Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T13:55:28.905Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Comments on Guimarães & Sawaya. Pretending to be venomous: is a snake's head shape a trustworthy signal to a predator?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 December 2011

Janne K. Valkonen*
Affiliation:
Centre of Excellence in Evolutionary Research, Department of Biological and Environmental Science, P.O. Box 35, 40014University of Jyväskylä, Finland
Johanna Mappes
Affiliation:
Centre of Excellence in Evolutionary Research, Department of Biological and Environmental Science, P.O. Box 35, 40014University of Jyväskylä, Finland
*
1Corresponding author. Email: janne.k.valkonen@jyu.fi

Extract

Several species of non-venomous snake are known to flatten their heads when disturbed, and this behaviour has been suggested to be a mimicry of vipers (Arnold & Ovenden 2002, Hailey & Davies 1986, Young et al. 1999). Using plasticine models, Guimarães & Sawaya (2011) tested the antipredatory function of a triangular head shape in snakes. Their article presents the first published empirical experiment testing the adaptive significance of vipers' triangular head shape. Guimarães & Sawaya (2011) found no support for the viper mimicry hypothesis. Accordingly, they concluded that ‘the shape of [the] head seemed not to confer advantage itself’. Although the use of plasticine models is a generally accepted method of testing predation pressure on snakes, we argue that the experiment may have failed to find the antipredatory function of triangulation due to the pooling of attacks by mammalian and avian predators. Mammals generally rely on olfactory cues during foraging. Plasticine has a strong odour which does not resemble the odour of any prey species. It is thus unlikely that mammals would treat snake replicas as true snakes.

Type
Short Communication
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

LITERATURE CITED

ARNOLD, E. N. & OVENDEN, D. V. 2002. Reptiles and amphibians of Europe. Princeton University Press, Princeton.Google Scholar
GUIMARÃES, M. & SAWAYA, R. J. 2011. Pretending to be venomous: is a snake's head shape a trustworthy signal to a predator? Journal of Tropical Ecology 27:437439.Google Scholar
HAILEY, A. & DAVIES, P. M. C. 1986. Effects of size, sex, temperature and condition on activity metabolism and defence behaviour of the viperine snake, Natrix maura. Journal of Zoology 208:541558.Google Scholar
NISKANEN, M. & MAPPES, J. 2005. Significance of the dorsal zigzag pattern of Vipera latastei gaditana against avian predators. Journal of Animal Ecology 74:10911101.Google Scholar
VALKONEN, J. K., NOKELAINEN, O. & MAPPES, J. 2011a. Antipredatory function of head shape for vipers and their mimics. PloS ONE 6 (7): e22272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VALKONEN, J. K., NISKANEN, M., BJÖRKLUND, M. & MAPPES, J. 2011b. Disruption or aposematism? Significance of dorsal zigzag pattern of European vipers. Evolutionary Ecology 25:1047–1063.Google Scholar
YOUNG, B. A., LAOR, J. & SOLOMON, J. 1999. The comparative biomechanics of an ophidian defensive behaviour: head triangulation in hognose snake (Heterodon) and an egg-eating snake (Dasypeltis). Journal of Zoology 248:169177.Google Scholar