Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-jbqgn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-22T13:24:09.093Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Multilingualism in older age: A research agenda from the cognitive perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 July 2019

Jessica G. Cox*
Affiliation:
Franklin and Marshall College, Lancaster, PA, USA

Abstract

Calls to diversify second language acquisition (SLA) (e.g., Ortega, 2013) have led to increased interest in multilingualism and inclusion of groups less represented in samples of university students, such as individuals at older ages. Nevertheless, we still have more questions than we do answers. This article outlines a research agenda targeting older adult language learning and multilinguals at older ages, both in and beyond the classroom. Since a key difference between young and older adults is cognitive aging, I follow a cognitive approach, focusing on how individual differences in cognition may affect language and vice versa, and how relevant sociocultural factors add to the interplay between language and cognition. Notably, this is not always a story of decline and deficits, but instead of both strengths and weaknesses that differ from those of young adults.

Type
Thinking Allowed
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adamuti-Trache, M. (2013). Language acquisition among adult immigrants in Canada: The effect of premigration language capital. Adult Education Quarterly, 63(2), 103126.Google Scholar
Amer, T., Campbell, K. L., & Hasher, L. (2016). Cognitive control as a double-edged sword. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20(12), 905915. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.002Google Scholar
Anderson, J., Mak, L., Chahi, A. K., & Bialystok, E. (2018). The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 250263. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9Google Scholar
Andrew, P. (2012). The social construction of age: Adult foreign language learners. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Antón, E., Fernández García, Y., Carreiras, M., & Andoni Duñabeitia, J. (2016). Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 147160.Google Scholar
Arxer, S. L., Ciriza, M. d. P., & Shappeck, M. (2017). Aging in a second language: A case study of aging, immigration, and an English learner speech community. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
Bak, T. H. (2016). Cooking pasta in La Paz: Bilingualism, bias, and the replication crisis. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 699717. doi: 10.1075/lab.16002.bakGoogle Scholar
Bak, T. H., Long, M. R., Vega-Mendoza, M., & Sorace, A. (2016). Novelty, challenge, and practice: The impact of intensive language learning on attentional functions. PLoS ONE, 11(4), e0153485. doi: 10.1371.journal.pone.0153485Google Scholar
Barkaoui, K., Swain, M., & Lapkin, S. (2011). Examining the quality of measures of change in cognition and affect for older adults: Two case studies. Journal of Aging Studies, 25(1), 6272.Google Scholar
Bernal Castañeda, S. (2016). Methodologies for teaching English to adult students in Spanish vocational education programs. Journal of Professional, Continuing and Online Education, 2(1), 123. doi: 10.18741/P9KW2PGoogle Scholar
Bernal Castañeda, S. (2017). Lifelong learning and limiting factors in second language acquisition for adult students in post-obligatory education. Cogent Psychology, 4(1), 115. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2017.1404699Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2016). Aging and bilingualism: Why does it matter? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(1/2), 18.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Abutalebi, J., Bak, T. H., Burke, D. M., & Kroll, J. F. (2016). Aging in two languages: Implications for public health. Ageing Research Reviews, 27, 5660.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290303.Google Scholar
Blumenfeld, H., Quinzon, S. J. R., Alsol, C., & Riera, S. A. (2017). Predictors of successful learning in multilingual older adults acquiring a majority language. Frontiers in Communication, 2, 119. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2017.00023Google Scholar
Cox, J. G. (2013). Older adult learners and SLA: Age in a new light. In Sanz, C. & Lado, B. (Eds.), Individual differences, L2 development & language program administration: From theory to application (pp. 90107). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Cox, J. G. (2017). Explicit instruction, bilingualism, and the older adult learner. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39(1), 2958.Google Scholar
Cox, J. G., & Sanz, C. (2015). Deconstructing PI for the ages: Explicit instruction v. practice in young and older bilinguals. Language Teaching Research, 53(2), 225248.Google Scholar
Cox, S. R., Bak, T. H., Allerhand, M. M., Redmond, P., Starr, J. M., Deary, I. J., & MacPherson, S. E. (2016). Bilingualism, social cognition and executive function: A tale of chickens and eggs. Neuropsychologia, 91, 299306.Google Scholar
de Bot, K., & Makoni, S. (2005). Language and aging in multilingual contexts. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
de Bot, K., & Schrauf, R. W. (2009). Language development over the lifespan. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
de Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S. (2015). Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26(1), 99107.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2013). Age effects in second language learning: stepping stones toward better understanding. Language Learning, 63(s1), 5267.Google Scholar
Derenowski, M. (2018). Identifying the characteristics of foreign language teachers who work with senior learners. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 145160). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Dunn, A. L., & Fox Tree, J. E. (2009). A quick, gradient Bilingual Dominance Scale. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(3), 273289.Google Scholar
