Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T14:59:06.569Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

“Mam, my trousers is fa'in doon!”: Community, caregiver, and child in the acquisition of variation in a Scottish dialect

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 January 2007

Jennifer Smith
Affiliation:
Glasgow University
Mercedes Durham
Affiliation:
University of York
Liane Fortune
Affiliation:
University of York

Abstract

Recent work on acquisition in sociolinguistic research suggests that some aspects of the structured variation found in adult speech are evident in children's speech from the very start of language acquisition, and input from the primary caregiver is crucial in this process. In this article we contribute to this research by conducting a cross-sectional analysis of the acquisition of variable forms in a Scottish dialect. Two linguistic variables are targeted in the speech of eleven children (2;10–3;6) and their primary caregivers. Quantitative analysis of over 5000 contexts of use demonstrates that one variable is conditioned by social and linguistic constraints in the speech of the caregiver and these constraints are matched by the children. In contrast, the other variable is influenced by a complex array of linguistic constraints only. We explore the ramifications of these findings for understanding the mechanisms involved in acquisition of variation from the very earliest stages.We gratefully acknowledge the support of the Economic and Social Research Council for award no. RES-000-22-0447. We thank Jack Chambers, Paul Foulkes, and three anonymous reviewers for very helpful feedback on earlier versions of this manuscript. We would also like to thank Hazel Steele for her input to the project in its early stages, and Moira Smith, our invaluable on-site fieldworker. We dedicate this article to the caregivers and children of Buckie who were willing to share their walks, playtimes, bathtimes, and even “meltdowns,” all in the name of linguistic research.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2007 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adger, David, & Smith, Jennifer. (in preparation). Variation and minimalism: A combined account.
Barber, Charles. (1997). Early Modern English. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Brown, Roger. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Chambers, Jack. (2003). Sociolinguistic theory. Oxford: Blackwell.
Cheshire, Jenny. (1982). Variation in an English Dialect: A sociolinguistic study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cheshire, Jenny. (1999). Taming the vernacular: Some repercussions for the study of syntactic variation and spoken grammar. In J. C. Conde-Sylvestre & J. M. Hernandez-Campoy (eds.), Cuadernos de filologia inglesa 8: 5980.
Clarke, Sandra. (1997). English verbal -s revisited: The evidence from Newfoundland. American Speech 72(3):227259.Google Scholar
Cross, T. G. (1977). Mothers' speech adjustments: The contributions of selected child listener variables. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press. 151188.
Cukor-Avila, Patricia. (1997). Change and stability in the use of verbal -s over time in AAVE. In Edgar W. Schneider (ed.), Englishes around the world. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 295306.
Davis, Barbara, & Lindblom, Bjorn. (2001). Phonetic variability in baby talk and development of vowel categories. In Francisco Lacerda, Claes von Hofsten, & Mikael Heimann (eds.), Emerging cognitive abilities in early infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 135171.
Denison, David. (1993/2004). English historical syntax: Verbal constructions. Longman Linguistics Library. London: Longman.
Eremeeva, V., & Stuart-Smith, J. (2003). A sociophonetic investigation of the vowels OUT and BIT in Glaswegian. Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona. 12051208.
Fasold, Ralph. (1972). Tense marking in Black English. Arlington, VA: Center for Applied Linguistics.
Ferguson, Charles. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist 66:103114.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers' speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology 20:104113.Google Scholar
Fischer, O., van Keemenade, A., Koopman, W., & van der Wurff, W. (2000). The syntax of Early English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Foulkes, Paul, Docherty, Gerry, & Watt, Dom. (1999). Tracking the emergence of structured variation: Realisations of (t) by Newcastle children. Leeds Working Papers in Linguistics and Phonetics 7:125.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul, Docherty, Gerry, & Watt, Dom. (2005). Phonological variation in child-directed speech. Language 81:77206.Google Scholar
Galasso, Joseph. (2004). The acquisition of inflection and the dual mechanism model: A case study. Proceedings of the 2004 Child Language Research Forum Stanford. 98108.
Garnica, O. K. (1977). Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 6388.
Gleason, J. B., & Perlmann, R. Y. (1985). Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language 16:161179.Google Scholar
Godfrey, Elizabeth, & Tagliamonte, Sali A. (1999). Another piece for the verbal -s story: Evidence from Devon in southwest England. Language Variation and Change 11(1):87121.Google Scholar
Grieser, D. L., & Kuhl, P. K. (1988). Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in parentese. Developmental Psychology 24:1420.Google Scholar
Hay, Jen, & Schreier, Daniel. (2004). Reversing the trajectory of language change: Subject–verb agreement with be in New Zealand English. Language Variation and Change 16:209235.Google Scholar
Hackenberg, Robert. (1972). Appalachian English: A sociolinguistic study. Doctoral dissertation, Georgetown University.
Henry, A., MacLaren, R., Wilson, J., & Finlay, C. (1997). The acquisition of negative concord in non-standard English. In E. Hughes, M. Hughes, & A. Greenhill (eds.), Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Houston, S. (1969). A sociolinguistic consideration of the Black English of children in northern Florida. Language 45:599607.Google Scholar
Johnston, Paul. (1997). Regional variation. In Charles Jones (ed.), The Edinburgh history of Scots. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. 433513.
Kerswill, Paul. (1996). Children, adolescents and language change. Language Variation and Change 8:177202.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul, & Williams, Ann. (2000). Creating a new town koine: Children and language change in Milton Keynes. Language in Society 29:65115.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul, & Williams, Ann. (2005). New towns and koineisation: Linguistic and social correlates. Linguistics 43:10231048Google Scholar
Kovac, C., & Adamson, H. D. (1981). Variation theory and first language acquisition. In D. Sankoff & H. Cedergren (eds.), Variation omnibus. Edmonton, Alberta: Linguistic Research, Inc.
Labov, William. (1972). Sociolinguistic patterns. Oxford: Blackwell.
Labov, William. (1989). The child as linguistic historian. Language Variation and Change 1:8597.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (1994). Principles of linguistic change. Volume 1: Internal factors. Oxford: Blackwell.
Labov, William. (2001). Principles of linguistic change. Volume 2: Social factors. Oxford: Blackwell.
Labov, William, Cohen, P., Robins, C., & Lewis, J. (1968). A study of the non-standard English of Negro and Puerto Rican Speakers in New York City. Cooperative Research Report 3288. Vols. I & II. Philadelphia: Linguistics Laboratory, University of Pennsylvania, U.S. Regional Survey.
Liu, Huei-Mei, Kuhl, Patricia K., & Tsao, Feng-Ming. (2003). An association between mothers' speech clarity and infants' speech discrimination skills. Developmental Science 6:F1F10.Google Scholar
Macafee, Caroline. (1994). Traditional dialect in the Modern World: A Glasgow case study. Frankfurt am main: Peter Lang.
Macafee, Caroline. (1997). Ongoing change in modern Scots: The social dimension. In Charles Jones (ed.), The Edinburgh history of the Scots language. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. 514548.
Macaulay, Ronald K. S. (1991). Locating dialect in discourse: The language of honest men and bonnie lassies in Ayr. New York: Oxford University Press.
Macaulay, Ronald, & Trevelyan, G. (1973). Language, education and employment in Glasgow. Final report to the SSRC.
Meechan, Marjory, & Foley, Michele. (1994). On resolving disagreement: Linguistic theory and variation—There's bridges. Language Variation and Change 6:6385.Google Scholar
Montgomery, Michael. (1989). Exploring the roots of Appalachian English. English World-Wide 10:227278.Google Scholar
Montgomery, Michael. (1997). Making transatlantic connections between varieties of English: The case of plural verbal -s. Journal of English Linguistics 25:122141.Google Scholar
Murray, James. (1873). The dialect of the southern counties of Scotland. London: Philological Society.
Newport, E., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Purcell, April. (1984). Code-switching Hawaiian style: Children's accommodation along a decreolizing continuum. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 46:7186.Google Scholar
Poplack, Shana, & Tagliamonte, Sali A. (1989). There's no tense like the present: Verbal -s inflection in early Black English. Language Variation and Change 1:4784.Google Scholar
Poplack, Shana, & Tagliamonte, Sali A. (2005). Back to the present: Verbal –s in the (African American) diaspora. In Raymond Hickey (ed.), Transported dialects: Legacies of non-standard colonial English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 203223.
Roberts, Julie. (1994). Acquisition of variable rules: (-t,d) deletion and (ing) production in preschool children. Doctoral dissertation, Department of Linguistics, University of Pennsylvania.
