Hostname: page-component-76dd75c94c-7vt9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T08:44:55.660Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Production of the double object construction: An experiment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 October 2017

Johannes Kizach*
Affiliation:
Department of English, School of Communication and Culture, Aarhus University, Jens Chr. Skous Vej 4, DK-8000 Aarhus C, Denmark. engjk@cc.au.dk
Get access

Abstract

In the double object construction (e.g. the man gave the woman some flowers) a preference has been observed for placing definite arguments before indefinite arguments when both appear post-verbally. In Danish it has been reported that examples with the indefinite–definite order are read more slowly than those with the definite–indefinite order in speeded acceptability judgement tasks, and they are less frequent in corpus texts. This short communication presents a memory recall experiment showing that the preference observed in comprehension and written production is also observed in on-line oral production. Participants produce definite–indefinite orders when attempting to recall definite–indefinite orders in 95% of the cases, but when attempting to recall indefinite–definite orders they alter the definiteness of one or both of the arguments and produce indefinite–definite orders only in 6% of the cases.

Type
Short Communications
Copyright
Copyright © Nordic Association of Linguistics 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Arnold, Jennifer E., Wasow, Thomas, Losongco, Anthony & Ginstrom, Ryan. 2000. Heaviness vs. newness: The effects of structural complexity and discourse status on constituent ordering. Language 76, 2855.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald. 2008. Analyzing Linguistic Data. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barr, Dale J., Levy, Roger, Scheepers, Christoph & Tily, Harry J.. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68 (3), 255278.Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Kliegl, Reinhold, Vasishth, Shravan & Baayen, [R.] Harald. 2015. Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv preprint arXiv:1506.04967. https://arxiv.org/abs/1506.04967 (accessed 20 April 2017).Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Maechler, Martin, Bolker, Ben & Walker, Steven. 2015. lme4: Linear Mixed-effects Models Using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-9. http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html (accessed 10 February 2016).Google Scholar
Bock, J. Kathryn & Warren, Richard K.. 1985. Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation. Cognition 21, 4767.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David. 2015. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer, Version 5.3.65, http://www.praat.org/ (accessed 13 March 2016).Google Scholar
Brown, Meredith, Savova, Virginia & Gibson, Edward. 2012. Syntax encodes information structure: Evidence from on-line reading comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 66 (1), 194209.Google Scholar
Clifton, Charles & Frazier, Lyn. 2004. Should given information come before new? Yes and no. Memory & Cognition 32 (6), 886895.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forster, Kenneth I. & Forster, Jonathan C.. 2003. DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 35 (1), 116124.Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy. 1983. Topic Continuity in Discourse: A Quantitative Cross-language Study. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Harrell, Frank E. & Dupont, Charles. 2015. Hmisc: Harrell miscellaneous (R package version 3.17-1). http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Hmisc (accessed 13 January 2016).Google Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 1994. A Performance Theory of Order and Constituency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 1998. A processing approach to word order in Danish. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 30 (1), 63101.Google Scholar
Kizach, Johannes & Balling, Laura Winther. 2013. Givenness, complexity, and the Danish dative alternation. Memory & Cognition 41 (8), 11591171.Google Scholar
Kizach, Johannes & Mathiasen, Thomas. 2013. The dative alternation in Danish and Polish: A learner's perspective. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics 49, 487507.Google Scholar
Kizach, Johannes & Vikner, Sten. Head adjacency and the Danish dative alternation. Studia Linguistica, doi:10.1111/stul.12047. Published online 21 March 2016.Google Scholar
MacDonald, Maryellen C. 2013. How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology 4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newmeyer, Frederick J. 2000. Language Form and Language Function. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Potter, Mary C. & Lombardi, Linda. 1998. Syntactic priming in immediate recall of sentences. Journal of Memory and Language 38 (3), 265282.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team. 2015. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org/ (accessed 13 January 2016).Google Scholar