Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T09:49:50.831Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Beyond the conservative hypothesis: a meta-analysis of lexical-semantic processing in Williams syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 May 2023

Carlos Romero-Rivas*
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Sara Rodríguez-Cuadrado
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Lucía Sabater
Affiliation:
Universidad Internacional de La Rioja, Logroño, Spain
Pablo Rodríguez Gómez
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, Madrid, Spain
Irene Hidalgo de la Guía
Affiliation:
Department of Spanish and Literature Theory, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Eva M. Moreno
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia, Madrid, Spain Faculty of Languages and Education, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid, Spain
Elena Garayzábal Heinze
Affiliation:
Department of General Linguistics, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Carlos Romero-Rivas; Email: carlos.romeror@uam.es

Abstract

Williams syndrome (WS) is a rare genetic disorder, characterised at the cognitive level by a phenotypic pattern of relative weaknesses (e.g., visuospatial skills) and strengths (e.g., some linguistic and nonverbal reasoning skills). In this study, we performed a systematic search and meta-analysis on lexical-semantic processing in WS, an area of knowledge in which contradictory results have been obtained. We found 42 studies matching our criteria, and, in total, 78 effect sizes were included in the meta-analysis. Results showed that individuals with WS have worse lexical-semantic skills than individuals with typical development, whether matched by chronological or mental age. However, people with WS have better lexical-semantic skills than people diagnosed with other cognitive disabilities. Finally, vocabulary skills seem to be relatively spared in WS, although they present some difficulties in semantic processing/integration, semantic memory organisation and verbal working memory skills. Taken together, these results support a neuroconstructivist approach, according to which the cognitive mechanisms involved in lexical-semantic processing may be modulated, even when performance in some tasks (i.e., vocabulary tasks) might be optimal.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This article was originally published with errors in the affiliations of two authors. The article has been updated and an erratum published at https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2023.33.

References

Annaz, D., Van Herwegen, J., Thomas, M., Fishman, R., Karmiloff‐Smith, A., & Rundblad, G. (2009). Comprehension of metaphor and metonymy in children with Williams syndrome. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(6), 962978. https://doi.org/10.3109/13682820802525005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Does the study of autism justify minimalist innate modularity? Learning and Individual Differences, 10(3), 179191. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(99)80129-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bello, A., Capirci, O., & Volterra, V. (2004). Lexical production in children with Williams syndrome: Spontaneous use of gesture in a naming task. Neuropsychologia, 42(2), 201213. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00172-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, T., Trauner, D., & Doherty, S. (1990). Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 37(S6), 115125. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.1320370621 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., & St. George, M. (2000). I. The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl 1), 729. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900561959 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellugi, U., Marks, S., Bihrle, A., & Sabo, H. (1988). Dissociation between language and cognitive functions in Williams syndrome. In Bishop, D. & Mogford, K. (Eds.), Language development in exceptional circumstances (pp. 177189). Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. (1994). Grammatical errors in specific language impairment: Competence or performance limitations? Applied Psycholinguistics, 15(4), 507550. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400006895 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Wiley.10.1002/9780470743386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brock, J. (2007). Language abilities in Williams syndrome: A critical review. Development and Psychopathology, 19(1), 97127. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940707006X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brock, J., Jarrold, C., Farran, E. K., Laws, G., & Riby, D. M. (2007). Do children with Williams syndrome really have good vocabulary knowledge? Methods for comparing cognitive and linguistic abilities in developmental disorders. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21(9), 673688. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200701541433 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clahsen, H., & Almazan, M. (1998). Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition, 68(3), 167198. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00049-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clahsen, H., & Temple, C. M. (2003). Words and rules in children with William’s syndrome. In Levy, Y. & Schaeffer, J. (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Toward a definition of specific language impairment (pp. 323352). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Del Re, A. C. (2013). compute.es: Compute effect sizes (R Package Version 0.2-2) [Computer software]. http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/compute.es Google Scholar
Del Re, A. C., & Hoyt, W. T. (2014). MAd: Meta-analysis with mean differences (R Package Version 0.8-2) [Computer software]. https://cran.r-project.org/package=MAd Google Scholar
Dolscheid, S., & Penke, M. (2018). Quantifier comprehension is linked to linguistic rather than to numerical skills. Evidence from children with Down Syndrome and Williams syndrome. PLoS One, 13(6), e0199743. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199743 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315(7109), 629634. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (2001). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fishman, I., Yam, A., Bellugi, U., Lincoln, A., & Mills, D. (2011). Contrasting patterns of language-associated brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(5), 630638. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq075 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/4737.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garayzábal Heinze, E., & Cuetos Vega, F. (2008). Aprendizaje de la lectura en los niños con síndrome de Williams. Psicothema, 20(4), 672–677.Google Scholar
Garayzábal Heinze, E., & Cuetos Vega, F. (2010). Procesamiento léxico-semántico en el síndrome de Williams. Psicothema, 22(4), 732738.Google ScholarPubMed