Roberts, Julie. (1997a). Acquisition of variable rules: A study of (-t,d) deletion in preschool children. Journal of Child Language 24:351372.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie. (1997b). Hitting a moving target: Acquisition of sound changes by Philadelphia children. Language Variation and Change 9:249266.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie. (2002). Child language variation. In J. Chambers, N. Schilling-Estes, & P. Trudgill (eds.), Handbook of language variation & change. Oxford: Blackwell.
Roberts, Julie. (2005). Acquisition of sociolinguistic variation. In Martin Ball (ed.), Clinical sociolinguistics. Oxford: Blackwell. 153164.
Roberts, Julie, & Labov, William. (1995). Learning to talk Philadelphian: Acquisition of short a by preschool children. Language Variation and Change 7:101112.Google Scholar
Robinson, Mairi. (ed.). (1987). Concise Scots Dictionary. Aberdeen, Scotland: Aberdeen University Press.
Romaine, Suzanne. (1975). Approaches to the description of Scots English: Work in progress. Department of Linguistics. University of Edinburgh 8:121124.
Romaine, Suzanne. (1979). The language of Edinburgh schoolchildren: The acquisition of sociolinguistic competence. Scottish Literary Journal 9:5561.Google Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne. (1984). The language of children and adolescents. Oxford: Blackwell.
Schneider, E. W. (1983). The origin of the verbal -s in Black English. American Speech 58:99113.Google Scholar
Singler, John Victor. (1991). Copula variation in Liberian Settler English and American Black English. In Walter F. Edwards & Donald Winford (eds.), Verb phrase patterns in Black English and Creole. Detroit, MI: Wayne State University Press. 129164.
Smith, Jennifer. (2000a). Synchrony and diachrony in the evolution of English: Evidence from Scotland. Doctoral dissertation, University of York.
Smith, Jennifer. (2000b). You Ø na hear o' that kind o' things: Negative do in Buckie Scots. English-World-Wide 21(2):231259.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer. (2001). Negative concord in the Old and New World: Evidence from Scotland. Language Variation and Change 13(2):109134.Google Scholar
Smith, Jennifer. (2003–2005). Caregiver, community and child in the acquisition of variable dialect features. ESRC funded project no RES-000-22-0447.
Smith, Jennifer, & Tagliamonte, Sali. (1998). “We was all thegither, I think we were all thegither”: Was-regularization in Buckie English. World Englishes 17(2):105126.Google Scholar
Snow, Catherine. (1994). Introduction. In C. Gallaway & B. Richards (eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 312.
Snow, Catherine. (1995). Issues in the study of input: Fine tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In Paul Fletcher & Brian MacWhinney (eds.), The handbook of child language. Oxford: Blackwell. 180193.
Stuart-Smith, J. (2003). The phonology of Modern Urban Scots. In J. Corbett; D. McClure & J. Stuart-Smith (eds.), Edinburgh Student Companion to Scots. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. 110137.
Tagliamonte, Sali, and Smith, Jennifer. (2003). Either it isn't or it's not: Neg/aux contraction in British dialects. English World-Wide 23(2):251282.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, S., Smith, J., & Lawrence, H. (2005). English dialects in the British Isles in cross-variety perspective: A base-line for future research. In M. Filppula, J. Klemola, M. Palander, & E. Penttilä (eds.), Dialects across borders. Selected papers from the 11th International Conference on Methods in Dialectology (Methods XI), Joensuu, August 2002. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 87117.
Weinreich, Uriel, Labov, William, & Herzog, M. (1968). Empirical foundations for a theory of language change. In W. P. Lehmann & Y. Malkiel (eds.), Directions for historical linguistics. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. 95188.
Wells, John. (1982). Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wolfram, Walt. (1969). A sociolinguistic description of Detroit Negro speech. Arlington, VA: Center for Applied Linguistics.
Wolfram, Walt, & Christian, Donna. (1975). Sociolinguistic variables in Appalachian dialects. Final report, National Institute of Education grant number 74-0026.
Youssef, Valerie. (1990). The early development of perfect aspect: Adverbial, verbal and contextual specifications. Journal of Child Language. 17:295312.Google Scholar
Youssef, Valerie. (1991). Variation as a feature of language acquisition in the Trinidad context. Language Variation and Change 3:75101.Google Scholar
Youssef, Valerie. (1993). Children's linguistic choices: Audience design and societal norms. Language in Society 22:257274.Google Scholar
Youssef, Valerie. (1996). Varilingualism: The competence underlying code-mixing in Trinidad and Tobago. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 11(1):122.Google Scholar