Gabryś-Barker, D. (2018). Concluding remarks and a way forward. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 201208). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Gallardo del Puerto, F., & Martínez Adrián, M. (2014). The incidence of previous foreign language contact in a lexical availability task: A study of senior learners. In Jiménez Catalán, R. M. (Ed.), Lexical availability in English and Spanish as a second language (pp. 5368). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer Science+Business Media.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V., Thomas, E. M., Guasch, N. V., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., … Jones, L. (2016). Teasing apart factors influencing executive function performance in bilinguals and monolinguals at different ages. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 605647.Google Scholar
Goral, M., Campanelli, L., & Spiro, A. I. (2015). Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 7989.Google Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 6781.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515530.Google Scholar
Grotek, M. (2018). Student needs and expectations concerning foreign language teachers in universities of the third age. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 127144). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Hartshorne, J. K., & Germine, L. T. (2015). When does cognitive functioning peak?: The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychological Science, 26(4), 433443.Google Scholar
Holliday, A., & Cooke, T. (1982). An ecological approach to ESP. Lancaster Practical Papers in English Language Education, 5, 123143.Google Scholar
Hubenthal, W. (2004). Older Russian immigrants’ experiences in learning English: Motivation, methods, and barriers. Adult Basic Education, 14(2), 104126.Google Scholar
Ingvalson, E. M., Nowicki, C., Zong, A., & Wong, P. C. M. (2017). Non-native speech learning in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 110. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00148Google Scholar
Keijzer, M. (2011). Language reversion versus general cognitive decline: Towards a new taxonomy of language change in elderly bilingual immigrants. In Schmid, M. S. & Lowie, W. (Eds.), Modeling bilingualism: From structure to chaos (pp. 221232). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. (2009). Language learning and study abroad: A critical reading of research. Basingstoke: Palgrave/Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kliesch, M., Giroud, N., Pfenninger, S. E., & Meyer, M. (2018). Research on second language acquisition in old adulthood: What we have and what we need. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 4875). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Kupisch, T., & van de Weijer, J. (2016). The role of the childhood environment for language dominance: A study of adult simultaneous bilingual speakers of German and French. In Silva-Corvalán, C. & Treffers-Daller, J. (Eds.), Language dominance in bilinguals: Issues of measurement and operationalization (pp. 174194). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lapkin, S., Swain, M., & Psyllakis, P. (2010). The role of languaging in creating zones of proximal development (ZPDs): A long-term care resident interacts with a researcher. Canadian Journal on Aging, 29(4), 477490.Google Scholar
Lenet, A. E., Sanz, C., Lado, B., Howard, J. H. J., & Howard, D. V. (2011). Aging, pedagogical conditions, and differential success in SLA: An empirical study. In Sanz, C. & Leow, R. P. (Eds.), Implicit and explicit language learning: Conditions, processes, and knowledge in SLA and bilingualism (pp. 7384). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Levy, B., & Langer, E. (1994). Aging free from negative stereotypes: successful memory in China and among the American deaf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(6), 989997.Google Scholar
Lipińska, D. (2017). The influence of age and L2 on third language acquisition in a corporate environment. Theory and Practice of Second Language Acquisition, 3(1), 727.Google Scholar
Liu, Y., & Wen, S. (2010). Utilizing crosstalk to help older adult language learners attain comprehensible input. Journal of Nan Kai, 7(2), 3744.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. (2015). Second language acquisition and task-based language teaching. Chichester: Wiley Blackwell.Google Scholar
Mackey, A., & Gass, S. M. (2015). Second language research: Methodology and design (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mackey, A., & Sachs, R. (2012). Older learners in SLA research: A first look at working memory, feedback, and L2 development. Language Learning, 62(3), 704740.Google Scholar
Majhanovich, S., & Deyrich, M.-C. (2017). Language learning to support active social inclusion: Issues and challenges for lifelong learning. International Review of Education, 63(4), 435452.Google Scholar
Marcotte, K., & Ansaldo, A. I. (2014). Age-related behavioural and neurofunctional patterns of second language word learning: Different ways of being successful. Brain & Language, 135, 919.Google Scholar
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940967.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 814.Google Scholar
Mohn, T. (2012, May 10). Learning a new language on location. The New York Times.Google Scholar
Mora, J. F., & Abad, M. P. (2016). Perceived benefits, motivations and preferences for foreign language learning by older adults: Insights from an initiative in Cuenca. Ecuador. Maskana, 7(1), 2338.Google Scholar
Müller, N. (2017). ‘Fear nó bean, a man or a woman?’: Bilingual encounters in residential eldercare in Ireland. In Plejert, C., Lindholm, C., & Schrauf, R. W. (Eds.), Multilingual interaction and dementia (pp. 5273). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2011). Is input more significant than starting age in foreign language acquisition? International Review of Applied Linguistics (IRAL), 49(2), 113133.Google Scholar
Murray, G. (2011). Older language learners, social learning spaces and community. In Benson, P. & Reinders, H. (Eds.), Beyond the language classroom (pp. 132145). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ooi, S. H., Goh, W. D., Sorace, A., & Bak, T. H. (2018). From bilingualism to bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(4), 867879.Google Scholar
Ortega, L. (2013). SLA for the 21st century: Disciplinary progress, transdisciplinary relevance, and the bi/multilingual turn. Language Learning, 63(s1), 124.Google Scholar
Park, D. C. (2000). The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive function. In Park, D. C. & Schwarz, N. (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 321). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. (2006). Bilingual minds: Emotional experience, expression, and representation. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pawlak, M., Derenowski, M., & Mystkowska-Wiertelak, A. (2018). The use of indirect language learning strategies by third-age learners: Insight from a questionnaire study. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 7690). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pfenninger, S. E., & Polz, S. (2018). Foreign language learning in the third age: A pilot feasibility study on cognitive, socio-affective and linguistic drivers and benefits in relation to previous bilingualism of the learner. Journal of the European Second Language Association, 2(1), 113.Google Scholar
Pfenninger, S. E., & Singleton, D. (2017). Beyond age effects in instructional L2 learning: Revisiting the age factor. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Piechurska-Kuciel, E., & Szyszka, M. (2018). Compensatory strategies in senior foreign language students. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (Ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 108124). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pot, A., Keijzer, M., & de Bot, K. (2018a). Do low L2 abilities impede healthy aging for migrant older adults in the Netherlands? Dutch Journal of Applied Linguistics, 7(1), 109120.Google Scholar
Pot, A., Keijzer, M., & de Bot, K. (2018b). Intensity of multilingual language use predicts cognitive performance in some multilingual older adults. Brain Sciences, 8(5), 92.Google Scholar
Pot, A., Keijzer, M., & de Bot, K. (2018c). The language barrier in migrant aging. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 119. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2018.1435627Google Scholar
Ramírez Gómez, D. (2016). Language teaching and the older adult: The significance of experience. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Hendrix, P., Shaoul, C., Milin, P., & Baayen, H. (2014). The myth of cognitive decline: Non-linear dynamics of lifelong learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6(1), 542.Google Scholar
Reinders, H., & Benson, P. (2017). Research agenda: Language learning beyond the classroom. Language Teaching, 50(4), 561578. doi: 10.1017/S0261444817000192Google Scholar
Ristin-Kauffman, N., & Gullberg, M. (2014). The effects of first exposure to an unknown language at different ages. Bulletin Suisse De Linguistique Appliquée, 99, 1729.Google Scholar
Salvatierra, J. L., & Rosselli, M. (2011). The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(1), 2637. doi: 10.1177/1367006910371021Google Scholar
Schmid, M. S., & Keijzer, M. (2009). First language attrition and reversion among older migrants. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 2009(200), 83101. doi: 10.1515/IJSL.2009.046Google Scholar
Schwab, J. F., Stillman, C. M., Howard, J. H. Jr., Schuler, K. D., Newport, E. L., & Howard, D. V. (2016). Aging and statistical learning of grammatical form classes. Psychology and Aging, 31(5), 481487.Google Scholar
Sevinç, Y., & Backus, A. (2017). Anxiety, language use and linguistic competence in an immigrant context: A vicious circle? International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. doi: 10.1080/13670050.2017.1306021Google Scholar
Singleton, D., & Pfenninger, S. E. (2019). Exploring a poorly understood variable: An agenda for classroom research on the age factor. Language Teaching, 52(1), 111127.Google Scholar
Słowik, A. (2016). Different perspectives on the use of L1 in teaching English to the elderly. Journal of Education Culture and Society, 2, 199216. doi: 10.15503/jecs20162.199.216Google Scholar
Słowik, A. (2017). Listening in older second language learners: The teachers’ perspective. Journal of Education Culture and Society, 1, 148163.Google Scholar
Stafford, C. A., Bowden, H. W., & Sanz, C. (2012). Optimizing language instruction: Matters of explicitness, practice, and cue learning. Language Learning, 62(3), 741768.Google Scholar
Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2017). Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(2), 401415. doi: 10.1017/S1366728917000682Google Scholar
Swain, M. (2013). Cognitive and affective enhancement among older adults: The role of languaging. Australian Review of Applied Linguistics, 36(1), 419.Google Scholar
Swain, M., Lapkin, S., & Deters, P. (2013). Exploring the effect of languaging activities on cognitive functioning: The case of an older adult in a long-term care facility. Activities, Adaptation & Aging, 37(1), 118. doi: 10.1080/01924788.2013.760142Google Scholar
Tagarelli, K. M., Ruiz, S., Moreno Vega, J. L., & Rebuschat, P. (2016). Variability in second language learning: The roles of individual differences, learning conditions, and linguistic complexity. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(2), 293316.Google Scholar
Tarone, E., Bigelow, M., & Hansen, K. (2009). Literacy and second language oracy. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Treffers-Daller, J. (2016). Language dominance: The construct, its measurement, and operationalization. In Silva-Corvalán, C. & Treffers-Daller, J. (Eds.), Language dominance in bilinguals: Issues of measurement and operationalization (pp. 235265). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. (2015). World population prospects: The 2015 revision, key findings, and advance tables (Working Paper No. ESA/P/WP.241). New York, USA.Google Scholar
Willis, J. (1996). A framework for task-based learning. Oxford: Pearson.Google Scholar
Wingfield, A., & Grossman, M. (2006). Language and the aging brain: Patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(6), 28302839.Google Scholar
Young-Scholten, M. (2013). Low-educated immigrants and the social relevance of second language acquisition research. Second Language Research, 29(4), 441454.Google Scholar