Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-25wd4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T05:32:41.867Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Collective Guilt and Attitudes Toward Recycling: Data From a North Queensland Sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 July 2012

Pamela M. Pensini*
Affiliation:
James Cook University, Australia
Nerina J. Caltabiano
Affiliation:
James Cook University, Australia
*
address for correspondence: Pamela Pensini, School of Arts and Social Sciences, James Cook University, Townsville, Australia4811. E-mail: Pamela.Pensini@jcu.edu.au

Abstract

There is overwhelming evidence that environmental concerns currently facing the modern world are, at least partly, due to the unsustainable use of resources in developed countries. In this study, feelings of collective guilt in members of a developed nation, Australia, were investigated as a factor motivating individuals to recycle. Eighty-five participants from North Queensland (24 male, 61 female; aged 17 to 64, M = 24.16, SD = 8.42) completed self-report measures of collective guilt, social desirability, social norms, attitudes toward recycling and percentage of household rubbish recycled. As predicted, those accepting more collective guilt for the actions of their ingroup reported more favourable attitudes toward recycling. However, collective guilt failed to predict actual recycling behaviour. The same was found for social norms in that they predicted attitudes to recycling, but not the amount of rubbish recycled. Attitudes toward recycling were the only successful predictor of the amount of rubbish recycled. This study has demonstrated the role of the emotion of collective guilt and social norms in one environmentally relevant domain. Avenues for future research regarding the implications of collective guilt on ecological behaviour have been discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behaviour. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Bartlett, M.S. (1954). A note on the multiplying factors for various chi square approximations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 16 (B), 296298.Google Scholar
Bizman, A., Yinon, Y., & Krotman, S. (2001). Group-based emotional distress: An extension of self-discrepancy theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 12911300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Branscombe, N.R., Doosje, B., & McGarty, C. (2003). Antecedents and consequences of collective guilt. In Mackie, D.M. & Smith, E.R. (Eds.), From prejudice to intergroup emotions: Differentiated reactions to social groups (pp. 4966). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Branscombe, N.R., Slugoski, B., & Kappen, D.M. (2004). The measurement of collective guilt: What it is and what it is not. In Branscombe, N.R. & Doosje, B. (Eds.), Collective guilt: International perspectives (pp. 1634). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, T. (2002). The effect of Indigenous awareness and level of identification with White Australia on feelings of collective guilt (Unpublished honours thesis). James Cook University, Australia.Google Scholar
Caltabiano, N.J., & Caltabiano, M.L. (1994). Demographic variables, cognitive and personality factors as predictors of environmentally responsible behaviour. In Vialle, W. (Ed.), Selected papers from the 29th annual Australian Psychological Society conference (pp. 209216). Wollongong, NSW: The Australian Psychological Society Ltd.Google Scholar
Chan, K. (1998). Mass communication and pro-environmental behaviour: Waste recycling in Hong Kong. Journal of Environmental Management, 52, 317325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowne, D.P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24 (4), 349354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, D. J., & Freudenburg, W.R. (1996). Gender and environmental risk concerns: A review and analysis of available research. Environment and Behaviour, 28 (3), 302339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doosje, B., Branscombe, N.R., Spears, R., & Manstead, A.R.S. (1998). Guilty by association: When one's group has a negative history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (4), 872886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillham, P.F. (2008). Participation in the environmental movement: Analysis of the European Union. International Sociology, 23 (1), 6793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halloran, M.J. (2007). Indigenous reconciliation in Australia: Do values, identity and collective guilt matter? Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 17, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, J.R., & Nielsen, J.M. (1991). Recycling as altruistic behaviour: Normative and behavioural strategies to expand participation in a community recycling program. Environment and Behaviour, 23, 195220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, H. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika, 39, 3136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kantola, S.J., Syme, G.J., & Campbell, N.A. (1982). The role of individual differences and external variables in a test of the sufficiency of Fishbein's model to explain behavioural intentions to conserve water. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 12 (1), 7083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotchen, M.J., & Reiling, S.D. (2000). Environmental attitudes, motivations, and contingent valuation of nonuse values: A case study involving endangered species. Ecological Economics, 32, 93107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mainieri, T., Barnett, E.G., Valdero, T.R., Unipan, J.J., & Oskamp, S. (1997). Green buying: The influence of environmental concern on consumer behaviour. The Journal of Social Psychology, 137 (2), 189204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McFarlane, B.L., & Boxall, P.C. (2003). The role of social psychological and social structural variables in environmental activism: An example of the forest sector. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 7987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGarty, C., Pederson, A., Leach, C.W., Mansell, T., Waller, J., & Bliuc, A.-M. (2005). Group-based guilt as a predictor of commitment to apology. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 659680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohai, P. (1992). Men, women, and the environment: An examination of the gender gap in environmental concern and activism. Society and Natural Resources, 5 (1), 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oskamp, S., Harrington, M.J., Edwards, T.C., Sherwood, D.L., Okuda, S.M., & Swanson, D.C. (1991). Factors influencing household recycling behaviour. Environment and Behaviour, 23, 494519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pensini, P.M., Slugoski, B.R., & Caltabiano, N.J. (in press). Predictors of environmental behaviour: A comparison of known groups. Management of Environmental Quality.Google Scholar
Roccas, S., Klar, Y., & Liviatan, I. (2006). The paradox of group-based guilt: Modes of national identification, conflict vehemence, and reactions to the in-group's moral violations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91 (4), 698711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, M.R., Branscombe, N.R., & Brehm, J.W. (2004). Gender inequality and the intensity of men's collective guilt. In Branscombe, N.R. & Doosje, B. (Eds.), Collective guilt: International perspectives (pp. 7594). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, S.H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 221279). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Slugoski, B., Branscombe, N.R., & Kappen, D.M. (2002, April). Development and validation of the Collective Guilt Scale (CGS). Paper presented at the Society for Australasian Social Psychologists, Adelaide, Australia.Google Scholar
Stern, P.C., Dietz, T., & Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientations, gender, and environmental concern. Environment and Behaviour, 25 (3), 322348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swim, J.K., & Miller, D.L. (1999). White guilt: Its antecedents and consequences for attitudes toward affirmative action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25 (4), 500514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H. (Ed.). (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tangney, J.P. (1995). Shame and guilt in interpersonal relationships. In Tangney, J.P. & Fischer, K.W. (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride (pp. 114139). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tindall, D.B., Davies, S., & Mauboules, C. (2003). Activism and conservation behaviour in an environmental movement: The contradictory effects of gender. Society and Natural Resources, 16, 909932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Liere, K.D. & Dunlap, R.E. (1980). The social bases of environmental concern: A review of hypotheses, explanations and empirical evidence. Public Opinion Quarterly, 44, 181197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, D.D.N., & Koger, S.M. (2004). The psychology of environmental problems (2nd ed.). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Zelezny, L.C., Chua, P.P., & Aldrich, C. (2000). Elaborating on gender differences in environmentalism. Journal of Social Issues, 56 (3), 443457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar