific associations with subsequent functional anisotropy (FA) across major white matter tracts, such that greater FA was predicted by greater prosociality in females, but by greater negative emotionality and lower daring in males. These findings during the transition between adolescence and adulthood highlight interesting brain–behavior patterns that require future investigation, using repeated MRI and dispositional assessments, to clarify the nature of the identified sex-moderated associations.

Two final articles in this special issue are reviews covering promising methodologies and future directions for research connecting neuroscience and personality–psychopathology models. Shane et al. (2021) provided an introduction into the application of machine-learning algorithms in neuroimaging studies consistent with recent transdiagnostic approaches (i.e., RDoC and HiTOP) in order to parse heterogeneity in large transdiagnostic samples. Focusing on the externalizing spectrum, specifically, they noted the paucity of studies using machine-learning approaches and offered suggestions for future work to further the classification of externalizing psychopathology and elucidate its neurobiological underpinnings. Finally, McNaughton (Reference McNaughton2020) provided a thoughtful reflection on the lack of biological elements in current explanatory models of personality and psychopathology, arguing that these constructs should provide predictive explanations of patterns of affects, cognitions, desires, and behaviors guided by causal theory. The author then proposed an evolutionary biology approach to build models of personality traits and their clinical extremes based on conserved neural-level modulators of key emotional–motivational systems.

1.4. Recommendations for advancing the neuroscience of personality–psychopathology

Whereas there has been a clear boom in the field of clinical neuroscience over the past few decades, several factors have unfortunately discouraged the integration of clinical neuroscience with personality neuroscience. Probably, the most important of these is the long-standing reliance in clinical neuroscience on categorical diagnostic constructs and case–control designs. Now that clinical neuroscience research is moving toward the study of transdiagnostic, dimensional constructs, integrating that the study of personality is a much more natural fit. However, the legacy of case–control studies unfortunately lives on in the tendency to study dimensional constructs in clinical populations rather than the general population. Within a clinical population, the ranges of personality/symptom dimensions and of neural functioning are likely to be restricted, making it harder to identify the dimensions of variation that are most important for psychopathology. An alternative approach is to characterize large diverse samples using dimensional constructs consistent with HiTOP and integrated personality–psychopathology approaches (Latzman et al., Reference Latzman, Palumbo, Krueger, Drislane and Patrick2020; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021). This approach, using broad HiTOP-conformant assessments that inherently account for the multidimensional nature of psychopathology, adopted by several studies in this special issue (Hyatt et al., Reference Hyatt, Hallowell, Owens, Weiss, Sweet and Miller2020; Neumann, Reference Neumann2020; Palumbo et al., Reference Palumbo, Perkins, Yancey, Brislin, Patrick and Latzman2020; Weiss et al., Reference Weiss, Jahn, Hyatt, Owens, Carter, Sweet and Haas2021), allows researchers to simultaneously characterize the underpinnings of latent dimensions that span the “normal” personality range (e.g., low to high disinhibition) and the clinical manifestations that lie on the same continuum (e.g., substance abuse and antisocial behavior). Such research efforts on the neural and biobehavioral bases of personality and psychopathology also leverage the psychometrically robust dimensional targets that have been refined through decades of converging evidence from quantitative personality and psychopathology research (DeYoung et al., Reference DeYoung, Carey, Krueger and Ross2016; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017). This approach to sampling not only maximizes the knowledge gained from mechanistic research, but also promotes better integration among neuroscience, personality, and psychopathology.

When adopting a population-based approach, researchers may reasonably be concerned that a general-population sample will not contain enough people with psychopathology of the sort they wish to study. A solution to this problem is to enrich general population samples through recruiting strategies that target the psychopathology of interest, rather than to fall back on purely clinical samples (DeYoung & Krueger, Reference DeYoung and Krueger2020; Latzman et al., Reference Latzman, Palumbo, Krueger, Drislane and Patrick2020). Examples of this approach are studies by Lahey et al. (Reference Lahey, Hinton, Meyer, Villalta-Gil, Hulle, Applegate and Zald2020) and Suzuki et al. (Reference Suzuki, Novak, Ait Oumeziane, Foti and Samuel2020) in this issue. This approach is likely more advantageous for studies of the genetic, neural, and cognitive/affective mechanisms of clinical phenomena than the “transdiagnostic” sampling approach (i.e., mixed patient populations) often recommended by RDoC (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010). This is because transdiagnostic samples, albeit moving beyond a traditional case–control design, still largely rely on binary decisions regarding the presence of diagnosis or whether participants have sought treatment, and thus may not fully capture the continuous variation of clinical features, nor its links to normal personality.

Another factor that has been a barrier to integrating personality and clinical neuroscience has been the general tendency in neuroscience to study general human phenomena rather than individual differences. Clinical case–control designs are of course a form of studying individual differences, but most non-clinical neuroscience, especially MRI research, has focused on understanding how the brain works through within-person comparisons of the brain in different states. In general, neuroscientists have not had a sufficient appreciation for the differences in statistical power that exist between within-person and between-person designs. Far more participants are typically necessary to study variation between people’s brains than to study brain function in people on average. This is because individual differences tend to be small in sufficiently powered samples (Paulus & Thompson, Reference Paulus and Thompson2019), but are often inflated in underpowered samples (Algermissen & Mehler, Reference Algermissen and Mehler2018; Marek et al., Reference Marek, Tervo-Clemmens, Calabro, Montez, Kay, Hatoum and Dosenbach2020). Coupled with the high cost of MRI scanning, this often served to discourage researchers from collecting samples large enough for high-quality research on individual differences. This has also had the unfortunate consequence that many clinical case–control studies, besides studying flawed diagnostic constructs, were grossly underpowered, resulting in inflated effects. Recently, however, the need for much larger samples has been recognized in clinical neuroscience (Marek et al., Reference Marek, Tervo-Clemmens, Calabro, Montez, Kay, Hatoum and Dosenbach2020; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021), which will facilitate integrating personality and clinical neuroscience. Considering articles in this special issue, whereas a few studies used small samples of less than 100 subjects, the majority of the empirical studies included several hundreds of participants, suggesting a positive trend toward more reproducible effects.

Finally, the vast majority of studies in the literature, as well as the majority of articles in this special issue (see Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Hinton, Meyer, Villalta-Gil, Hulle, Applegate and Zald2020, for an exception), are cross-sectional in nature. There is thus a clear need for studies using large longitudinal samples to explicitly consider development. That is, how do dimensions on the psychopathology–personality continuum change across the lifespan and in their association with neural systems? The growth of large, publicly available datasets should provide researchers an opportunity to begin to investigate these important questions in sufficiently large, adequately powered samples ultimately leading to a more reliable understanding of the neuroscientific correlates of personality and psychopathology.

A final set of recommendations relates to practical and political issues at the intersection of the approach we outline here, and traditional psychiatric approaches to the classification of personality and psychopathology. Although the influence of categorical approaches to delineating mental disorders is waning under the weight of the corpus of evidence that psychopathological signs and symptoms do not cluster into readily identified and distinguishable categories of disease, traditional categorical labels still unfortunately frame much discourse in neuroscience. Ultimately, though, neuroscience might be a key locus of leverage in this ongoing discussion. Relative to mental health professionals who are reluctant to part ways with traditional categorical labels, neuroscientists may be more willing to simply follow the evidence where it leads. Indeed, this dynamic is a key part of the RDoC saga, in which NIMH leaders essentially grew weary of tethering technological innovation to categorical labels better suited to 19th century medical textbooks than to efforts to employ modern imaging technologies to understand substrates of human suffering. Our hope is that the approaches described in this editorial and associated special issue can help pave the way for further innovations in phenotype characterization that better reflects the empirical structure of human individual differences, and thereby provide compelling targets for neuroscientific inquiry.

2. Conclusions

It is quite evident that quantitatively derived, integrative models of personality–psychopathology represent a particularly promising conduit for advancing our understanding of the neurobiological foundation of human experience, both functional and dysfunctional. Together, the articles included in this special issue provide an important advance. Indeed, whereas we recognize that this field is still in its infancy, these articles represent exciting and rigorous examples of multidisciplinary research that can yield important insights into the neural, cognitive, and socioaffective underpinnings of personality and psychopathology. We hope that readers share our excitement for this growing field, and we are optimistic that this special issue will serve as a catalyst to motivate scholars to pursue new research into the interconnection between neuroscience and quantitative personality–psychopathology models.

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Professor Philip Corr, Editor Personality Neuroscience, for his support and encouragement of this editorial and this special issue.

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

Footnotes

In R. D. Latzman, G. Michelini, C. G. DeYoung, & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Novel investigations of the connection between quantitative personality psychopathology models and neuroscience.

References

Algermissen, J., & Mehler, D. M. A. (2018). May the power be with you: Are there highly powered studies in neuroscience, and how can we get more of them? Journal of Neurophysiology, 119, 21142117. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00765.2017 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, T. A., Schreiber, A. M., Hall, N. T., & Hallquist, M. N. (2020). From description to explanation: Integrating across multiple levels of analysis to inform neuroscientific accounts of dimensional personality pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, 34, 650676. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2020.34.5.650 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chmielewski, M., Bagby, R. M., Markon, K., Ring, A. J., & Ryder, A. G. (2014). Openness to experience, intellect, schizotypal personality disorder, and psychoticism: Resolving the controversy. Journal of Personality Disorders, 28, 483499. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2014_28_128 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuthbert, B. N. (2015). Research domain criteria: Toward future psychiatric nosologies. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 17, 8997. https://doi.org/10.31887/DCNS.2015.17.1/bcuthbert Google ScholarPubMed
De Fruyt, F., De Clercq, B., De Bolle, M., Wille, B., Markon, K., & Krueger, R. F. (2013). General and maladaptive traits in a five-factor framework for DSM-5 in a university student sample. Assessment, 20, 295307. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191113475808 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., & Blain, S. D. (2020). Personality neuroscience. In Corr, P. J. & Matthews, G. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (2nd ed., pp. 273291). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Carey, B. E., Krueger, R. F., & Ross, S. R. (2016). Ten aspects of the Big Five in the personality inventory for DSM-5. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7, 113123. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000170 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G., Grazioplene, R. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2012). From madness to genius: The openness/intellect trait domain as a paradoxical simplex. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 6378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.12.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., & Krueger, R. F. (2020). To wish impossible things: On the ontological status of latent variables and the prospects for theory in psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 31, 289296. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2020.1853462 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerritsen, C., Iyengar, Y., DaSilva, T., Koppel, A., Rusjan, P., Bagby, R., & Mizrahi, R. (2020). Personality traits in psychosis and psychosis risk linked to TSPO expression: A neuroimmune marker. Personality Neuroscience, 3, E14. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2020.14 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gore, W. L., & Widiger, T. A. (2013). The DSM-5 dimensional trait model and five-factor models of general personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 816821. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032822 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith, J. W., Zinbarg, R. E., Craske, M. G., Mineka, S., Rose, R. D., Waters, A. M., & Sutton, J. M. (2010). Neuroticism as a common dimension in the internalizing disorders. Psychological Medicine, 40, 11251136. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291709991449 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N., McGrath, M. J., Viechtbauer, W., & Kuppens, P. (2020). Dimensions over categories: A meta-analysis of taxometric research. Psychological Medicine, 50, 14181432. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172000183X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyatt, C. S., Hallowell, E. S., Owens, M. M., Weiss, B. M., Sweet, L. H., & Miller, J. D. (2020). An fMRI investigation of the relations between Extraversion, internalizing psychopathology, and neural activation following reward receipt in the Human Connectome Project sample. Personality Neuroscience, 3, e13. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2020.11 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 748751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keyser-Marcus, L. A., Ramey, T., Bjork, J., Adams, A., Moeller, F. G. (2021). Development and feasibility study of an addiction-focused phenotyping assessment battery. The American Journal on Addictions, 30, 398405. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajad.13170 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Jonas, K. G., Carpenter, W. T., Dretsch, M. N., Eaton, N. R., Forbes, M. K., … Watson, D. (2020). Validity and utility of hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): I. Psychosis superspectrum. World Psychiatry, 19, 151172. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20730 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., Bagby, R. M., … Zimmerman, M. (2017). The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 454477. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000258 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Cicero, D. C., Conway, C. C., DeYoung, C. G., … Wright, A. G. C. (2021). The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): A quantitative nosology based on consensus of evidence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17, 83108. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-093304 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krueger, R. F., Hobbs, K. A., Conway, C. C., Dick, D. M., Dretsch, M. N., Eaton, N. R., … HiTOP Utility Workgroup. (2020). Validity and utility of the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): II. Externalizing superspectrum. World Psychiatry, 19, 151172. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20730 Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F., Hobbs, K. A., Conway, C. C., Dick, D. M., Dretsch, M. N., Eaton, N. R., … Goldman, D. (2016). Addictions neuroclinical assessment: A neuroscience-based framework for addictive disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 80, 179189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.10.024 Google Scholar
Krueger, R. F., Kotov, R., Watson, D., Forbes, M. K., Eaton, N. R., Ruggero, C. J., … Zimmermann, J. (2018). Progress in achieving quantitative classification of psychopathology. World Psychiatry, 17, 282293. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20566 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., & Kramer, M. D. (2007). Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: An integrative quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 645666. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.645 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Hinton, K. E., Meyer, F. C., Villalta-Gil, V., Hulle, C. A. V., Applegate, B., … Zald, D. H. (2020). Sex differences in associations of socioemotional dispositions measured in childhood and adolescence with brain white matter microstructure 12 years later. Personality Neuroscience, 3, e5. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2020.3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latzman, R. D., DeYoung, C. G., & the HiTOP Neurobiological Foundations Workgroup (2020). Using empirically-derived dimensional phenotypes to accelerate clinical neuroscience: The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP) framework. Neuropsychopharmacology, 45, 10831085. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0639-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latzman, R. D., & Kumari, V. (2020). Neurobiological investigations of dimensionally conceptualized personality pathology: Mapping a way forward for the clinical neuroscience of personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 34, 577585. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2020.34.5.577 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latzman, R. D., Palumbo, I. M., Krueger, R. F., Drislane, L. E., & Patrick, C. J. (2020). Modeling relations between triarchic biobehavioral traits and DSM internalizing disorder dimensions. Assessment, 27, 11001115. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191119876022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lo, M.-T., Hinds, D. A., Tung, J. Y., Franz, C., Fan, C.-C., Wang, Y., … Chen, C.-H. (2017). Genome-wide analyses for personality traits identify six genomic loci and show correlations with psychiatric disorders. Nature Genetics, 49, 152156. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3736 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marek, S., Tervo-Clemmens, B., Calabro, F. J., Montez, D. F., Kay, B. P., Hatoum, A. S., … Dosenbach, N. U. F. (2020). Towards reproducible brain-wide association studies [preprint]. Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.08.21.257758 Google Scholar
Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the structure of normal and abnormal personality: An integrative hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 139157. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.139 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNaughton, N. (2020). Personality neuroscience and psychopathology: Should we start with biology and look for neural-level factors? Personality Neuroscience, 3, 116. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2020.5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michelini, G., Palumbo, I. M., DeYoung, C. G., Latzman, R. D., & Kotov, R. (2021). Linking RDoC and HiTOP: A new interface for advancing psychiatric nosology and neuroscience. Clinical Psychology Review, 86, 102025. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, C. S. (2020). Structural equation modeling of the associations between amygdala activation, personality, and internalizing, externalizing symptoms of psychopathology. Personality Neuroscience, 3, e8. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2020.8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palumbo, I. M., Perkins, E. R., Yancey, J. R., Brislin, S. J., Patrick, C. J., & Latzman, R. D. (2020). Toward a multimodal measurement model for the neurobehavioral trait of affiliative capacity. Personality Neuroscience, 3, e11. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2020.9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, C. J., Kramer, M. D., Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2013). Optimizing efficiency of psychopathology assessment through quantitative modeling: Development of a brief form of the Externalizing Spectrum Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 25, 13321348. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034864 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, M. P., & Thompson, W. K. (2019). The challenges and opportunities of small effects: The new normal in academic psychiatry. JAMA Psychiatry, 76, 353354. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.4540 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeland, O. B., Frei, O., Kauppi, K., Hill, W. D., Li, W., Wang, Y., … for the NeuroCHARGE (Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology) Cognitive Working Group (2017). Identification of genetic loci jointly influencing schizophrenia risk and the cognitive traits of verbal-numerical reasoning, reaction time, and general cognitive function. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 1065. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.1986 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stepp, S. D., Yu, L., Miller, J. D., Hallquist, M. N., Trull, T. J., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2012). Integrating competing dimensional models of personality: Linking the SNAP, TCI, and NEO using item response theory. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 3, 107126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025905 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, M., Marquardt, C., Disner, S., Burton, P., Davenport, N., Lissek, S., & Sponheim, S. (2020). Posttraumatic stress symptomatology and abnormal neural responding during emotion regulation under cognitive demands: Mediating effects of personality. Personality Neuroscience, 3, E9. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2020.10 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suzuki, T., Novak, K. D., Ait Oumeziane, B., Foti, D., & Samuel, D. B. (2020). The hierarchical structure of error-related negativities elicited from affective and social stimuli and their relations to personality traits. Personality Neuroscience, 3, e15. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2020.15 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, K. M., Yalch, M. M., Krueger, R. F., Wright, A. G. C., Markon, K. E., & Hopwood, C. J. (2013). The convergent structure of DSM-5 personality trait facets and five-factor model trait domains. Assessment, 20, 308311. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191112457589 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Dijk, I., Krueger, R. F., & Laceulle, O. M. (2021). DSM-5 alternative personality disorder model traits as extreme variants of five-factor model traits in adolescents. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 12, 5969. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000409 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viding, E., & McCrory, E. (2020). Disruptive behavior disorders: The challenge of delineating mechanisms in the face of heterogeneity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 811817. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20070998 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Levin-Aspenson, H. F., Waszczuk, M. A., Conway, C. C., Dalgleish, T., Dretsch, M. N., … HiTOP Utility Workgroup (in press). Validity and utility of hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): III. Emotional dysfunction superspectrum. World Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Weiss, B., Jahn, A., Hyatt, C. S., Owens, M. M., Carter, N. T., Sweet, L. H., … Haas, B. W. (2021). Investigating the neural substrates of antagonistic externalizing and social-cognitive theory of mind: An fMRI examination of functional activity and synchrony. Personality Neuroscience, 4, e1. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2020.12 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widiger, T. A., Sellbom, M., Chmielewski, M., Clark, L. A., DeYoung, C. G., Kotov, R.,… Wright, A. G. C. (2019). Personality in a hierarchical model of psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 7, 7792. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618797105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (2007). Plate tectonics in the classification of personality disorder: Shifting to a dimensional model. American Psychologist, 62, 7183. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.71 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, A. G. C., & Simms, L. J. (2015). A metastructural model of mental disorders and pathological personality traits. Psychological Medicine, 45, 23092319. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715000252 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Proposed research landscape connecting normal range personality traits, maladaptive personality/psychopathology dimensions in the Hierarchical Taxonomy or Psychopathology (HiTOP) model, and constructs included in Research Domain Criteria (RDoC), Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment (ANA), and the National Institute on Drug Addiction’s Phenotyping Battery (NIDA PhAB). Normal personality dimensions reflect the Big Five model. Negative emotionality, incentive salience, and executive function are included in both ANA and NIDA PhAB, whereas the sleep domain is specific to NIDA PhAB. The links between HiTOP and RDoC are the strongest with the most empirical support, to date (see Michelini et al., 2021, for a comprehensive review). Less prominent or supported links are not shown. Due to paucity of relevant studies, it was not possible to link the recently introduced RDoC sensorimotor domain to any HiTOP spectra, nor the HiTOP somatoform spectrum to any RDoC domains. Negative associations between HiTOP and RDoC are presented in red and positive associations in blue. Double arrows indicate that, within an RDoC domain, some constructs show positive links, whereas others show negative links to the HiTOP spectrum (see Michelini et al., 2021, for details). Associations between normal personality and HiTOP dimensions, as well as between RDoC and ANA or NIDA PhAB domains, are shown with black symbols for approximate equality (≈). The gray approximate equality symbol depicts the fact that not all components of openness are roughly equivalent to HiTOP thought disorder (those related to intellectual engagement are largely unrelated). The links between RDoC and NIDA PhAB domains of metacognition and interoceptive processes are unclear and are not shown here.