Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T11:07:54.931Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Variable stem-final fricative voicing in American English plurals: Different pa[ð ~ θ]s of change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 October 2018

Laurel MacKenzie*
Affiliation:
New York University

Abstract

This paper investigates analogical leveling in a small set of English nouns that have irregular plural forms. In these nouns, all of which end in a voiceless fricative, the fricative standardly voices in the plural (e.g., wolfwol[v]es, pathpa[ð]s, househou[z]es). Using audio data from three large spoken corpora of American English, I demonstrate that this stem-final fricative voicing is variable and conditioned by a number of factors, most notably the identity of the stem-final fricative—with /f/-final lexemes (e.g., wolf), /θ/-final lexemes (e.g., path), and the /s/-final lexeme house all patterning differently in apparent time—and the frequency of a lexeme in its plural form. I argue that the way these two factors affect the variation is reminiscent of the patterns seen in children's first language acquisition errors, providing a potential source for the variation and underscoring the importance of considering morphophonological factors when accounting for patterns of change.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Thanks to Lucy Giannasi and Amy Hughes for data collection, supported by the University of Manchester's Q-Step Centre. Thanks also to Charles Yang, Isaac Bleaman, members of NYU's Sociolinguistics Lab, audiences at the Second Edinburgh Symposium on Historical Phonology at the University of Edinburgh (December 3–4, 2015) and New Ways of Analyzing Variation 45 in Vancouver (November 3–6, 2016), and two anonymous reviewers for many helpful comments.

References

REFERENCES

Bates, Douglas, Mächler, Martin, Bolker, Ben, & Walker, Steve. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67:148.Google Scholar
Becker, Michael, Nevins, Andrew, & Levine, Jonathan. (2012). Asymmetries in generalizing alternations to and from initial syllables. Language 88:231268.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul, & Weenink, David. (2015). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [computer program]. Version 5.4.14. Available at: http://www.praat.org. Accessed July 30, 2015.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, Marc, & New, Boris. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods 41:977990.Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., & Slobin, Dan I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58:265289.Google Scholar
Chambers, J. K. (2009). Sociolinguistic theory: Linguistic variation and its social significance. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam, & Halle, Morris. (1968). The sound pattern of English. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cieri, Christopher, Graff, David, Kimball, Owen, Miller, Dave, & Walker, Kevin. (2004). Fisher English training speech parts 1 and 2. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Clopper, Cynthia G. (2011). Checking for reliability. In Di Paolo, M. & Yaeger-Dror, M. (eds.), Sociophonetics: A student's guide. London: Routledge. 188197.Google Scholar
Davidson, Lisa. (2016). Variability in the implementation of voicing in American English obstruents. Journal of Phonetics 54:3550.Google Scholar
De Schryver, Johan, Neijt, Anneke, Ghesquière, Pol, & Ernestus, Mirjam. (2008). Analogy, frequency, and sound change: The case of Dutch devoicing. Journal of Germanic Linguistics 20:159195.Google Scholar
Fruehwald, Josef. (2017). Generations, lifespans, and the zeitgeist. Language Variation and Change 29:127.Google Scholar
Godfrey, John J., & Holliman, Edward. (1997). Switchboard-1 release 2. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
Hayes, Bruce. (2011). Introductory phonology. Malden: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hooper, Joan B. (1976). Word frequency in lexical diffusion and the source of morphophonological change. In Christie, W. M., , Jr. (ed.), Current progress in historical linguistics: Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Historical Linguistics, Tucson, Arizona, 12–16 January 1976. Amsterdam: North Holland. 96105.Google Scholar
Jansen, Wouter. (2007). Phonological voicing, phonetic voicing, and assimilation in English. Language Sciences 29:270293.Google Scholar
Jespersen, Otto. (1909). A modern English grammar on historical principles, part I: Sounds and spellings. Heidelberg: Carl Winters Universitätsbuchhandlung.Google Scholar
Jespersen, Otto. (1942). A modern English grammar on historical principles, part VI: Morphology. Copenhagen: Ejnar Munksgaard.Google Scholar
Johnson, Daniel Ezra. (2009). Getting off the GoldVarb standard: Introducing Rbrul for mixed-effects variable rule analysis. Language and Linguistics Compass 3:359383.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (2001). Principles of linguistic change: Social factors. Malden: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, William, & Rosenfelder, Ingrid. (2011). The Philadelphia Neighborhood Corpus of LING 560 studies, 1972–2010. With support of NSF contract 921643. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Lass, Roger. (2000). Phonology and morphology. In Lass, R. (ed.), The Cambridge history of the English language. Vol. 3: 1476–1776. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 56186.Google Scholar
Myers, Scott. (2010). Regressive voicing assimilation: Production and perception studies. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 40:163179.Google Scholar
Phillips, Betty S. (1984). Word frequency and the actuation of sound change. Language 60:320342.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. (1994). The language instinct. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Raphael, Lawrence J. (1972). Preceding vowel duration as a cue to the perception of the voicing characteristic of word-final consonants in American English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 51:12961303.Google Scholar
Regel, Stefanie, Opitz, Andreas, Müller, Gereon, & Friederici, Angela D. (2015). The past tense debate revisited: Electrophysiological evidence for subregularities of irregular verb inflection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27:18701885.Google Scholar
Ringe, Donald, & Eska, Joseph F. (2013). Historical linguistics: Toward a twenty-first century reintegration. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian. (2005). Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies in sociolinguistics. In Ammon, U., Dittmar, N., Mattheier, K. J., & Trudgill, P. (eds.), Sociolinguistics: An international handbook of the science of language and society. Vol. 2. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. 10031012.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, Scott. (2016). Contextual and morphological effects in speech production. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Smith, Caroline L. (1997). The devoicing of /z/ in American English: Effects of local and prosodic context. Journal of Phonetics 25:471500.Google Scholar
van Heuven, Walter J. B., Mandera, Pawel, Keuleers, Emmanuel, & Brysbaert, Marc. (2014). SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 67:11761190.Google Scholar
Weide, Robert. (2008). The CMU Pronouncing Dictionary. Pittsburgh: Carnegie Mellon University, Speech Group.Google Scholar
Yang, Charles. (2002). Knowledge and learning in natural language. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar