Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-jbqgn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-17T15:37:18.872Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Rhythm in Acousmatic Music

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 August 2020

James Andean*
Affiliation:
Music, Technology and Innovation – Institute for Sonic Creativity (MTI2), De Montfort University, Leicester, UK

Abstract

‘Rhythm’ is often equated with ‘metered pulse’; as the latter is often eschewed by contemporary music, including acousmatic music, this is often assumed to mean an absence of rhythm. This article proposes that, in fact, acousmatic music does indeed contain rhythmic qualities, and further, that rhythm is one of the dominating forces of acousmatic music, even when pulse or metre at first glance appear to be lacking. This stems from the roots of acousmatic philosophy in phenomenology and a steady focus on our experience of the world around us. Importantly, this points simultaneously towards rhythmic qualities of our environment and towards rhythmic qualities of our embodied experience of that world, due to rhythmic aspects of our bodies, our perception and our cognitive faculties.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© Cambridge University Press, 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Andean, J. 2016. A Temporal Basis for Acousmatic Rhythm. Leonardo Music Journal 26: 6870.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. 1971. Aesthetics and Psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Chion, M. 1982. La musique électroacoustique. Paris: Presses universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Chion, M. 1983. Guide des objets sonores. Paris: INA, Editions Buchet/Chastel.Google Scholar
Clarke, E. 2005. Ways of Listening: An Ecological Approach to the Perception of Musical Meaning. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clynes, M. and Walker, J. 1982. Neurobiologic Functions of Rhythm, Time, and Pulse in Music. In Clynes, M. (ed.) Music, Mind, and Brain: The Neuropsychology of Music. New York: Plenum, 171216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraisse, P. 1982. Rhythm and Tempo. In Deutsch, D. (ed.) The Psychology of Music. New York: Academic Press, 149–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huron, D. 2008. Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jones, M. R., Moynihan, H., Mackenzie, N. and Puente, J. 2002. Temporal Aspects of Stimulus-driven Attending in Dynamic Arrays. Psychological Science 13(4): 313–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krause, B. 1987. The Niche Hypothesis: How Animals Taught Us to Dance and Sing. Bio Science 61(3): 203–16.Google Scholar
Mauro, D. 2006. The Rhythmic Brain. CogSci/ICCS 2006 Proceedings. http://csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu/proceedings/2006/iccs/p163.pdf Google Scholar
Roads, C. 2001. Microsound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schafer, R. M. 1977. The Tuning of the World. Philadelphia, PA: University of Philadelphia Press.Google Scholar
Thaut, M. 2005. Rhythm, Music, and the Brain: Scientific Foundations and Clinical Applications. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Thaut, M. 2009. The Musical Brain: An Artful Biological Necessity. Karger Gazette 70: 24.Google Scholar
Windsor, W. L. 1995. A Perceptual Approach to the Description and Analysis of Acousmatic Music. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, City University, London.Google Scholar