Garayzábal Heinze, E., Osório, A., Lens, M., & Sampaio, A. (2014). Concrete and relational vocabulary: Comparison between Williams and Smith–Magenis syndromes. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(12), 33653371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.055 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gosch, A., Städing, G., & Pankau, R. (1994). Linguistic abilities in children with Williams–Beuren syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 52(3), 291296. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.1320520308 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greer, J., Hamiliton, C., Riby, D. M., & Riby, L. M. (2014). Deeper processing is beneficial during episodic memory encoding for adults with Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(7), 17201726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.03.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grice, S. J., Spratling, M. W., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Halit, H., Csibra, G., de Haan, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2001). Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. Neuroreport, 12(12), 26972700.10.1097/00001756-200108280-00021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327(7414), 557560. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, C. F. (2013). Contextual integration of causal coherence in people with Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(10), 33323342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, C. F. (2014). Modality effect on contextual integration in people with Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(7), 15711578. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.03.049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, C. F. (2017). Semantic priming and associative priming in people with Williams syndrome. Advances in Health and Disease, 2, 171196.Google Scholar
Hsu, C. F. (2020). Neural correlates of causal inferences and semantic priming in people with Williams syndrome: An fMRI study. Journal of Intellectual Disability – Diagnosis and Treatment, 8(4), 698709. https://doi.org/10.6000/2292-2598.2020.08.04.13 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, C. F., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Tzeng, O., Chin, R. T., & Wang, H. C. (2007, July). Semantic knowledge in Williams syndrome: Insights from comparing behavioural and brain processes in false memory tasks. In 2007 IEEE 6th international conference on development and learning (pp. 4852). IEEE. https://doi.org/10.1109/DEVLRN.2007.4354074 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, C. F., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2011). Contextual effect in people with Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(2), 781787. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.11.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarrold, C., Hartley, S. J., Phillips, C., & Baddeley, A. D. (2000). Word fluency in Williams syndrome: Evidence for unusual semantic organisation? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 5(4), 293319. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800050199739 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
JASP Team (2020). JASP (Version 0.14.1) [Computer software].Google Scholar
Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Specific language impairment: A deficit in grammar or processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(7), 240247. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01186-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S. C., & Carey, S. (1998). Knowledge enrichment and conceptual change in folkbiology: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognitive Psychology, 37(2), 156200. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0695 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly, J., & Lincoln, A. (2000). II. Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl 1), 3046. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900561968 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Pathyways to language. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Crucial differences between developmental cognitive neuroscience and adult neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 513524. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649709540693 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389398. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01230-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, B. P., & Mervis, C. B. (1999). Contrasting patterns of cognitive abilities of 9- and 10-year-olds with Williams syndrome or Down Syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16(2), 177196. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN1602_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein-Tasman, B. P., & Mervis, C. B. (2018). Distinctive personality characteristics of 8-, 9-, and 10-year-olds with Williams syndrome. In Williams syndrome (pp. 269290). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozel, B. A., Barak, B., Kim, C., Mervis, C. B., Osborne, L. R., Porter, M., & Pober, B. R. (2021). Williams syndrome. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 7(1), 122. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41572-021-00276-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacroix, A., Aguert, M., Dardier, V., Stojanovik, V., & Laval, V. (2010). Idiom comprehension in French-speaking children and adolescents with Williams’ syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(2), 608616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2009.12.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laing, E., Grant, J., Thomas, M., Parmigiani, C., Ewing, S., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). Love is… an abstract word: The influence of lexical semantics on verbal short-term memory in Williams syndrome. Cortex, 41(2), 169179. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70891-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laing, E., & Jarrold, C. (2007). Comprehension of spatial language in Williams syndrome: Evidence for impaired spatial representation of verbal descriptions. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21(9), 689704. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200701541441 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C. S., & Binder, K. S. (2014). An investigation into semantic and phonological processing in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, 227235. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0175) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, Y., & Bechar, T. (2003). Cognitive, lexical and morpho-syntactic profiles of Israeli children with Williams syndrome. Cortex, 39(2), 255271. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70108-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, Y., & Hermon, S. (2003). Morphological abilities of Hebrew-speaking adolescents with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 5983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, L., & Chu, H. (2018). Quantifying publication bias in meta‐analysis. Biometrics, 74(3), 785794. https://doi.org/10.1111/biom.12817 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukács, A., Pléh, C., & Racsmány, M. (2004). Language in Hungarian children with Williams syndrome. In Bartke, S. & Siegmüller, J. (Eds.), Williams syndrome across languages (pp. 187220). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M., Thomas, M. S., & Westermann, G. (2007). Neuroconstructivism-I: How the brain constructs cognition. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martens, M. A., Wilson, S. J., & Reutens, D. C. (2008). Research review: Williams syndrome: A critical review of the cognitive, behavioral, and neuroanatomical phenotype. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(6), 576608. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01887.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menghini, D., Verucci, L., & Vicari, S. (2004). Reading and phonological awareness in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychology, 18(1), 2937. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.29 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. B., & Greiner de Magalhães, C. (2022). Williams syndrome. In Beauchamp, M., Peterson, R. L., Ris, M. D., Taylor, H. G., & Yeates, K. O. (Eds.), Pediatric neuropsychology: Research, theory, and practice. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & John, A. E. (2008). Vocabulary abilities of children with Williams syndrome: Strengths, weaknesses, and relation to visuospatial construction ability. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 967982. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/071) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. B., & John, A. E. (2010). Cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with Williams syndrome: Implications for intervention approaches. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C: Seminars in Medical Genetics, 154(2), 229248. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.c.30263 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Morris, C. A., Bertrand, J., & Robinson, B. F. (1999). Williams syndrome: Findings from an integrated program of research. In Tager-Flusberg, H. (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 65110). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Robinson, B. F., Bertrand, J., Morris, C. A., Klein-Tasman, B. P., & Armstrong, S. C. (2000). The Williams syndrome cognitive profile. Brain and Cognition, 44(3), 604628. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2000.1232 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miezah, D., Porter, M., Rossi, A., Kazzi, C., Batchelor, J., & Reeve, J. (2021). Cognitive profile of young children with Williams syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 65(8), 784794. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12860 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, C. A., Braddock, S. R., COUNCIL ON GENETICS, Chen, E., Trotter, T. L., Berry, S. A., Burke, L. W., Geleske, T. A., Hamid, R., Hopkin, R. J., Introne, W. J., Lyons, M. J., Scheuerle, A. E., & Stoler, J. M. (2020). Health care supervision for children with Williams syndrome. Pediatrics, 145(2). https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2019-3761 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naylor, L., & Van Herwegen, J. (2012). The production of figurative language in typically developing children and Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(2), 711716. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.11.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T., Gopnik, A., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). Asynchrony in the cognitive and lexical development of young children with Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Language, 32(2), 427438. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000904006737 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzini, G., Vicari, S., Volterra, V., Milani, L., & Ossella, M. T. (1999). Children with Williams syndrome: Is there a single neuropsychological profile? Developmental Neuropsychology, 15(1), 141155. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649909540742 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pezzino, A. S., Marec-Breton, N., Gonthier, C., & Lacroix, A. (2021). Factors explaining deficits in reading acquisition: The case of Williams syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 64(10), 38943908. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_JSLHR-19-00404 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinheiro, A. P., Galdo-Álvarez, S., Sampaio, A., Niznikiewicz, M., & Gonçalves, Ó. F. (2010). Electrophysiological correlates of semantic processing in Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(6), 14121425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: The ingredients of language. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Porter, M. A., & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive heterogeneity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 275306. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2702_5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, M. A., Dobson-Stone, C., Kwok, J. B., Schofield, P. R., Beckett, W., & Tassabehji, M. (2012). A role for transcription factor GTF2IRD2 in executive function in Williams–Beuren syndrome. PLoS One, 7(10), e47457. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0047457 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purser, H. R., Thomas, M. S., Snoxall, S., Mareschal, D., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2011). Definitions versus categorization: Assessing the development of lexico-semantic knowledge in Williams syndrome. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 46(3), 113. https://doi.org/10.3109/13682822.2010.497531 Google ScholarPubMed
Race, D. S., & Hillis, A. B. (2015). Naming. In Tonga, A. W. (Ed.), Brain mapping (pp. 455459). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-397025-1.00267-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, S. J., & Temple, C. M. (2009). The representation of semantic knowledge in a child with Williams syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26(3), 307337. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290903126320 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rombouts, E., Maes, B., & Zink, I. (2020). An investigation into the relationship between Quality of pantomime gestures and visuospatial skills. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 36(3), 179189. https://doi.org/10.1080/07434618.2020.1811760 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossen, M., Klima, E. S., Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., & Jones, W. (1996). Interaction between language and cognition: Evidence from Williams syndrome. In Beitchman, J. H., Cohen, N., Konstantareas, M., & Tannock, R. (Eds.), Language, learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives (pp. 367392). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
RStudio Team (2015). RStudio: Integrated development for R [Computer software]. https://www.rstudio.com/ Google Scholar
Scott, P., Mervis, C. B., Bertrand, J., Klein, B. P., Armstrong, S. C., & Ford, A. L. (1995). Semantic organization and word fluency in 9- and 10-year-old children with Williams syndrome. Genetic Counseling, Special Issue, 6, 172173.Google Scholar
Serrano-Juárez, C. A., Venegas-Vega, C. A., Yáñez-Téllez, M. G., Rodríguez-Camacho, M., Silva-Pereyra, J., Salgado-Ceballos, H., & Prieto-Corona, B. (2018). Cognitive, behavioral, and adaptive profiles in Williams syndrome with and without loss of GTF2IRD2. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 24(9), 896904. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617718000711 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, W. (2007). Down Syndrome: Cognitive phenotype. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13(3), 228236. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20156 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stojanovik, V. (2006). Social interaction deficits and conversational inadequacy in Williams syndrome. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 19(2), 157173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2005.11.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stojanovik, V., & van Ewijk, L. (2008). Do children with Williams syndrome have unusual vocabularies? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(1), 1834. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.06.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strømme, P., Bjømstad, P. G., & Ramstad, K. (2002). Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 17(4), 269271. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F088307380201700406 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H., Plesa-Skwerer, D., Faja, S., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). People with Williams syndrome process faces holistically. Cognition, 89(1), 1124. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00049-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, M. S., Dockrell, J. E., Messer, D., Parmigiani, C., Ansari, D., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). Speeded naming, frequency and the development of the lexicon in Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(6), 721759. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960500258528 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C. (2010). Language acquisition in developmental disorders. In Language acquisition across linguistic and cognitive systems (pp. 6790). John Benjamins Publishing Company.10.1075/lald.52.06thoCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Modeling language acquisition in atypical phenotypes. Psychological Review, 110, 647682. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.647 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, M. S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). Can developmental disorders reveal the component parts of the language faculty? Language Learning and Development, 1, 6592. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15473341lld0101_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., Purser, H. R. M., & Richardson, F. M. (2013). Modularity and developmental disorders. In: The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology, Vol. 2: Self and other (pp. 481506). Oxford Academic. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958474.013.0019 Google Scholar
Tyler, L. K., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Voice, J. K., Stevens, T., Grant, J., Udwin, O., Davies, M., & Howlin, P. (1997). Do individuals with Williams syndrome have bizarre semantics? Evidence for lexical organization using an on-line task. Cortex, 33(3), 515527. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70233-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Udwin, O., & Yule, W. (1990). Expressive language of children with Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 37(S6), 108114. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.1320370620 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Udwin, O., & Yule, W. (1991). A cognitive and behavioural phenotype in Williams syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13(2), 232244. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639108401040 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Herwegen, J., Dimitriou, D., & Rundblad, G. (2013). Development of novel metaphor and metonymy comprehension in typically developing children and Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(4), 13001311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Bates, E., Caselli, M. C., Pasqualetti, P., Gagliardi, C., Tonucci, F., & Volterra, V. (2004). Neuropsychological profile of Italians with Williams syndrome: An example of a dissociation between language and cognition? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(6), 862876. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704106073 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Carlesimo, G., Brizzolara, D., & Pezzini, G. (1996). Short-term memory in children with Williams syndrome: A reduced contribution of lexical-semantic knowledge to word span. Neuropsychologia, 34(9), 919925. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(96)00007-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Caselli, M. C., Gagliardi, C., Tonucci, F., & Volterra, V. (2002). Language acquisition in special populations: A comparison between Down and Williams syndromes. Neuropsychologia, 40(13), 24612470. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00083-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Pezzini, G., Sabbadini, L., & Vicari, S. (1996). Linguistic abilities in Italian children with Williams syndrome. Cortex, 32(4), 663677. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(96)80037-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O., Tonucci, F., & Vicari, S. (2003). Early linguistic abilities of Italian children with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23(1–2), 3358. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2003.9651886 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M. W., & Thomas, M. S. C. (2007). Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science, 10, 7583. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00567.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westermann, G., Thomas, M. S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2011). Neuroconstructivism. In Goswami, U. (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 723747). Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ypsilanti, A., Grouios, G., Zikouli, A., & Hatzinikolaou, K. (2006). Speed of naming in children with Williams and Down Syndromes. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 31(2), 8794. https://doi.org/10.1080/13668250600710872 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed